心理学报 ›› 2018, Vol. 50 ›› Issue (11): 1197-1211.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.01197
• 研究报告 • 下一篇
收稿日期:
2017-09-07
发布日期:
2018-09-25
出版日期:
2018-11-25
通讯作者:
白学军,姚海娟
E-mail:bxuejun@126.com;yhjrenfei@163.com
基金资助:
Received:
2017-09-07
Online:
2018-09-25
Published:
2018-11-25
Contact:
BAI Xuejun,YAO Haijuan
E-mail:bxuejun@126.com;yhjrenfei@163.com
摘要:
本研究采用2个实验, 考察创造性思维测验得分高低者在Stroop任务干扰条件上的差异, 从行为和生理指标探讨认知抑制与创造性思维的关系, 以及时间压力对认知抑制与创造性思维关系的调节作用。实验1采用Stroop颜色命名任务。结果发现, 相比低创者, 高创者的反应时干扰效应量和正确率干扰效应量均更小。实验2采用更灵活的Stroop字义-颜色命名转换任务, 操纵不同的时间压力条件, 并记录被试完成任务时的皮肤电活动。结果发现, 高创者在有时间压力条件下的干扰效应量显著小于无时间压力条件下, 而低创者在有和无时间压力条件下的干扰效应量无显著差异; 高创者在颜色命名任务的不一致条件下的皮肤电活动变化显著高于一致条件, 而低创者在颜色命名任务的一致和不一致条件下无显著差异。研究表明:总体而言, 相比低创者, 高创者的认知抑制能力更高, 能够有效抑制优势的但不相关的反应倾向。时间压力在认知抑制与创造性思维的关系中起调节作用, 高创者面对不同任务要求能够灵活调整自身的认知抑制水平, 并表现出变化的生理唤醒水平。结果支持创造性思维的适应性认知抑制假说。
中图分类号:
白学军, 姚海娟. (2018). 高低创造性思维水平者的认知抑制能力:行为和生理的证据. 心理学报, 50(11), 1197-1211.
BAI Xuejun, YAO Haijuan. (2018). Differences in cognitive inhibition between persons with high and low creativity: Evidences from behavioral and physiological studies. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 50(11), 1197-1211.
创造性组别 | 字色一致 | 字色不一致 |
---|---|---|
高创组 | 97.60(3.55) | 96.98 (2.39) |
低创组 | 98.64(2.55) | 95.73(4.14) |
表1 高创组和低创组在各实验条件下的正确率(%) [M (SD)]
创造性组别 | 字色一致 | 字色不一致 |
---|---|---|
高创组 | 97.60(3.55) | 96.98 (2.39) |
低创组 | 98.64(2.55) | 95.73(4.14) |
时间压力 情境 | 创造性组别 | 字义命名 | 颜色命名 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
一致 | 不一致 | 一致 | 不一致 | ||
有时间压力 | 高创组 | 92.36 (7.29) | 82.29 (14.56) | 91.32 (6.52) | 83.33 (7.95) |
低创组 | 89.93 (6.76) | 80.21 (14.56) | 92.71 (7.13) | 80.21 (10.22) | |
无时间压力 | 高创组 | 96.88 (5.06) | 96.53 (3.91) | 99.31 (2.41) | 95.49 (5.46) |
低创组 | 98.61 (2.71) | 95.83 (4.35) | 98.96 (2.59) | 90.97 (7.07) |
表2 高创组和低创组在不同时间压力情境下的正确率(%) [M (SD)]
时间压力 情境 | 创造性组别 | 字义命名 | 颜色命名 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
一致 | 不一致 | 一致 | 不一致 | ||
有时间压力 | 高创组 | 92.36 (7.29) | 82.29 (14.56) | 91.32 (6.52) | 83.33 (7.95) |
低创组 | 89.93 (6.76) | 80.21 (14.56) | 92.71 (7.13) | 80.21 (10.22) | |
无时间压力 | 高创组 | 96.88 (5.06) | 96.53 (3.91) | 99.31 (2.41) | 95.49 (5.46) |
低创组 | 98.61 (2.71) | 95.83 (4.35) | 98.96 (2.59) | 90.97 (7.07) |
[1] | Ansburg, P. I., & Hill , K. (2003). Creative and analytic thinkers differ in their use of attentional resources. Personality and Individual Differences, 34( 7), 1141-1152. |
[2] | Amabile T. M., Hadley C. N., & Kramer S. J . (2002). Creativity under the gun. Harvard Business Review, 80( 8), 52-61. |
[3] |
Bach, D. R., & Friston, K. J . (2013). Model-based analysis of skin conductance responses: Towards causal models in psychophysiology. Psychophysiology, 50( 1), 15-22.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01483.x URL pmid: 23094650 |
[4] | Bai X. J., Gong Y. B., Hu W. P., Han Q., & Yao H. J . ( 2014). The inhibitory mechanism of individuals with different scientific creativity. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 12( 2), 151-155. |
[ 白学军, 巩彦斌, 胡卫平, 韩琴, 姚海娟 . ( 2014). 不同科学创造力个体干扰抑制机制的比较. 心理与行为研究, 12( 2), 151-155.] | |
[5] | Beaty R. E., Silvia P. J., Nusbaum E. C., Jauk E., & Benedek M . (2014). The role of associative and executive processes in creative cognition. Memory & Cognition, 42( 7), 1186-1197. |
[6] |
Benedek M., Franz F., Heene M., & Neubauer A. C . (2012). Differential effects of cognitive inhibition and intelligence on creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 53( 4), 480-485.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.014 URL |
[7] | Benedek M., Jauk E., Fink A., Koschutnig K., Reishofer G., Ebner F., & Neubauer A. C . (2014 a). To create or to recall? Neural mechanisms underlying the generation of creative new ideas. NeuroImage, 88, 125-133. |
[8] | Benedek M., Jauk E., Sommer M., Arendasy M., & Neubauer A. C . (2014 b). Intelligence, creativity, and cognitive control: The common and differential involvement of executive functions in intelligence and creativity. Intelligence, 46, 73-83. |
[9] |
Boucsein W., Fowles D. C., Grimnes S., Ben-Shakhar G., Roth W. T., Dawson M. E., & Filion D. L . (2012). Publication recommendations for electrodermal measurements. Society for psychophysiological research Ad Hoc committee on electrodermal measures. Psychophysiology, 49( 8), 1017-1034.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01384.x URL pmid: 7291438 |
[10] |
Burch G. S. J., Hemsley D. R., Pavelis C., & Corr P. J . (2006). Personality, Creativity and Latent inhibition. European Journal of Personality, 20( 2), 107-122.
doi: 10.1002/per.572 URL |
[11] | Carson S. H., Peterson J. B., & Higgins D. M . (2003). Decreased latent inhibition is associated with increased achievement in high-functioning individuals. J ournal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85( 3), 499-506. |
[12] | Cheng L. F., Hu W. P., & Jia X. J . ( 2015). Cognitive inhibition and artistic creativity: The moderating effect of cognitive style. Psychological Development and Education, 31( 3), 287-295. |
[ 程丽芳, 胡卫平, 贾小娟 . ( 2015). 认知抑制对艺术创造力的影响: 认知风格的调节作用. 心理发展与教育, 31( 3), 287-295.]
doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2015.03.05 URL |
|
[13] |
Cheng L. F., Hu W. P., Jia X. J., & Runco M. A . (2016). The different role of cognitive inhibition in early versus late creative problem finding. Psychological of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 10( 1), 32-41.
doi: 10.1037/aca0000036 URL |
[14] |
Chirila, C., & Feldman , A. (2012). Study of latent inhibition at high-level creative personality: The link between creativity and psychopathology. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 33, 353-357.
doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.01.142 URL |
[15] | Colombo B., Bartesaghi N., Simonelli L., & Antonietti A . (2015). The combined effects of neurostimulation and priming on creative thinking. A preliminary tDCS study on dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 403-414. |
[16] |
Darini M., Pazhouhesh H., & Moshiri F . (2011). Relationship between employee’s innovation (creativity) and time management. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 25, 201-213.
doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.10.541 URL pmid: 17054907 |
[17] |
Dawson M. E., Schell A.M., & Courtney C.G . (2011). The skin conductance response, anticipation, and decision-making. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 4( 2), 111-116.
doi: 10.1037/a0022619 URL |
[18] |
Demaree H. A., Schmeichel B. J., Robinson J. L., Pu J., Everhart D. E., & Berntson G. G . (2006). Up- and down regulating facial disgust: Affective, vagal, sympathetic, and respiratory consequences. Biological Psychology, 71( 1), 90-99.
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.02.006 URL |
[19] |
Dorfman L., Martindale C., Gassimova V., & Vartanian O . (2008). Creativity and speed of information processing: A double dissociation involving elementary versus inhibitory cognitive tasks. Personality and Individual Differences, 44( 6), 1382-1390.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.12.006 URL |
[20] |
Edl S., Benedek M., Papousek I., Weiss E. M., & Fink A . (2014). Creativity and the Stroop interference effect. Personality and Individual Differences, 69, 38-42.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.05.009 URL |
[21] | Eysenck H. J. (1995). Problems in the behavioural sciences, 12 Genius: The natural history of creativity. New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press. |
[22] | Fink, A., & Benedek, M . (2014). EEG alpha power and creative ideation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 44, 111-123. |
[23] |
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133( 1), 101-135.
doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.101 URL |
[24] |
Galang A. J. R., Castelo V. L. C., Santos L. C., Perlas C. M. C ., & Angeles, M. A. B. (2016). Investigating the prosocial psychopath model of the creative personality: Evidence from traits and psychophysiology. Personality and Individual Differences, 100, 28-36.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.081 URL |
[25] | Gendolla G. H. E., Wright, R. A., & Richter, M.(2012) . Effort intensity: Some insights from the cardiovascular system. In R. M. Ryan (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of human motivation (pp. 420-438). New York: Oxford University Press. |
[26] |
Groborz, M., & Necka, E. (2003). Creativity and cognitive control: explorations of generation and evaluation skills. Creativity Research Journal, 15( 2-3), 183-197.
doi: 10.1207/S15326934CRJ152&3_09 URL |
[27] | Gu C. H., Wang Y. L., Wu C. F., Xie X. L., Cui C. Z., Wang Y. X., … Zhou Z. K . ( 2015). Brain correlates underlying social creative thinking: EEG Alpha activity in trait vs. state creativity. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 47( 6), 765-773. |
[ 谷传华, 王亚丽, 吴财付, 谢祥龙, 崔承珠, 王亚娴 , … 周宗奎. ( 2015). 社会创造性的脑机制: 状态与特质的EEG α波活动特点. 心理学报, 47( 6), 765-773.] | |
[28] |
Harnishfeger K. K. (1995). The development of cognitive inhibition: Theories, definitions, and research evidence. In F. N. Dempster & C. J. Brainerd (Eds.), Interference and inhibition in cognition (pp.175-204). San Diego, CA, US: Academic Press.
doi: 10.1016/B978-012208930-5/50007-6 URL |
[29] | Hu W. P., Cheng L. F., Jia X. J., Han M., & Chen Y. H . ( 2015). The influence of cognitive inhibition to creative scientific problem finding: Mediating effect of field cognitive style. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 13( 6), 721-728. |
[ 胡卫平, 程丽芳, 贾小娟, 韩蒙, 陈英和 . ( 2015). 认知抑制对创造性科学问题提出的影响:认知风格的中介作用. 心理与行为研究, 13( 6), 721-728.] | |
[30] |
Huang P. H., Qiu L. H., Shen L., Zhang Y., Song Z., Qi Z. Q., … Xie P . (2013). Evidence for a left-over-right inhibitory mechanism during figural creative thinking in healthy nonartists. Human Brain Mapping, 34( 10), 2724-2732.
doi: 10.1002/hbm.v34.10 URL |
[31] |
Jauk E., Benedek M., Dunst B., & Neubauer A. C . (2013). The relationship between intelligence and creativity: New support for the threshold hypothesis by means of empirical breakpoint detection. Intelligence, 41( 4), 212-221.
doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2013.03.003 URL |
[32] |
Jauk E., Benedek M., & Neubauer A. C . (2014). The road to creative achievement: A latent variable model of ability and personality predictors. European Journal of Personality, 28( 1), 95-105.
doi: 10.1002/per.v28.1 URL |
[33] |
Karau, S. J., & Kelly, J. R . (1992). The effects of time scarcity and time abundance on group performance quality and interaction process. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 28( 6), 542-571.
doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(92)90045-L URL |
[34] |
Kobayashi N., Yoshino A., Takahashi Y., & Nomura S . (2007). Autonomic arousal in cognitive conflict resolution. Autonomic Neuroscience, 132( 1-2), 70-75.
doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2006.09.004 URL pmid: 17067858 |
[35] | Kris, E. (1952). Psychoanalytic exploration in art. New York: International Universities Press. |
[36] | Kwiatkowski, J. (2002). Individual differences in the neurophysiology of creativity (Unpublished doctorial dissertation). The University of Maine. |
[37] |
Lemche E., Sierra-Siegert M., David A. S., Phillips M. L., Gasston D., Williams S. C. R., & Giampietro V. P . (2016). Cognitive load and autonomic response patterns under negative priming demand in depersonalization-derealization disorder. European Journal of Neuroscience, 43( 7), 971-978.
doi: 10.1111/ejn.13183 URL |
[38] |
MacDonald A. W., Cohen J. D., Stenger V. A., & Carter C. S . (2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288( 5472), 1835-1838.
doi: 10.1126/science.288.5472.1835 URL |
[39] | Martindale, C. (1999). Biological basis of creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 137-152). New York: Cambridge University Press. |
[40] |
Martindale, C. (2007). Creativity, primordial cognition, and personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 43( 7), 1777-1785.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.05.014 URL |
[41] | Mayseless, N., & Shamay-Tsoory, S. G . (2015). Enhancing verbal creativity: Modulating creativity by altering the balance between right and left inferior frontal gyrus with tDCS. Neuroscience, 291, 167-176. |
[42] |
Patterson J. C., Ungerleider L. G., & Bandettini P. A . (2002). Task-independent functional brain activity correlation with skin conductance changes: An fMRI study. Neurolmage, 17( 4), 1797-1806.
doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1306 URL |
[43] |
Pennebaker, J. W., & Chew, C. H . (1985). Behavioral inhibition and electrodermal activity during deception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49( 5), 1427-1433.
doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.49.5.1427 URL pmid: 4078683 |
[44] |
Peterson, J. B., & Carson, S. H . (2000). Latent inhibition and openness to experience in a high-achieving student population. Personality and Individual Differences, 28( 2), 323-332.
doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00101-4 URL |
[45] |
Peterson J. B., Smith K. W., & Carson S . (2002). Openness and extraversion are associated with reduced latent inhibition: Replication and commentary. Personality and Individual Differences, 33( 7), 1137-1147.
doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00004-1 URL |
[46] |
Posada-Quintero H. F., Florian J. P., Orjuela-Cañón A. D., Aljama-Corrales T., Charleston-Villalobos S., & Chon K. H . (2016). Power spectral density analysis of electrodermal activity for sympathetic function assessment. Annals of Biomedical Engineering, 44( 10), 3124-3135.
doi: 10.1007/s10439-016-1606-6 URL |
[47] |
Radel R., Davranche K., Fournier M., & Dietrich A . (2015). The role of (dis)inhibition in creativity: Decreased inhibition improves idea generation. Cognition, 134, 110-120.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.09.001 URL |
[48] |
Rominger C., Fink A., Weiss E. M., Bosch J., & Papousek I . (2017). Allusive thinking (remote associations) and auditory top-down inhibition skills differentially predict creativity and positive schizotypy. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 22( 2), 108-121.
doi: 10.1080/13546805.2016.1278361 URL |
[49] |
Silvia P. J., Beaty R. E., Nusbaum E. C., Eddington K. M., & Kwapil T. R . (2014). Creative motivation: Creative achievement predicts cardiac autonomic markers of effort during divergent thinking. Biological Psychology, 102, 30-37.
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.07.010 URL |
[50] |
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I . (1996). Investigating in creativity. American Psychologist, 51( 7), 677-688.
doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.51.7.677 URL |
[51] | Storm, B. C., & Patel, T. N . (2014). Forgetting as a consequence and enabler of creative thinking. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40( 6), 1594-1609. |
[52] |
Stroop, J. R . (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18( 6), 643-662.
doi: 10.1037/h0054651 URL |
[53] | Szollos, A. (2009). Toward a psychology of chronic time pressure: Conceptual and methodological review. Time & Society, 18( 2-3), 332-350. |
[54] | Vartanian, O. A . (2002). Cognitive disinhibition and creativity. ( Unpublished doctorial dissertation), The University of Maine, Orono, The United States of America. |
[55] | Vartanian, O. (2009). Variable attention facilitates creative problem solving. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3( 1), 57-59. |
[56] |
Vartanian O., Martindale C., & Kwiatkowski J . (2007). Creative potential, attention, and speed of information processing. Personality and Individual Differences, 43( 6), 1470-1480.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.04.027 URL |
[57] | Wo J. Z., Chen W. R., Liu Y., & Lin C. D . ( 2010). The eye movements differences during category learning process between high and low creativity students. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 42( 2), 251-261. |
[ 沃建中, 陈婉茹, 刘杨, 林崇德 .( 2010). 创造能力不同学生的分类加工过程差异的眼动特点. 心理学报, 42( 2), 251-261.] | |
[58] | Yao, H. J., & Bai, X. J . ( 2014). The relationship between creative thinking and cognitive inhibition. Journal of Psychological Science, 37( 2), 316-321. |
[ 姚海娟, 白学军 . ( 2014). 创造性思维与认知抑制的关系. 心理科学, 37( 2), 316-321.] | |
[59] | Ye R. M., Hong D. H., & Torrance P. E . ( 1988). Cross cultural comparison of creative thinking between Chinese and American students using Torrance Test. Chinese Journal of Applied Psychology, 3( 3), 22-29. |
[ 叶仁敏, 洪德厚 , 保尔·托兰斯. ( 1988). 《托兰斯创造性思维测验》(TTCT)的测试和中美学生的跨文化比较. 应用心理学, 3( 3), 22-29.] | |
[60] |
Zabelina, D. L., & Robinson, M. D . (2010). Creativity as flexible cognitive control. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4( 3), 136-143.
doi: 10.1037/a0017379 URL |
[61] |
Zabelina D. L., Robinson M. D., Council J. R., & Bresin K . (2012). Patterning and nonpatterning in creative cognition: Insights from performance in a random number generation task. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6( 2), 137-145
doi: 10.1037/a0025452 URL |
[62] |
Zabelina, D. L., & Beeman , M. (2013). Short-term attentional perseveration associated with real-life creative achievement. Frontiers in Psychology, 4( 191), 1-8.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00191 URL pmid: 3635848 |
[63] | Zhang K., Du X. M., Tong Y. G . ( 2017). The Differences between High Creative Persons and Low Creative Persons in Directing Forgetting Effects. Journal of Psychological Science, 40( 3), 514-519. |
[ 张克, 杜秀敏, 仝宇光 . ( 2017). 高低创造性思维水平者定向遗忘效应的差异研究. 心理科学, 40( 3), 514-519.] | |
[64] | Zhang L. J., Qiao L., Chen Q. L., Yang W. J., Xu M. S., Yao X. N., … Yang D . (2016). Gray matter volume of the lingual gyrus mediates the relationship between inhibition function and divergent thinking. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1532-1541. |
[65] | Zhang, H-C., & Wang, X-P . ( 1989). Standardization research on RAVEN’s standard progressive matrices in China. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2, 113-121. |
[ 张厚粲, 王晓平 . ( 1989). 瑞文标准推理测验在我国的修订. 心理学报, 2, 113-121.] | |
[66] | Zhang J. H., Wang Y. N., Chu Y. X., & Xu X. Z . ( 2011). The effects of three pressures and creative self-efficacy on creativity. Journal of Psychological Science, 34( 4), 993-998. |
[ 张景焕, 王亚男, 初玉霞, 徐希铮 . ( 2011). 三种压力与创意自我效能感对创造力的影响. 心理科学, 34( 4), 993-998.] | |
[67] | Zhou, Y . (2013). The effects of emotional states on executive functioning. Advances in Psychological Science, 21( 7), 1186-1199. |
[ 周雅 . (2013). 情绪唤起对执行功能的作用. 心理科学进展, 21( 7), 1186-1199.] | |
[68] | Zmigrod S., Zmigrod L., & Hommel B . (2015). Zooming into creativity: Individual differences in attentional global- local biases are linked to creative thinking. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1647-1654. |
[69] |
Zvi L., Nachson I., & Elaad E . (2012). Effects of coping and cooperative instructions on guilty and informed innocents’ physiological responses to concealed information. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 84( 2), 140-148.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.01.022 URL |
[1] | 陈嫣然, 梁正, 赵庆柏, 黄宇, 李松清, 于全磊, 周治金. 创造性思维中语义搜索过程:基于答案累积时间函数和语义相似性的量化分析[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(8): 881-891. |
[2] | 钟毅平, 张文洁, 李亚蕾, 范伟. 时间压力对错误记忆的影响:情绪的调节作用 *[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(9): 929-939. |
[3] | 刘湍丽, 白学军. 部分线索对记忆提取的影响:认知抑制能力的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(9): 1158-1171. |
[4] | 赵庆柏;柯娓;童彪;周治金; 周宗奎. 网络语言的创造性加工过程:新颖N400与LPC[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(2): 143-154. |
[5] | 赵庆柏, 魏琳琳, 李瑛, 周治金, 赵黎莉, 唐磊. 新颖语义联结形成的右半球优势效应[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(11): 1370-1382. |
[6] | 周元元, 胡杨利, 张琴, 赵彦成. 时间压力下你想听什么?参照组影响对冲动购买的调节[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(11): 1439-1448. |
[7] | 范亮艳;范晓芳;罗位超;吴功航;严序;尹大志;吕岳;朱君明;徐冬溶. 艺术设计中创造性思维的fMRI研究:一项基于智能CAD的探索[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(4): 427-436 . |
[8] | 王芹,白学军,郭龙健,沈德立. 负性情绪抑制对社会决策行为的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2012, 44(5): 690-697. |
[9] | 原琳,彭明,刘丹玮,周仁来. 认知评价对主观情绪感受和生理活动的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2011, 43(08): 898-906. |
[10] | 崔亚飞,李红,李富洪. 归纳推理属性效应中背景关系提取原则的探究[J]. 心理学报, 2010, 42(12): 1148-1155. |
[11] | 白学军,朱昭红,沈德立,刘楠. 奖惩线索条件下内外倾个体的自主唤醒和行为反应[J]. 心理学报, 2009, 41(06): 492-500. |
[12] | 王勇慧,朱昭红,汪萍,王振宏. 奖惩对注意缺陷多动障碍儿童情感决策的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2008, 40(09): 975-986. |
[13] | 刘儒德,陈琦. 不同情境下临场自我调节学习活动对学习结果的中介影响[J]. 心理学报, 2000, 32(2): 197-202. |
[14] | 向亦文,阎克乐,陆运青. 大学生皮肤电反应、MMPI及其关系的初步研究[J]. 心理学报, 2000, 32(1): 95-98. |
[15] | 施建农,徐凡. 超常与常态儿童的兴趣、动机与创造性思维的比较研究[J]. 心理学报, 1997, 29(3): 271-277. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||