心理学报 ›› 2023, Vol. 55 ›› Issue (6): 861-876.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.00861
• 研究报告 • 下一篇
收稿日期:
2022-06-11
发布日期:
2023-03-06
出版日期:
2023-06-25
通讯作者:
孙宇浩
E-mail:sunyuhao@zstu.edu.cn
基金资助:
WANG Zhe, NI Hao, FENG Dan, YAN Linlin, SUN Yu-Hao P()
Received:
2022-06-11
Online:
2023-03-06
Published:
2023-06-25
摘要:
人们对熟悉面孔的识别绩效显著好于陌生面孔。然而, 对于熟悉度提升面孔识别绩效背后的认知加工与信息整合机制, 我们仍然知之甚少。本研究招募了两个班级的大学生(分别为相互接触1个月和相互接触13个月), 操纵被试第一注视点所落的区域(眼睛或嘴巴), 呈现完整面孔、区域部件(regional part, 眼睛或嘴巴)和区域外周(regional periphery, 完整面孔上, 遮挡眼睛或嘴巴后剩余的外周部分), 用延迟匹配任务测量两组学生对同班同学面孔和陌生人面孔的识别绩效。获得四个发现:(1)接触1个月的被试组表现出较弱的熟悉度效应, 接触13个月的被试组表现出较强的熟悉度效应。(2)熟悉面孔的区域部件和区域外周的识别绩效都高于陌生面孔。(3)在面孔熟悉度增加的过程中, 眼睛区域和嘴巴区域表现出区域异步性(regional asynchronization)。(4)眼睛区域在面孔识别中存在特殊的重要性。综上, 实验提示面孔熟悉是一个具有区域异步性的绩效提升累积过程, 以眼睛区域为中心的面孔整体加工在面孔知觉与记忆表征的形成过程中起到关键作用。
中图分类号:
王哲, 倪昊, 封丹, 严璘璘, 孙宇浩. (2023). 面孔熟悉过程中部件加工与整体加工的区域异步性和眼睛区域特异性. 心理学报, 55(6), 861-876.
WANG Zhe, NI Hao, FENG Dan, YAN Linlin, SUN Yu-Hao P. (2023). Regional asynchrony and eye region-specificity in part-based processing and holistic processing during face familiarization. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 55(6), 861-876.
被试 | 陌生面孔 | 熟悉面孔 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | 注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | |
接触1个月的被试组 | 88.0 (1.4) [85.3, 90.7] | 87.9 (1.5) [85.0, 90.8] | 92.0 (0.9) [90.1, 93.9] | 90.3 (1.1) [88.0, 92.6] |
接触13个月的被试组 | 86.3 (1.4) [83.4, 89.2] | 84.5 (1.5) [81.4, 87.6] | 94.2 (0.10) [92.2, 96.2] | 92.4 (1.2) [90.0, 94.8] |
表1 接触1个月的被试组和接触13个月的被试组在注视眼睛或嘴巴条件下识别完整的陌生面孔和熟悉面孔的正确率(%)
被试 | 陌生面孔 | 熟悉面孔 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | 注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | |
接触1个月的被试组 | 88.0 (1.4) [85.3, 90.7] | 87.9 (1.5) [85.0, 90.8] | 92.0 (0.9) [90.1, 93.9] | 90.3 (1.1) [88.0, 92.6] |
接触13个月的被试组 | 86.3 (1.4) [83.4, 89.2] | 84.5 (1.5) [81.4, 87.6] | 94.2 (0.10) [92.2, 96.2] | 92.4 (1.2) [90.0, 94.8] |
被试 | 陌生面孔 | 熟悉面孔 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | 注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | |
接触1个月的被试组 | 1440 (50) [1339, 1540] | 1388 (45) [1298, 1478] | 1360 (42) [1276, 1445] | 1352 (45) [1261, 1443] |
接触13个月的被试组 | 1234 (53) [1129, 1340] | 1195 (47) [1101, 1290] | 1195 (44) [1106, 1284] | 1156 (48) [1060, 1252] |
表2 接触1个月的被试组和接触13个月的被试组注视眼睛或嘴巴时识别完整陌生和熟悉面孔的正确反应时(ms)
被试 | 陌生面孔 | 熟悉面孔 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | 注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | |
接触1个月的被试组 | 1440 (50) [1339, 1540] | 1388 (45) [1298, 1478] | 1360 (42) [1276, 1445] | 1352 (45) [1261, 1443] |
接触13个月的被试组 | 1234 (53) [1129, 1340] | 1195 (47) [1101, 1290] | 1195 (44) [1106, 1284] | 1156 (48) [1060, 1252] |
陌生面孔 | 熟悉面孔 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | 注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | |
接触1个月的被试组 | 72.4 (1.9) [68.6, 76.2] | 78.5 (1.6) [75.3, 81.8] | 75.7 (2.0) [71.7, 79.6] | 84.7 (2.0) [80.8, 88.6] |
接触13个月的被试组 | 71.1 (2.0) [67.1, 75.1] | 81.4 (1.7) [78.0, 84.8] | 78.3 (2.1) [74.1, 82.5] | 81.8 (2.1) [77.6, 85.9] |
表3 接触1个月的被试组和接触13个月的被试组识别陌生和熟悉面孔的眼睛或嘴巴区域部件的正确率(%)
陌生面孔 | 熟悉面孔 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | 注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | |
接触1个月的被试组 | 72.4 (1.9) [68.6, 76.2] | 78.5 (1.6) [75.3, 81.8] | 75.7 (2.0) [71.7, 79.6] | 84.7 (2.0) [80.8, 88.6] |
接触13个月的被试组 | 71.1 (2.0) [67.1, 75.1] | 81.4 (1.7) [78.0, 84.8] | 78.3 (2.1) [74.1, 82.5] | 81.8 (2.1) [77.6, 85.9] |
被试 | 陌生面孔 | 熟悉面孔 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | 注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | |
接触1个月的被试组 | 2002 (63) [1875, 2129] | 1773 (56) [1621, 1846] | 1938 (62) [1814, 2062] | 1814 (64) [1685, 1943] |
接触13个月的被试组 | 1780 (67) [1646, 1914] | 1577 (59) [1458, 1695] | 1703 (65) [1572, 1834] | 1604 (68) [1468, 1740] |
表4 接触1个月的被试组和接触13个月的被试组识别陌生面孔和熟悉面孔的眼睛区域或嘴巴区域部件的正确反应时(ms)
被试 | 陌生面孔 | 熟悉面孔 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | 注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | |
接触1个月的被试组 | 2002 (63) [1875, 2129] | 1773 (56) [1621, 1846] | 1938 (62) [1814, 2062] | 1814 (64) [1685, 1943] |
接触13个月的被试组 | 1780 (67) [1646, 1914] | 1577 (59) [1458, 1695] | 1703 (65) [1572, 1834] | 1604 (68) [1468, 1740] |
被试 | 陌生面孔 | 熟悉面孔 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | 注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | |
接触1个月的被试组 | 88.4 (1.3) [85.7, 91.1] | 83.9 (1.7) [80.5, 87.3] | 91.7 (1.2) [89.4, 94.1] | 92.0 (1.0) [90.1, 94.0] |
接触13个月的被试组 | 86.4 (1.4) [83.6, 89.3] | 83.3 (1.8) [79.7, 87.0] | 93.5 (1.3) [91.0, 96.0] | 92.4 (1.0) [90.4, 94.5] |
表5 接触1个月的被试组和接触13个月的被试组识别陌生和熟悉面孔的眼睛和嘴巴区域外周的正确率(%)
被试 | 陌生面孔 | 熟悉面孔 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | 注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | |
接触1个月的被试组 | 88.4 (1.3) [85.7, 91.1] | 83.9 (1.7) [80.5, 87.3] | 91.7 (1.2) [89.4, 94.1] | 92.0 (1.0) [90.1, 94.0] |
接触13个月的被试组 | 86.4 (1.4) [83.6, 89.3] | 83.3 (1.8) [79.7, 87.0] | 93.5 (1.3) [91.0, 96.0] | 92.4 (1.0) [90.4, 94.5] |
被试 | 陌生面孔 | 熟悉面孔 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | 注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | |
接触1个月的被试组 | 1393 (41) [1310, 1475] | 1448 (47) [1354, 1543] | 1303 (42) [1219, 1388] | 1335 (45) [1245, 1424] |
接触13个月的被试组 | 1285 (43) [1198, 1372] | 1329 (50) [1230, 1429] | 1149 (44) [1060, 1238] | 1199 (47) [1105, 1294] |
表6 接触1个月的被试组和接触13个月的被试组识别陌生和熟悉面孔的眼睛和嘴巴区域外周的正确反应时(ms)
被试 | 陌生面孔 | 熟悉面孔 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | 注视眼睛 | 注视嘴巴 | |
接触1个月的被试组 | 1393 (41) [1310, 1475] | 1448 (47) [1354, 1543] | 1303 (42) [1219, 1388] | 1335 (45) [1245, 1424] |
接触13个月的被试组 | 1285 (43) [1198, 1372] | 1329 (50) [1230, 1429] | 1149 (44) [1060, 1238] | 1199 (47) [1105, 1294] |
变量 | 完整面孔识别任务 | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
陌生面孔 | 熟悉面孔 | ||||||
注视 眼睛 | 注视 嘴巴 | 注视 眼睛 | 注视 嘴巴 | ||||
区域部件识别任务 | 接触1个月的被试组 | 陌生面孔 | 眼睛 | 0.231 | — | — | — |
嘴巴 | — | 0.200 | — | — | |||
熟悉面孔 | 眼睛 | — | — | 0.334 | — | ||
嘴巴 | — | — | — | 0.304 | |||
接触13个月的被试组 | 陌生面孔 | 眼睛 | 0.354 | — | — | — | |
嘴巴 | — | 0.065 | — | — | |||
熟悉面孔 | 眼睛 | — | — | 0.562** | — | ||
嘴巴 | — | — | — | 0.288 |
表7 完整面孔识别任务与区域部件识别任务的识别正确率的相关
变量 | 完整面孔识别任务 | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
陌生面孔 | 熟悉面孔 | ||||||
注视 眼睛 | 注视 嘴巴 | 注视 眼睛 | 注视 嘴巴 | ||||
区域部件识别任务 | 接触1个月的被试组 | 陌生面孔 | 眼睛 | 0.231 | — | — | — |
嘴巴 | — | 0.200 | — | — | |||
熟悉面孔 | 眼睛 | — | — | 0.334 | — | ||
嘴巴 | — | — | — | 0.304 | |||
接触13个月的被试组 | 陌生面孔 | 眼睛 | 0.354 | — | — | — | |
嘴巴 | — | 0.065 | — | — | |||
熟悉面孔 | 眼睛 | — | — | 0.562** | — | ||
嘴巴 | — | — | — | 0.288 |
变量 | 完整面孔识别任务 | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
陌生面孔 | 熟悉面孔 | ||||||
注视 眼睛 | 注视 嘴巴 | 注视 眼睛 | 注视 嘴巴 | ||||
区域外周识别任务 | 接触1个月的 被试组 | 陌生面孔 | 眼睛 | 0.412* | — | — | — |
嘴巴 | — | 0.376 | — | — | |||
熟悉面孔 | 眼睛 | — | — | 0.428* | — | ||
嘴巴 | — | — | — | 0.139 | |||
接触13个月的 被试组 | 陌生面孔 | 眼睛 | 0.439* | — | — | — | |
嘴巴 | — | 0.272 | — | — | |||
熟悉面孔 | 眼睛 | — | — | 0.609** | — | ||
嘴巴 | — | — | — | 0.175 |
表8 完整面孔识别任务与区域外周识别任务的识别正确率的相关
变量 | 完整面孔识别任务 | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
陌生面孔 | 熟悉面孔 | ||||||
注视 眼睛 | 注视 嘴巴 | 注视 眼睛 | 注视 嘴巴 | ||||
区域外周识别任务 | 接触1个月的 被试组 | 陌生面孔 | 眼睛 | 0.412* | — | — | — |
嘴巴 | — | 0.376 | — | — | |||
熟悉面孔 | 眼睛 | — | — | 0.428* | — | ||
嘴巴 | — | — | — | 0.139 | |||
接触13个月的 被试组 | 陌生面孔 | 眼睛 | 0.439* | — | — | — | |
嘴巴 | — | 0.272 | — | — | |||
熟悉面孔 | 眼睛 | — | — | 0.609** | — | ||
嘴巴 | — | — | — | 0.175 |
[1] |
Alzueta E., Melcón M., Poch C., & Capilla A. (2019). Is your own face more than a highly familiar face? Biological Psychology, 142, 100-107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2019.01.018
doi: S0301-0511(18)30196-0 URL pmid: 30738092 |
[2] |
Andrews S., Burton A. M., Schweinberger S. R., & Wiese H. (2017). Event-related potentials reveal the development of stable face representations from natural variability. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(8), 1620-1632. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1195851
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1195851 URL |
[3] |
Baker K. A., Laurence S., & Mondloch C. J. (2017). How does a newly encountered face become familiar? The effect of within-person variability on adults’ and children’s perception of identity. Cognition, 161, 19-30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.12.012s
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.12.012 URL |
[4] |
Bortolon C., & Raffard S. (2018). Self-face advantage over familiar and unfamiliar faces: A three-level meta-analytic approach. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(4), 1287-1300. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1487-9
doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1487-9 URL |
[5] |
Bruce V. (1986). Influences of familiarity on the processing of faces. Perception, 15(4), 387-397. https://doi.org/10.1068/p150387
URL pmid: 3822723 |
[6] |
Burton A. M., Wilson S., Cowan M., & Bruce V. (1999). Face recognition in poor-quality video: Evidence from security surveillance. Psychological Science, 10(3), 243-248. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00144
doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00144 URL |
[7] |
Caldara R., Schyns P., Mayer E., Smith M. L., Gosselin F., & Rossion B. (2005). Does prosopagnosia take the eyes out of face representations? Evidence for a defect in representing diagnostic facial information following brain damage. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(10), 1652-1666. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892905774597254
URL pmid: 16269103 |
[8] |
Cassia V. M., Picozzi M., Kuefner D., & Casati M. (2009). Short article: Why mix-ups don’t happen in the nursery: Evidence for an experience-based interpretation of the other-age effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(6), 1099-1107. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210802617654
doi: 10.1080/17470210802617654 URL |
[9] |
Dalton P. (1993). The role of stimulus familiarity in context- dependent recognition. Memory & Cognition, 21(2), 223-234. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03202735
doi: 10.3758/BF03202735 URL |
[10] |
DeGutis J., Mercado R. J., Wilmer J., & Rosenblatt A. (2013). Individual differences in holistic processing predict the own-race advantage in recognition memory. PLOS ONE, 8(4), e58253. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0058253
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0058253 URL |
[11] |
Devue C., & Brédart S. (2011). The neural correlates of visual self-recognition. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(1), 40-51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.09.007
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.09.007 URL pmid: 20880722 |
[12] |
Dowsett A. J., Sandford A., & Burton A. M. (2016). Face learning with multiple images leads to fast acquisition of familiarity for specific individuals. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(1), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2015.1017513
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1017513 URL |
[13] |
Ge L., Anzures G., Wang Z., Kelly D. J., Pascalis O., Quinn P. C., … Lee K. (2008). An inner face advantage in children’s recognition of familiar peers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101(2), 124-136. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.05.006
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.05.006 URL |
[14] |
Harrison V., & Hole G. J. (2009). Evidence for a contact- based explanation of the own-age bias in face recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(2), 264-269. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.2.264
doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.2.264 URL |
[15] | Heisz J. J., & Shore D. I. (2008). More efficient scanning for familiar faces. Journal of Vision, 8(1). https://doi.org/10.1167/8.1.9 |
[16] |
Jackson M. C., & Raymond J. E. (2008). Familiarity enhances visual working memory for faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(3), 556-568. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.34.3.556
doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.3.556 URL |
[17] | Jansari A., Miller S., Pearce L., Cobb S., Sagiv N., Williams A. L., … Hanley J. R. (2015). The man who mistook his neuropsychologist for a popstar: When configural processing fails in acquired prosopagnosia. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, Article 390. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00390 |
[18] |
Kaufmann J. M., Schweinberger S. R., & Burton A. M. (2009). N250 ERP correlates of the acquisition of face representations across different images. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(4), 625-641. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21080
doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21080 URL pmid: 18702593 |
[19] |
Kikutani M., Roberson D., & Hanley J. R. (2008). What’s in the name? Categorical perception for unfamiliar faces can occur through labeling. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(4), 787-794. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.15.4.787
doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.4.787 URL |
[20] |
Klatzky R. L., & Forrest F. H. (1984). Recognizing familiar and unfamiliar faces. Memory & Cognition, 12(1), 60-70. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196998
doi: 10.3758/BF03196998 URL |
[21] |
Klin A., Jones W., Schultz R., Volkmar F., & Cohen D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(9), 809-816. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.59.9.809
doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.9.809 URL pmid: 12215080 |
[22] |
Kramer R. S. S., Manesi Z., Towler A., Reynolds M. G., & Burton A. M. (2018). Familiarity and within-person facial variability: The importance of the internal and external features. Perception, 47(1), 3-15. https://doi.org/10.1177/0301006617725242
doi: 10.1177/0301006617725242 URL pmid: 28803526 |
[23] |
Kuefner D., Macchi Cassia V., Picozzi M., & Bricolo E. (2008). Do all kids look alike? Evidence for an other-age effect in adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(4), 811-817. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.34.4.811
doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.4.811 URL |
[24] |
Levin D. T., & Beale J. M. (2000). Categorical perception occurs in newly learned faces, other-race faces, and inverted faces. Perception & Psychophysics, 62(2), 386-401. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03205558
doi: 10.3758/BF03205558 URL |
[25] |
Megreya A. M., & Burton A. M. (2006). Unfamiliar faces are not faces: Evidence from a matching task. Memory & Cognition, 34(4), 865-876. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193433
doi: 10.3758/BF03193433 URL |
[26] |
Mohr S., Wang A., & Engell A. D. (2018). Early identity recognition of familiar faces is not dependent on holistic processing. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 13(10), 1019-1027. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsy079
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsy079 URL pmid: 30247645 |
[27] |
Nemrodov D., Anderson T., Preston F. F., & Itier R. J. (2014). Early sensitivity for eyes within faces: A new neuronal account of holistic and featural processing. NeuroImage, 97, 81-94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.042
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.042 URL pmid: 24768932 |
[28] |
Osborne C. D., & Stevenage S. V. (2013). Familiarity and face processing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(1), 108-120. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2012.699077
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.699077 URL |
[29] |
Popova T., & Wiese H. (2022). The time it takes to truly know someone: Neurophysiological correlates of face and identity learning during the first two years. Biological Psychology, 170, 108312. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2022.108312
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2022.108312 URL |
[30] |
Ramon M. (2015). Differential processing of vertical interfeature relations due to real-life experience with personally familiar faces. Perception, 44(4), 368-382. https://doi.org/10.1068/p7909
URL pmid: 26492723 |
[31] |
Ritchie K. L., Kramer R. S. S., Mileva M., Sandford A., & Burton A. M. (2021). Multiple-image arrays in face matching tasks with and without memory. Cognition, 211, 104632. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104632
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104632 URL |
[32] |
Rossion B. (2008). Picture-plane inversion leads to qualitative changes of face perception. Acta Psychologica, 128(2), 274-289.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.02.003 pmid: 18396260 |
[33] |
Rossion B. (2009). Distinguishing the cause and consequence of face inversion: The perceptual field hypothesis. Acta Psychologica, 132(3), 300-312.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.08.002 pmid: 19747674 |
[34] |
Royer J., Blais C., Barnabé-Lortie V., Carré M., Leclerc J., & Fiset D. (2016). Efficient visual information for unfamiliar face matching despite viewpoint variations: It’s not in the eyes! Vision Research, 123, 33-40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2016.04.004
doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2016.04.004 URL |
[35] |
Sandford A., & Bindemann M. (2020). Discrimination and recognition of faces with changed configuration. Memory & Cognition, 48(2), 287-298. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-019-01010-7
doi: 10.3758/s13421-019-01010-7 URL |
[36] |
Sugiura M., Mano Y., Sasaki A., & Sadato N. (2011). Beyond the memory mechanism: Person-selective and nonselective processes in recognition of personally familiar faces. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(3), 699-715. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21469
doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21469 URL pmid: 20350171 |
[37] |
Tanaka J. W., Curran T., Porterfield A. L., & Collins D. (2006). Activation of preexisting and acquired face representations: The N250 event-related potential as an index of face familiarity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(9), 1488-1497. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.9.1488
URL pmid: 16989550 |
[38] | Tanaka J. W., Kiefer M., & Bukach C. M. (2004). A holistic account of the own-race effect in face recognition: Evidence from a cross-cultural study. Cognition, 93(1), B1- B9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2003.09.011 |
[39] |
Tanaka J. W., & Sung A. (2016). The “eye avoidance” hypothesis of autism face processing. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(5), 1538-1552. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1976-7
doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1976-7 URL pmid: 24150885 |
[40] |
Towler J., Fisher K., & Eimer M. (2018). Holistic face perception is impaired in developmental prosopagnosia. Cortex, 108, 112-126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.07.019
doi: S0010-9452(18)30236-3 URL pmid: 30165324 |
[41] |
van Belle G., de Graef P., Verfaillie K., Rossion B., & Lefevre P. (2010). Face inversion impairs holistic perception: Evidence from gaze-contingent stimulation. Journal of Vision, 10(5), 10-10. https://doi.org/10.1167/10.5.10
doi: 10.1167/10.5.10 URL pmid: 20616142 |
[42] | Visconti di Oleggio Castello M., Wheeler K. G., Cipolli C., & Gobbini M. I. (2017). Familiarity facilitates feature- based face processing. Plos One, 12(6), e0178895. |
[43] |
Wang Z., Chen Y., Liu W., & Sun Y.-H. P. (2022). An eye region-specific cross-dimension covariation enhancement effect in facial featural and configural information change detection. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 54(3), 236. https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.00236
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.00236 URL |
[王哲, 陈亚春, 刘万鹏, 孙宇浩. (2022). 眼睛区域构型信息与特征信息的跨维共变增益效应及其加工特异性. 心理学报, 54(3), 236.] | |
[44] |
Wang Z., Quinn P. C., Jin H., Sun Y.-H. P., Tanaka J. W., Pascalis O., & Lee K. (2019). A regional composite-face effect for species-specific recognition: Upper and lower halves play different roles in holistic processing of monkey faces. Vision Research, 157, 89-96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2018.03.004
doi: S0042-6989(18)30047-6 URL pmid: 29653136 |
[45] | Wang Z., Quinn P. C., Tanaka J. W., Yu X., Sun Y.-H. P., Liu J., … Lee K. (2015). An other-race effect for configural and featural processing of faces: Upper and lower face regions play different roles. Frontiers in Psychology, 06, Article 599. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00559 |
[46] |
White D., Burton A. M., Jenkins R., & Kemp R. I. (2014). Redesigning photo-ID to improve unfamiliar face matching performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 20(2), 166-173. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000009
doi: 10.1037/xap0000009 URL |
[47] |
Wilcockson T. D., Burns E. J., Xia B., Tree J., & Crawford T. J. (2020). Atypically heterogeneous vertical first fixations to faces in a case series of people with developmental prosopagnosia. Visual Cognition, 28(4), 311-323. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2020.1797968
doi: 10.1080/13506285.2020.1797968 URL |
[48] |
Wolf J. M., Tanaka J. W., Klaiman C., Cockburn J., Herlihy L., Brown C., … Phillips R. (2008). Specific impairment of face‐processing abilities in children with autism spectrum disorder using the Let’s Face It! Skills battery. Autism Research, 1(6), 329-340. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.56
doi: 10.1002/aur.v1:6 URL |
[49] |
Zimmermann F. G., & Eimer M. (2013). Face learning and the emergence of view-independent face recognition: An event-related brain potential study. Neuropsychologia, 51(7), 1320-1329. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.03.028
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.03.028 URL pmid: 23583970 |
[1] | 王哲, 陈亚春, 刘万鹏, 孙宇浩. 眼睛区域构型信息与特征信息的跨维共变增益效应及其加工特异性[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(3): 236-247. |
[2] | 严璘璘;王哲;张智君;宋赛尉;孙宇浩. 知觉经验对面孔整体加工的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(10): 1199-1209. |
[3] | 樊倩;隋雪;符永川. 面孔知觉中特征、结构和整体加工策略的眼动研究[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(8): 1062-1071. |
[4] | 陈兰,翟细春,周新林. 两位数的整体与局部加工[J]. 心理学报, 2009, 41(05): 406-413. |
[5] | 周国梅,傅小兰. 异同判断加工中整体和局部特征的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2004, 36(06): 681-689. |
[6] | 刘瑞光,黄希庭. 运动视觉信息中时间知觉线索的实验研究[J]. 心理学报, 1999, 31(1): 15-20. |
[7] | 杨治良,叶阁蔚,王新发. 汉字内隐记忆的实验研究(Ⅰ)──内隐记忆存在的条件[J]. 心理学报, 1994, 26(1): 1-7. |
[8] | 王甦,王晓非. 刺激结构对视觉定位的影响[J]. 心理学报, 1993, 25(2): 8-14. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||