Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2023, Vol. 31 ›› Issue (5): 736-746.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2023.00736
• Regular Articles • Previous Articles Next Articles
YE Shuqi, YIN Junting, LI Zhaoxian, LUO Junlong()
Received:
2022-06-18
Online:
2023-05-15
Published:
2023-02-13
Contact:
LUO Junlong
E-mail:luo831023@163.com
CLC Number:
YE Shuqi, YIN Junting, LI Zhaoxian, LUO Junlong. The influence mechanism of emotion on intuitive and analytical processing[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(5): 736-746.
[1] |
艾炎, 胡竹菁. (2018). 推理判断中双重加工过程的协作与转换机制. 心理科学进展, 26(10), 1794-1806.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.01794 |
[2] |
Averbeck, J. M., Jones, A., & Robertson, K. (2011). Prior knowledge and health messages: An examination of affect as heuristics and information as systematic processing for fear appeals. Southern Communication Journal, 76(1), 35-54.
doi: 10.1080/10417940902951824 URL |
[3] |
Awad, S., Debatin, T., & Ziegler, A. (2021). Embodiment: I sat, I felt, I performed-Posture effects on mood and cognitive performance. Acta Psychologica, 218, 103353.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2021.103353 URL |
[4] |
Bago, B., & de Neys, W. (2017). Fast logic?: Examining the time course assumption of dual process theory. Cognition, 158, 90-109.
doi: S0010-0277(16)30254-2 pmid: 27816844 |
[5] |
Bago, B., & de Neys, W. (2019). The intuitive greater good: Testing the corrective dual process model of moral cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(10), 1782-1801.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000533 URL |
[6] |
Barasz, K., & Hagerty, S. F. (2021). Hoping for the Worst? A Paradoxical Preference for Bad News. Journal of Consumer Research, 48(2), 270-288.
doi: 10.1093/jcr/ucab004 URL |
[7] |
Barkus, E. (2020). Effects of working memory training on emotion regulation: Transdiagnostic review. PsyCh Journal, 9(2), 258-279.
doi: 10.1002/pchj.353 pmid: 32166891 |
[8] |
Baumann, N., & Kuhl, J. (2002). Intuition, affect, and personality: Unconscious coherence judgments and self- regulation of negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(5), 1213-1223.
pmid: 12416923 |
[9] |
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5(4), 323-370.
doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323 URL |
[10] |
Bodenhausen, G. V., Sheppard, L. A., & Kramer, G. P. (1994). Negative affect and social judgment: The differential impact of anger and sadness. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24(1), 45-62.
doi: 10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0992 URL |
[11] |
Bolte, A., Goschke, T., & Kuhl, J. (2003). Emotion and intuition: Effects of positive and negative mood on implicit judgments of semantic coherence. Psychological Science, 14(5), 416-421.
doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01456 pmid: 12930470 |
[12] |
Bookbinder, S. H., & Brainerd, C. J. (2017). Emotionally negative pictures enhance gist memory. Emotion, 17(1), 102-119.
doi: 10.1037/emo0000171 pmid: 27454002 |
[13] |
Boot, A. B., Dijkstra, K., & Zwaan, R. A. (2021). The processing and evaluation of news content on social media is influenced by peer-user commentary. Humanities and Social Sciences Communications, 8(1), 1-11.
doi: 10.1057/s41599-020-00684-8 |
[14] |
Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation I: Defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion, 1(3), 276-298.
pmid: 12934687 |
[15] |
Brashier, N. M., & Marsh, E. J. (2020). Judging truth. Annual Review of Psychology, 71, 499-515.
doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010419-050807 pmid: 31514579 |
[16] |
Breuer, C., Rumpf, C., & Boronczyk, F. (2021). Sponsor message processing in live broadcasts—A pilot study on the role of game outcome uncertainty and emotions. Psychology & Marketing, 38(5), 896-907.
doi: 10.1002/mar.v38.5 URL |
[17] |
Brunetti, M., Perrucci, M. G., di Naccio, M. R., Ferretti, A., del Gratta, C., Casadio, C., & Romani, G. L. (2014). Framing deductive reasoning with emotional content: An fMRI study. Brain and Cognition, 87, 153-160.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.03.017 pmid: 24747514 |
[18] |
Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source versus message cues in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(5), 752-766.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.39.5.752 URL |
[19] |
Clewett, D. V., Huang, R., Velasco, R., Lee, T. H., & Mather, M. (2018). Locus coeruleus activity strengthens prioritized memories under arousal. Journal of Neuroscience, 38(6), 1558-1574.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2097-17.2017 pmid: 29301874 |
[20] |
Curci, A., Lanciano, T., Soleti, E., & Rimé, B. (2013). Negative emotional experiences arouse rumination and affect working memory capacity. Emotion, 13(5), 867-880.
doi: 10.1037/a0032492 pmid: 23731432 |
[21] |
de Neys, W. (2012). Bias and conflict: A case for logical intuitions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(1), 28-38.
doi: 10.1177/1745691611429354 pmid: 26168420 |
[22] | de Neys, W. (2014). Conflict detection, dual processes, and logical intuitions: Some clarifications. Thinking & Reasoning, 20(2), 169-187. |
[23] |
de Neys, W. (2021). On dual-and single-process models of thinking. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(6), 1412-1427.
doi: 10.1177/1745691620964172 pmid: 33621468 |
[24] |
de Neys, W., & Pennycook, G. (2019). Logic, Fast and Slow: Advances in Dual-Process Theorizing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(5), 503-509.
doi: 10.1177/0963721419855658 |
[25] |
de Vries, M., Holland, R. W., Corneille, O., Rondeel, E., & Witteman, C. L. (2012). Mood effects on dominated choices: Positive mood induces departures from logical rules. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 25(1), 74-81.
doi: 10.1002/bdm.716 URL |
[26] |
di Muro, F., & Murray, K. B. (2012). An arousal regulation explanation of mood effects on consumer choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(3), 574-584.
doi: 10.1086/664040 URL |
[27] |
Dolcos, F., LaBar, K. S., & Cabeza, R. (2004). Dissociable effects of arousal and valence on prefrontal activity indexing emotional evaluation and subsequent memory: An event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage, 23(1), 64-74.
pmid: 15325353 |
[28] |
Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66(3), 183-201.
pmid: 13658305 |
[29] |
Evans, J. St. B. T. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255-278.
pmid: 18154502 |
[30] |
Evans, J. St. B. T. (2011). Dual-process theories of reasoning: Contemporary issues and developmental applications. Developmental Review, 31(2-3), 86-102.
doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2011.07.007 URL |
[31] | Evans, J. St. B. T. (2019). Reflections on reflection: The nature and function of type 2 processes in dual-process theories of reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning, 25(4), 383-415. |
[32] |
Evans, J. S. B., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 223-241.
doi: 10.1177/1745691612460685 pmid: 26172965 |
[33] |
Fedorikhin, A., & Patrick, V. M. (2010). Positive mood and resistance to temptation: The interfering influence of elevated arousal. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(4), 698-711.
doi: 10.1086/655665 URL |
[34] | Feldman, V., Hristova, P., & Kokinov, B. (2010). How does anxiety influence analogical mapping? In S. Ohlsson & R. Catrambone (Eds.), Cognition in Flux: Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1453-1458). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum. |
[35] |
Forgas, J. P. (2019). Happy believers and sad skeptics? Affective influences on gullibility. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(3), 306-313.
doi: 10.1177/0963721419834543 |
[36] |
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56(3), 218-226.
doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.56.3.218 pmid: 11315248 |
[37] |
Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13(1), 34-40.
doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00406 pmid: 11892776 |
[38] |
Gilet, A. L., & Jallais, C. (2011). Valence, arousal and word associations. Cognition and Emotion, 25(4), 740-746.
doi: 10.1080/02699931.2010.500480 URL |
[39] | Gladwin, T. E., & Figner, B. (2014). “Hot” cognition and dual systems:Introduction, criticisms, and ways forward. In E.Wilhelms & V. F.Reyna (Eds.), Frontiers of cognitive psychology series: Neuroeconomics, judgment, and decision making (pp. 157-180). New York, NY: Psychology Press. |
[40] |
Gonçalves, M., & Cash, P. (2021). The life cycle of creative ideas: Towards a dual-process theory of ideation. Design Studies, 72, 100988.
doi: 10.1016/j.destud.2020.100988 URL |
[41] |
Gronchi, G., Righi, S., Pierguidi, L., Giovannelli, F., Murasecco, I., & Viggiano, M. P. (2018). Automatic and controlled attentional orienting in the elderly: A dual- process view of the positivity effect. Acta Psychologica, 185, 229-234.
doi: S0001-6918(17)30291-3 pmid: 29550693 |
[42] | Gupta, U. (2019). Interplay of germane load and motivation during math problem solving using worked examples. Educational Research: Theory and Practice, 30(1), 67-71. |
[43] |
Gyurak, A., Gross, J. J., & Etkin, A. (2011). Explicit and implicit emotion regulation: A dual-process framework. Cognition and Emotion, 25(3), 400-412.
doi: 10.1080/02699931.2010.544160 pmid: 21432682 |
[44] |
Haj-Ali, H., Anderson, A. K., & Kron, A. (2020). Comparing three models of arousal in the human brain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 15(1), 1-11.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa012 pmid: 31993651 |
[45] |
Han, L., Liu, Y., Zhang, D., Jin, Y., & Luo, Y. (2013). Low-arousal speech noise improves performance in n-back task: An ERP study. PloS One, 8(10), e76261.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076261 URL |
[46] | Handley, S. J., & Trippas, D. (2015). Chapter Two-Dual processes and the interplay between knowledge and structure: A new parallel processing model. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 62, 33-58. |
[47] |
Handley, S. J., Newstead, S. E., & Trippas, D. (2011). Logic, beliefs, and instruction: A test of the default interventionist account of belief bias. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(1), 28-43.
doi: 10.1037/a0021098 URL |
[48] |
Huntsinger, J. R., & Ray, C. (2016). A flexible influence of affective feelings on creative and analytic performance. Emotion, 16(6), 826-837.
doi: 10.1037/emo0000188 pmid: 27078743 |
[49] |
Isen, A. M., & Patrick, R. (1983). The effect of positive feelings on risk taking: When the chips are down. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 31(2), 194-202.
doi: 10.1016/0030-5073(83)90120-4 URL |
[50] |
Ito, T. A., Larsen, J. T., Smith, N. K., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: The negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(4), 887-900.
doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.75.4.887 pmid: 9825526 |
[51] |
Keren, G., & Schul, Y. (2009). Two is not always better than one: A critical evaluation of two-system theories. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(6), 533-550.
doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01164.x pmid: 26161732 |
[52] |
Kim, Y., Park, K., Kim, Y., Yang, W., Han, D., & Kim, W. S. (2020). The impact of visual art and high affective arousal on heuristic decision-making in consumers. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 565829.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.565829 URL |
[53] |
Kircanski, K., Notthoff, N., DeLiema, M., Samanez-Larkin, G. R., Shadel, D., Mottola, G.,... Gotlib, I. H. (2018). Emotional arousal may increase susceptibility to fraud in older and younger adults. Psychology and Aging, 33(2), 325-337.
doi: 10.1037/pag0000228 pmid: 29658750 |
[54] | Körner, A., & Strack, F. (2018). Embodiment:Reflective and impulsive processes. In G. Hauke & A. Kritikos (Eds.), Embodiment: A practitioner‘s guide (pp.97-112). New York, NY: Springer. |
[55] |
Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F. (2000). Affect and stereotypic thinking: A test of the mood-and-general-knowledge- model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(12), 1587-1597.
doi: 10.1177/01461672002612012 URL |
[56] |
Kron, A., Pilkiw, M., Banaei, J., Goldstein, A., & Anderson, A. K. (2015). Are valence and arousal separable in emotional experience? Emotion, 15(1), 35-44.
doi: 10.1037/a0038474 pmid: 25664950 |
[57] |
Laird, J. D., Wagener, J. J., Halal, M., & Szegda, M. (1982). Remembering what you feel: Effects of emotion on memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42(4), 646-657.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.42.4.646 URL |
[58] |
Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., & Hamm, A. O. (1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology, 30(3), 261-273.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb03352.x pmid: 8497555 |
[59] | Langley, S. (2018). Facilitating positive emotions for greater creativity and innovation. In L. Petitta, C. E. J. Härtel, N. M. Ashkanasy, & W. J. Zerbe (Eds.), Research on emotion in organizations: Vol. 14: Individual, relational, and contextual dynamics of emotions (pp.259-270). Bingley, UK: Emerald Publishing Ltd. |
[60] |
Li, X., Li, Y., Wang, X., Fan, X., Tong, W., & Hu, W. (2020). The effects of emotional valence on insight problem solving in global-local processing: An ERP study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 155, 194-203.
doi: S0167-8760(20)30165-3 pmid: 32599003 |
[61] |
Li, Y., Xiao, X., Ma, W., Jiang, J., Qiu, J., & Zhang, Q. (2013). Electrophysiological evidence for emotional valence and competitive arousal effects on insight problem solving. Brain Research, 1538, 61-72.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.09.021 pmid: 24076208 |
[62] |
Maran, T., Sachse, P., Martini, M., Weber, B., Pinggera, J., Zuggal, S., & Furtner, M. (2017). Lost in time and space: States of high arousal disrupt implicit acquisition of spatial and sequential context information. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 206.
doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00206 pmid: 29170634 |
[63] |
Mather, M., Clewett, D., Sakaki, M., & Harley, C. W. (2016). Norepinephrine ignites local hotspots of neuronal excitation: How arousal amplifies selectivity in perception and memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, e200.
doi: 10.1017/S0140525X15000667 URL |
[64] |
Mather, M., & Sutherland, M. R. (2011). Arousal-biased competition in perception and memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(2), 114-133.
doi: 10.1177/1745691611400234 pmid: 21660127 |
[65] |
Mills, C., Wu, J., & D’Mello, S. (2019). Being sad is not always bad: The influence of affect on expository text comprehension. Discourse Processes, 56(2), 99-116.
doi: 10.1080/0163853X.2017.1381059 URL |
[66] |
Olson, M. A., McNulty, J. K., March, D. S., Joiner, T. E., Rogers, M. L., & Hicks, L. L. (2021). Automatic and controlled antecedents of suicidal ideation and action: A dual-process conceptualization of suicidality. Psychological Review, 129(2), 388-414.
doi: 10.1037/rev0000286 pmid: 33829836 |
[67] |
Paulhus, D. L., & Lim, D. T. (1994). Arousal and evaluative extremity in social judgments: A dynamic complexity model. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24(1), 89-99.
doi: 10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0992 URL |
[68] |
Pennycook, G., de Neys, W., Evans, J. St. B. T., Stanovich, K. E., & Thompson, V. A. (2018). The mythical dual-process typology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(8), 667-668.
doi: S1364-6613(18)30099-8 pmid: 29937320 |
[69] |
Plass, J. L., & Kalyuga, S. (2019). Four ways of considering emotion in cognitive load theory. Educational Psychology Review, 31(2), 339-359.
doi: 10.1007/s10648-019-09473-5 |
[70] |
Putkinen, V., Makkonen, T., & Eerola, T. (2017). Music-induced positive mood broadens the scope of auditory attention. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(7), 1159-1168.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx038 pmid: 28460035 |
[71] |
Remmers, C., & Zander, T. (2017). Why you don’t see the forest for the trees when you are anxious: Anxiety impairs intuitive decision making. Clinical Psychological Science, 6(1), 48-62.
doi: 10.1177/2167702617728705 URL |
[72] |
Rotgans, J. I., Schmidt, H. G., Rosby, L. V., Tan, G. J., Mamede, S., Zwaan, L., & Low-Beer, N. (2019). Evidence supporting dual-process theory of medical diagnosis: A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. Medical Education, 53(2), 143-152.
doi: 10.1111/medu.13681 pmid: 30417416 |
[73] |
Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1161-1178.
doi: 10.1037/h0077714 URL |
[74] | Salehinejad, M. A., Ghanavati, E., Rashid, M. H. A., & Nitsche, M. A. (2021). Hot and cold executive functions in the brain: A prefrontal-cingular network. Brain and Neuroscience Advances, 5, 1-19. |
[75] | Schwarz, N. (2012). Feelings-as-information theory. In P. A. M. Van Lange, A. W. Kruglanski, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (pp. 289-308). Sage Publications Ltd. |
[76] |
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2003). Mood as information: 20 years later. Psychological Inquiry, 14(3-4), 296-303.
doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1403&4_20 URL |
[77] | Shapiro, S., & MacInnis, D. J. (2002). Understanding program-induced mood effects: Decoupling arousal from valence. Journal of Advertising, 31(4), 15-26. |
[78] |
Sohn, J. H., Kim, H. E., Sohn, S., Seok, J. W., Choi, D., & Watanuki, S. (2015). Effect of emotional arousal on inter-temporal decision-making: An fMRI study. Journal of Physiological Anthropology, 34(1), 1-8.
doi: 10.1186/s40101-015-0041-y URL |
[79] |
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(5), 645-665.
doi: 10.1017/s0140525x00003435 pmid: 11301544 |
[80] | Steudner, T. (2021, June). The effects of positive feelings and arousal on privacy decision-making. Paper presented at the 23rd ITS Biennial Conference, Online Conference/ Gothenburg, Sweden. |
[81] |
Suo, T., Jia, X., Song, X., & Liu, L. (2021). The differential effects of anger and sadness on intertemporal choice: An ERP study. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 15, 638989.
doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.638989 URL |
[82] |
Sweller, J. (2010). Element interactivity and intrinsic, extraneous, and germane cognitive load. Educational Psychology Review, 22(2), 123-138.
doi: 10.1007/s10648-010-9128-5 URL |
[83] | Sweller, J., Ayres, P., & Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory. New York: Springer. |
[84] |
Tice, D. M., Bratslavsky, E., & Baumeister, R. F. (2001). Emotional distress regulation takes precedence over impulse control: If you feel bad, do it! Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(1), 53-67.
pmid: 11195891 |
[85] |
Tiedens, L. Z., & Linton, S. (2001). Judgment under emotional certainty and uncertainty: The effects of specific emotions on information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 973-988.
doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.81.6.973 pmid: 11761319 |
[86] |
van Hoof, J., Verschaffel, L., de Neys, W., & van Dooren, W. (2020). Intuitive errors in learners’ fraction understanding: A dual-process perspective on the natural number bias. Memory & Cognition, 48(7), 1171-1180.
doi: 10.3758/s13421-020-01045-1 |
[87] | Vanlessen, N., Rossi, V., de Raedt, R., & Pourtois, G. (2013). Positive emotion broadens attention focus through decreased position-specific spatial encoding in early visual cortex: Evidence from ERPs. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 13(1), 60-79. |
[88] |
Wen, T. J., Carter, J., Sar, S., Anghelcev, G., & Ham, C. D. (2021). Effects of affect: How mood and arousal influence consumer evaluation of search engine result page (SERP) Ad Snippets. Journal of Interactive Advertising, 21(3), 145-158.
doi: 10.1080/15252019.2021.1950081 URL |
[89] |
Wichary, S., Mata, R., & Rieskamp, J. (2016). Probabilistic inferences under emotional stress: How arousal affects decision processes. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 29(5), 525-538.
doi: 10.1002/bdm.v29.5 URL |
[90] |
Xie, W., Lu Sing, J. C., Martinez-Flores, A., & Zhang, W. (2022). Induced negative arousal modulates the speed of visual working memory consolidation. Emotion, 22(1), 179-197.
doi: 10.1037/emo0001059 pmid: 34990193 |
[91] | Xu, X., Wu, J. H., & Li, Q. (2020). What drives consumer shopping behavior in live streaming commerce?. Journal of Electronic Commerce Research, 21(3), 144-167. |
[92] |
Yoon, S. H., & Kim, H. W. (2019). What content and context factors lead to selection of a video clip? The heuristic route perspective. Electronic Commerce Research, 19(3), 603-627.
doi: 10.1007/s10660-019-09355-6 |
[93] |
Young, J. Q., Thakker, K., John, M., Friedman, K., Sugarman, R., van Merriënboer, J. J. G., … O'Sullivan, P. S. (2021). Exploring the relationship between emotion and cognitive load types during patient handovers. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 26(5), 1463-1489.
doi: 10.1007/s10459-021-10053-y |
[94] |
Zander-Schellenberg, T., Remmers, C., Zimmermann, J., Thommen, S., & Lieb, R. (2019). It was intuitive, and it felt good: A daily diary study on how people feel when making decisions. Cognition and Emotion. 33(7), 1505-1513.
doi: 10.1080/02699931.2019.1570914 pmid: 30688133 |
[95] |
Zhou, L., Yang, Y., & Li, S. (2022). Music-induced emotions influence intertemporal decision making. Cognition and Emotion, 36(2), 211-229.
doi: 10.1080/02699931.2021.1995331 URL |
[1] | DING Linjie, LI Xu, YIN Shufei. Positivity effects in working memory: The effects of emotional valence and task relevance [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2021, 29(4): 652-664. |
[2] | GUAN Xuxu, WANG Hongbo. Neural mechanisms of suppression-induced forgetting [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2021, 29(4): 665-676. |
[3] | LI Caiwen, ZANG Fenying, XUAN Yuming, FU Xiaolan. Estimating the time-to-collision with a threatening object [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(10): 1650-1661. |
[4] | LI Xuejuan, ZHANG Lingcong, LI Hong. The neurobiological system for the influence of emotional arousal on memory consolidation [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2017, 25(10): 1749-1757. |
[5] | LI Bin;XU Fuming;WANG Wei;ZHANG Hui;LUO Hanbing. The Origin of Loss Aversion [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2014, 22(8): 1319-1327. |
[6] | ZHONG Yi-Ping;CHEN Yun;ZHANG Shan-Ming;YANG Qing-Song. The Mechanism and Variability of Implicit Self-positivity Bias [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2012, 20(12): 1908-1919. |
[7] | WANG Xiao-Jia;FAN Chun-Lei;ZHANG Zi-Chun. Emotional Retrograde Memory Enhancement Effect [J]. , 2011, 19(4): 521-527. |
[8] | Jiang Chunping,Zhou Xiaolin. Emotional Automatic Processing and Control Processing [J]. , 2004, 12(5): 688-692. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||