Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2022, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (7): 1511-1523.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.01511
• Regular Articles • Previous Articles Next Articles
YANG Qi1, JIANG Xiaoming2(), ZHOU Xiaolin2()
Received:
2021-06-18
Online:
2022-07-15
Published:
2022-05-17
Contact:
JIANG Xiaoming,ZHOU Xiaolin
E-mail:xiaoming.jiang@shisu.edu.cn;xz104@pku.edu.cn
CLC Number:
YANG Qi, JIANG Xiaoming, ZHOU Xiaolin. Presupposition processing in language comprehension[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(7): 1511-1523.
[1] | 陈嘉映. (2003). 语言哲学. 北京大学出版社. |
[2] | 蒋晓鸣. (2020). 文化互鉴视角下非言语表情的嗓音编码和解码. 同济大学学报(社会科学版), 31(1), 116-124. |
[3] | 蒋晓鸣, 周晓林. (2013). 语用等级含义加工的脑与认知机制. 语言学研究, (2), 32-42. |
[4] | 隋雪, 史汉文, 李雨桐. (2021). 语言加工过程中的观点采择及其认知机制. 心理科学进展, 29(6), 990-999. |
[5] | 魏在江. (2014). 语用预设的认知语用研究. 上海外语教育出版社. |
[6] | 张亚旭, 蒋晓鸣, 黄永静. (2007). 言语工作记忆, 句子理解与句法依存关系加工. 心理科学进展, 15(01), 22-28. |
[7] |
Abusch, D. (2010). Presupposition triggering from alternatives. Journal of Semantics, 27(1), 37-80.
doi: 10.1093/jos/ffp009 URL |
[8] |
An, S., Bill, C., & Yang, Q. (2020). Comprehension of the presupposition trigger Ye “Also” by mandarin-speaking preschoolers with and without autism spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 570453.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.570453 URL |
[9] |
Brown-Schmidt, S., Gunlogson, C., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2008). Addressees distinguish shared from private information when interpreting questions during interactive conversation. Cognition, 107(3), 1122-1134.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.11.005 pmid: 18164700 |
[10] | Brown-Schmidt, S., & Heller, D. (2018). Perspective-taking during conversation. In S. Rueschemeyer & M. G. Gaskell (Eds.), Oxford Handbook of Psycholinguistics (2nd ed.) (pp. 551-574). Oxford University Press. |
[11] |
Burkhardt, P. (2006). Inferential bridging relations reveal distinct neural mechanisms: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Brain and Language, 98(2), 159-168.
pmid: 16725188 |
[12] | Cheung, C. C. H., Politzer-Ahles, S., Hwang, H., Chui, R. L. Y., Leung, M. T., & Tang, T. P. Y. (2017). Comprehension of presuppositions in school-age Cantonese-speaking children with and without autism spectrum disorders. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 31(7-9), 557-572. |
[13] | Cheung, C. C. H., Rong, Y., Chen, F., Leung, M. T., & Tang, T. P. Y. (2020). Comprehension of presupposition triggers in Cantonese-speaking children with and without autism spectrum disorders. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 34(4), 388-406. |
[14] | Clark, H. H., & Marshall, C.R. (1981). Definite reference and mutual knowledge. In A.H. Joshe, B. Webber, & I.A. Sag (Eds.), Elements of discourse understanding (pp. 10-63). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. |
[15] |
Clifton, C.,Jr. (2013). Situational context affects definiteness preferences: Accommodation of presuppositions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(2), 487-501.
doi: 10.1037/a0028975 URL |
[16] |
Coopmans, C. W., & Nieuwland, M. S. (2020). Dissociating activation and integration of discourse referents: Evidence from ERPs and oscillations. Cortex, 126, 83-106.
doi: S0010-9452(20)30024-1 pmid: 32065957 |
[17] |
Degen, J., Hawkins, R. D., Graf, C., Kreiss, E., & Goodman, N. D. (2020). When redundancy is useful: A Bayesian approach to “overinformative” referring expressions. Psychological Review, 127(4), 591-621.
doi: 10.1037/rev0000186 URL |
[18] |
Dietrich, S., Hertrich, I., Seibold, V. C., & Rolke, B. (2019). Discourse management during speech perception: A functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study. NeuroImage, 202, 116047.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116047 URL |
[19] | Domaneschi, F. (2016). Presuppositions and cognitive processes: Understanding the information taken for granted. Berlin, Germany: Springer. |
[20] |
Domaneschi, F., Canal, P., Masia, V., Vallauri, E. L., & Bambini, V. (2018). N400 and P600 modulation in presupposition accommodation: The effect of different trigger types. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 45, 13-35.
doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2017.08.002 URL |
[21] |
Domaneschi, F., Carrea, E., Penco, C., & Greco, A. (2014). The cognitive load of presupposition triggers: Mandatory and optional repairs in presupposition failure. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(1), 136-146.
doi: 10.1080/01690965.2013.830185 URL |
[22] |
Domaneschi, F., & Di Paola, S. (2018). The processing costs of presupposition accommodation. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 47(3), 483-503.
doi: 10.1007/s10936-017-9534-7 pmid: 29147833 |
[23] |
Domaneschi, F., & Di Paola, S. (2019). The aging factor in presupposition processing. Journal of Pragmatics, 140, 70-87.
doi: 10.1016/j.pragma.2018.11.014 |
[24] |
Feng, W., Wu, Y., Jan, C. Yu, H., Jiang, X., & Zhou, X. (2017). Effects of contextual relevance on pragmatic inference during conversation: An fMRI study. Brain and Language, 171, 52-61.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2017.04.005 URL |
[25] | Feng, W., Yu, H., & Zhou, X. (2021). Understanding particularized and generalized conversational implicatures: Is theory-of-mind necessary? Brain & Language, 212, 104878. |
[26] |
Franke, M., & Degen, J. (2016). Reasoning in reference games: Individual-vs. population-level probabilistic modeling. PloS One, 11(5), e0154854.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0154854 URL |
[27] |
Frank, M. C., & Goodman, N. D. (2012). Predicting pragmatic reasoning in language games. Science, 336(6084), 998-998.
doi: 10.1126/science.1218633 URL |
[28] |
Galati, A., & Brennan, S. E. (2021). What is retained about common ground? Distinct effects of linguistic and visual co-presence. Cognition, 215, 104809.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104809 URL |
[29] | Glanzberg, M. (2005). Presuppositions, truth values, and expressing propositions. In G. Preyer & G. Peters (Eds.), Contextualism in philosophy: Knowledge, meaning, and truth (pp. 349-396). Oxford University Press |
[30] |
Holler, J., & Levinson, S. C. (2019). Multimodal language processing in human communication. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(8), 639-652.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.05.006 URL |
[31] |
Jacoby, N., & Fedorenko, E. (2020). Discourse-level comprehension engages medial frontal Theory of Mind brain regions even for expository texts. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 35(6), 780-796.
doi: 10.1080/23273798.2018.1525494 pmid: 32984430 |
[32] |
Jiang, X., Li, Y., & Zhou, X. (2013). Even a rich man can afford that expensive house: ERP responses to construction-based pragmatic constraints during sentence comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 51(10), 1857-1866.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.06.009 URL |
[33] | Jiang, X., Tan, Y., & Zhou, X. (2009). Processing the universal quantifier during sentence comprehension: ERP evidence. Neuropsychologia, 47(8-9), 1799-1815. |
[34] |
Jiang, X., & Zhou, X. (2020). An alternative structure rescues failed semantics? Strong global expectancy reduces local-mismatch N400 in Chinese flexible structures. Neuropsychologia, 140, 107380.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107380 URL |
[35] |
Jouravlev, O., Stearns, L., Bergen, L., Eddy, M., Gibson, E., & Fedorenko, E. (2016). Processing temporal presuppositions: An event-related potential study. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(10), 1245-1256.
doi: 10.1080/23273798.2016.1209531 URL |
[36] |
Keysar, B., Barr, D. J., Balin, J. A., & Brauner, J. S. (2000). Taking perspective in conversation: The role of mutual knowledge in comprehension. Psychological Science, 11(1), 32-38.
pmid: 11228840 |
[37] |
Kirsten, M., Tiemann, S., Seibold, V. C., Hertrich, I., Beck, S., & Rolke, B. (2014). When the polar bear encounters many polar bears: Event-related potential context effects evoked by uniqueness failure. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(9), 1147-1162.
doi: 10.1080/23273798.2014.899378 URL |
[38] |
Li, S., Jiang, X., Yu, H., & Zhou, X. (2014). Cognitive empathy modulates the processing of pragmatic constraints during sentence comprehension. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(8), 1166-1174.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nst091 URL |
[39] |
Masia, V., Canal, P., Ricci, I., Vallauri, E. L., & Bambini, V. (2017). Presupposition of new information as a pragmatic garden path: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 42, 31-48.
doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2016.11.005 URL |
[40] |
Mi, Q., Wang, C., Camerer, C. F., & Zhu, L. (2021). Reading between the lines: Listener’s vmPFC simulates speaker cooperative choices in communication games. Science Advances, 7(10), eabe6276.
doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abe6276 URL |
[41] | Mitchell, D. C. (2004). On-line methods in language processing:Introduction and historical review. In M. Carreiras, & C. J. Clifton (Eds.), The on-line study of sentence comprehension: Eyetracking, ERPs, and beyond (pp. 15-32). Brighton, UK: Psychology Press. |
[42] |
Murphy, G. L. (1984). Establishing and accessing referents in discourse. Memory & Cognition, 12(5), 489-497.
doi: 10.3758/BF03198311 URL |
[43] |
Nieuwland, M. S., & Martin, A. E. (2017). Neural oscillations and a nascent corticohippocampal theory of reference. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 29(5), 896-910.
doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01091 pmid: 28129065 |
[44] |
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2007). Do people use language production to make predictions during comprehension? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(3), 105-110.
pmid: 17254833 |
[45] |
Rapp, A. M., Mutschler, D. E., & Erb, M. (2012). Where in the brain is nonliteral language? A coordinate-based meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Neuroimage, 63(1), 600-610.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.022 URL |
[46] |
Reyes-Aguilar, A., Valles-Capetillo, E., & Giordano, M. (2018). A quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of pragmatic language comprehension: In search of a universal neural substrate. Neuroscience, 395, 60-88.
doi: S0306-4522(18)30720-6 pmid: 30414881 |
[47] |
Schneider, C., Bade, N., Franke, M., & Janczyk, M. (2021). Presuppositions of determiners are immediately used to disambiguate utterance meaning: A mouse-tracking study on the German language. Psychological Research, 85, 1348-1366.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-020-01302-7 URL |
[48] |
Schneider, C., Bade, N., & Janczyk, M. (2020). Is immediate processing of presupposition triggers automatic or capacity-limited? A combination of the PRP approach with a self-paced reading task. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 49, 247-273.
doi: 10.1007/s10936-019-09686-3 pmid: 32030581 |
[49] |
Schneider, C., & Janczyk, M. (2020). Capacity limitations of processing presuppositions triggered by determiners. Acta Psychologica, 211, 103159.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103159 URL |
[50] |
Schneider, C., Schonard, C., Franke, M., Jäger, G., & Janczyk, M. (2019). Pragmatic processing: An investigation of the (anti-) presuppositions of determiners using mouse-tracking. Cognition, 193, 104024.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104024 URL |
[51] | Schwarz, F. (2014). Presuppositions are fast, whether hard or soft-evidence from the visual world. In Snider, T., D’Antonio, S., & Wiegand, M. (Eds.), Semantics and linguistic theory (Vol. 24, pp. 1-22). LSA and CLC Publications. |
[52] |
Schwarz, F. (2016). Experimental work in presupposition and presupposition projection. Annual Review of Linguistics, 2, 273-292.
doi: 10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011415-040809 URL |
[53] |
Shetreet, E., Alexander, E. J., Romoli, J., Chierchia, G., & Kuperberg, G. (2019). What we know about knowing: Presuppositions generated by factive verbs influence downstream neural processing. Cognition, 184, 96-106.
doi: S0010-0277(18)30307-X pmid: 30584974 |
[54] | Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1986). Relevance: Communication and cognition (Vol. 142). Massachusetts, USA: Harvard University Press. |
[55] |
Stalnaker, R. (2002). Common ground. Linguistics and Philosophy, 25(5/6), 701-721.
doi: 10.1023/A:1020867916902 URL |
[56] |
Stillman, P. E., Shen, X., & Ferguson, M. J. (2018). How mouse-tracking can advance social cognitive theory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(6), 531-543.
doi: S1364-6613(18)30073-1 pmid: 29731415 |
[57] | Tiemann, S., Schmid, M., Bade, N., Rolke, B., Hertrich, I., Ackermann, H.,... Beck, S. (2011). Psycholinguistic evidence for presuppositions:On-line and off-line data. In R. Ingo et al. (Eds.), Proceedings of Sinn und Bedeutung (Vol. 15, pp. 581-596). Saarbrucken, Gemmany: Universaar-Saarland University Press. |
[58] |
van Moort, M. L., Jolles, D. D., Koornneef, A., & van den Broek, P. (2020). What you read versus what you know: Neural correlates of accessing context information and background knowledge in constructing a mental representation during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(11), 2084-2101.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000764 URL |
[59] | van Moort, M. L., Koornneef, A., & van den Broek, P. W. (2018). Validation: Knowledge-and text-based monitoring during reading. Discourse Processes, 55(5-6), 480-496. |
[60] |
van Moort, M. L., Koornneef, A., & van den Broek, P. W. (2021). Differentiating text-based and knowledge-based validation processes during reading: Evidence from eye movements. Discourse Processes, 58(1), 22-41.
doi: 10.1080/0163853X.2020.1727683 URL |
[61] | Wang, L., & Schumacher, P. B. (2013). New is not always costly: Evidence from online processing of topic and contrast in Japanese. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 363. |
[62] |
Willems, R. M., de Boer, M., de Ruiter, J. P., Noordzij, M. L., Hagoort, P., & Toni, I. (2010). A dissociation between linguistic and communicative abilities in the human brain. Psychological Science, 21(1), 8-14.
doi: 10.1177/0956797609355563 pmid: 20424015 |
[63] |
Yang, X., Zhang, X., Yang, Y., & Lin, N. (2018). How context features modulate the involvement of the working memory system during discourse comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 111, 36-44.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.01.010 URL |
[64] |
Yang, X., Zhang, X., Zhang, Y., Zhang, Q., & Li, X. (2020). How working memory capacity modulates the time course of semantic integration at sentence and discourse level. Neuropsychologia, 140, 107383.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107383 URL |
[65] |
Ye, Z. & Zhou, X. (2009). Conflict control during sentence comprehension: fMRI evidence. Neuroimage, 48(1), 280-290.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.032 URL |
[66] |
Zang, C., Zhang, L., Zhang, M., Bai, X., Yan, G., Jiang, X.,... Zhou, X. (2019). Eye movements reveal delayed use of construction-based pragmatic information during online sentence reading: A case of Chinese Lian… dou Construction. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2211.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02211 URL |
[67] |
Zhang, X., Zhang, Y., Zhang, Z., Yang, X., & Yang, Y. (2021). How working memory capacity modulates the time course of indirect replies comprehension: An event-related potential study. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 36(10), 1246-1257.
doi: 10.1080/23273798.2021.1924386 URL |
[1] | LI Bin1,2; MA Hongyu1; LI Aimei2; LING Wenquan2. The Trigger, Research Paradigm and Measurement of Nostalgic [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2015, 23(7): 1289-1298. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||