[1] |
Atchley P., Jones S. E., & Hoffman L. (2003). Visual marking: A convergence of goal-and stimulus-driven processes during visual search. Percept Psychophys, 65(5), 667.
|
[2] |
Berger A., & Henik A. (2000). The endogenous modulation of IOR is nasal-temporal asymmetric. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(3), 421-428.
|
[3] |
Berger A., Henik A., & Rafal R. (2005). Competition between endogenous and exogenous orienting of visual attention. Journal Experimental Psychology General, 134(2), 207-221.
|
[4] |
Botta F., Lupiáñez J., & Chica A. B. (2014). When endogenous spatial attention improves conscious perception: Effects of alerting and bottom-up activation. Consciousness & Cognition, 23, 63-73.
|
[5] |
Burg E. V. D., Olivers C. N. L., Bronkhorst A. W., & Theeuwes J. (2008). Pip and pop: Non-spatial auditory signals improve spatial visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception & Performance, 34(5), 1053-1065.
|
[6] |
Busse L., Katzner S., & Treue S. (2008). Temporal dynamics of neuronal modulation during exogenous and endogenous shifts of visual attention in macaque area MT. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(42), 16380-16385.
|
[7] |
Chica A. B., Bartolomeo P., & Lupiáñez J. (2013). Two cognitive and neural systems for endogenous and exogenous spatial attention. Behavioural Brain Research, 237, 107-123.
|
[8] |
Corbetta M., & Shulman G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201.
|
[9] |
Fairhall S. L., & Macaluso E. (2009). Spatial attention can modulate audiovisual integration at multiple cortical and subcortical sites. European Journal of Neuroscience, 29(6), 1247-1257.
|
[10] |
Folk C. L., & Remington R. (1998). Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: Evidence for two forms of attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception & Performan, 24(3), 847.
|
[11] |
Gowen E., Abadi R. V., Poliakoff E., Hansen P. C., & Miall R. C. (2007). Modulation of saccadic intrusion by exogenous and endogenous attention. Brain Research, 1141(1), 154-167.
|
[12] |
Granholm E., Asarnow R. F., Sarkin A. J., & Dykes K. L. (1996). Pupillary responses index cognitive resource limitations. Psychophysiology, 33(4), 457-461.
|
[13] |
Grubb M. A., White A. L., Heeger D. J., & Carrasco M. (2015). Interactions between voluntary and involuntary attention modulate the quality and temporal dynamics of visual processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(2), 437-444.
|
[14] |
Grundy J. G., Barker R. M., Anderson J. A. E., & Shedden J. M. (2019). The relation between brain signal complexity and task difficulty on an executive function task. NeuroImage, 198, 104-113.
|
[15] |
Gustavo R., Coull J. T., Nobre A. C., & Sam G. (2011). Behavioural dissociation between exogenous and endogenous temporal orienting of attention. Plos One, 6(1), e14620.
|
[16] |
Hopfinger J. B., & West V.M. (2006). Interactions between endogenous and exogenous attention on cortical visual processing. NeuroImage, 31(2), 774-789.
|
[17] |
Johnston W. A., & Heinz S.P. (1979). Depth of nontarget processing in an attention task. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception & Performance, 5(1), 168.
|
[18] |
Julia F. C., Daniel C., Beer A. L., & Daphne B. (2018). Neural bases of enhanced attentional control: Lessons from action video game players. Brain & Behavior, 8(7), e01019.
|
[19] |
Kosslyn S. M., Thompson W. L., Wraga M., & Alpert N. M. (2001). Imagining rotation by endogenous versus exogenous forces: Distinct neural mechanisms. Neuroreport, 12(11), 2519-2525.
|
[20] |
Lavie N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception & Performance, 21(3), 451-468.
|
[21] |
Lavie N. (2010). Attention, distraction, and cognitive control under load. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(3), 143-148.
|
[22] |
Lunn J., Sjoblom A., Ward J., Soto-Faraco S., & Forster S. (2019). Multisensory enhancement of attention depends on whether you are already paying attention. Cognition, 187, 38-49.
|
[23] |
Lupiáñez J., Botta F., Martín-Arévalo E., Chica A. B. (2014). The spatial orienting paradigm: How to design and interpret spatial attention experiments. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 40, 35-51.
|
[24] |
Mayer A. R., Dorflinger J. M., Rao S. M., & Seidenberg M. (2004). Neural networks underlying endogenous and exogenous visual-spatial orienting. Neuroimage, 23(2), 534-541.
|
[25] |
Meyer K. N., Du F., Parks E., & Hopfinger J. B. (2018). Exogenous vs. endogenous attention: Shifting the balance of fronto-parietal activity. Neuropsychologia, 111, 307-316.
|
[26] |
Miller J. (1982). Divided attention: Evidence for coactivation with redundant signals. Cognitive Psychology, 14(2), 247-279.
|
[27] |
Odegaard B., Wozny D. R., & Shams L. (2016). The effects of selective and divided attention on sensory precision and integration. Neuroscience Letters, 614(9), 24-28.
|
[28] |
Otten M., Schreij D., & Los S. A. (2016). The interplay of goal-driven and stimulus-driven influences on spatial orienting. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 78(6), 1642-1654.
|
[29] |
Pauszek J. R., & Gibson B.S. (2016). High spatial validity is not sufficient to elicit voluntary shifts of attention. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 78(7), 2110-2123.
|
[30] |
Peelen M. V., Heslenfeld D. J., & Theeuwes J. (2004). Endogenous and exogenous attention shifts are mediated by the same large-scale neural network. NeuroImage, 22(2), 822-830.
|
[31] |
Peng X., Chang R. S., Li Q., Wang A. J., & Tang X. Y. (2019). Visually induced inhibition of return affects the audiovisual integration under different SOA conditions. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 51(7), 759-771.
|
|
[ 彭姓, 常若松, 李奇, 王爱君, 唐晓雨. (2019). 不同SOA下视觉返回抑制对视听觉整合的调节作用. 心理学报, 51(7), 759-771.]
|
[32] |
Peng X., Chang R. S., Ren G. Q., Wang A. J., & Tang X. Y. (2018). The interaction between exogenous attention and multisensory integration. Advances in Psychological Science, 26(12), 43-54.
|
|
[ 彭姓, 常若松, 任桂琴, 王爱君, 唐晓雨. (2018). 外源性注意与多感觉整合的交互关系. 心理科学进展, 26(12), 43-54.]
|
[33] |
Posner M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1), 3-25.
|
[34] |
Senkowski D., Saint-Amour D., Hofle M., & Foxe J. J. (2011). Multisensory interactions in early evoked brain activity follow the principle of inverse effectiveness. NeuroImage, 56(4), 2200-2208.
|
[35] |
Senkowski D., Talsma D., Herrmann C. S., & Woldorff M. G. (2005). Multisensory processing and oscillatory gamma responses: Effects of spatial selective attention. Experimental Brain Research, 166(3-4), 411-426.
|
[36] |
Sugihara T., Diltz M. D., Averbeck B. B., & Romanski L. M. (2006). Integration of auditory and visual communication information in the primate ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(43), 11138-11147.
|
[37] |
Talsma D., Doty T. J., & Woldorff M. G. (2007). Selective attention and audiovisual integration: Is attending to both modalities a prerequisite for early integration? Cerebral Cortex, 17(3), 679-690.
|
[38] |
Talsma D., Senkowski D., Soto-Faraco S., & Woldorff M. G. (2010). The multifaceted interplay between attention and multisensory integration. Trends in Cognitive Science, 14(9), 400-410.
|
[39] |
Talsma D., & Woldorff M. G. (2005). Selective attention and multisensory integration: Multiple phases of effects on the evoked brain activity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(7), 1098-1114.
|
[40] |
Tang X., Wu J., & Shen Y. (2016). The interactions of multisensory integration with endogenous and exogenous attention. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 61, 208-224.
|
[41] |
Tang, X Y., Sun J. Y., & Peng X. (2020). The effect of bimodal divided attention on inhibition of return with audiovisual targets. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 52(3), 257-268.
|
|
[ 唐晓雨, 孙佳影, 彭姓. (2020). 双通道分配性注意对视听觉返回抑制的影响. 心理学报, 52(3), 257-268.]
|
[42] |
Tang, X Y., Wu Y. N., Peng X., Wang A. J., & Li Q. (2020). The influence of endogenous spatial cue validity on audiovisual integration. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 52(7), 835-846.
|
|
[ 唐晓雨, 吴英楠, 彭姓, 王爱君, 李奇. (2020). 内源性空间线索有效性对视听觉整合的影响. 心理学报, 52(7), 835-846.]
|
[43] |
van der Stoep N., van der Stigchel S., & Nijboer, T. C. W. (2015). Exogenous spatial attention decreases audiovisual integration. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77(2), 464-482.
|
[44] |
van der Stoep N., van der Stigchel S., Nijboer T. C. W., & Spence C. (2016). Visually induced inhibition of return affects the integration of auditory and visual information. Perception, 46(1), 6-17.
|
[45] |
Washburn D. A., & Putney R.T. (2001). Attention and task difficulty: When is performance facilitated?. Learning & Motivation, 32(1), 36-47.
|
[46] |
Yantis S., & Jonides J. (1990). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Voluntary versus automatic allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 16(1), 121-134.
|
[47] |
Yu, H. (2019). The effects of attention on multisensory integration (Unpublished master’s thesis). Liaoning Normal University, Dalian, China.
|
|
[ 于宏. (2019). 注意对多感觉整合的影响 (硕士学位论文). 辽宁师范大学, 大连. ]
|