Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2021, Vol. 29 ›› Issue (11): 2062-2072.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2021.02062
• Regular Articles • Previous Articles Next Articles
XIONG Guanxing1, YE Jinming1, SUN Hailong2()
Received:
2021-03-12
Online:
2021-11-15
Published:
2021-09-23
Contact:
SUN Hailong
E-mail:sunhailong@gdufs.edu.cn
CLC Number:
XIONG Guanxing, YE Jinming, SUN Hailong. The trend effect of probability estimation and its influence on decision-making from the perspective of psychological momentum[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2021, 29(11): 2062-2072.
[1] | 杜雪蕾, 许洁虹, 苏寅, 李纾. (2012). 用文字概率衡量不确定性: 特征和问题. 心理科学进展, 20(5), 651-661. |
[2] | 孙庆洲, 邬青渊, 张静, 江程铭, 赵雷, 胡凤培. (2019). 风险决策的概率权重偏差: 心理机制与优化策略. 心理科学进展, 27(5), 905-913. |
[3] | 谭成慧, 马姗姗, 朱传林, 赵源, 王炫懿, 疏德明, 刘电芝. (2020). 人际互动中的“自我-他人”区分: 多水平的探索. 心理科学进展, 28(11), 1890-1900. |
[4] | 谭飞, 李爱梅, 孙海龙, 侯芬. (2016). 聚焦“缺失”对当下幸福体验的影响. 心理科学进展, 24(10), 1544-1550. |
[5] | 王晓田. (2019). 如何用行为经济学应对不确定性: 拓展有效助推的范围. 心理学报, 51(04), 407-414. |
[6] | 熊冠星. (2020). 克服“束手无策”:化解不确定性的决策之道. 武汉大学出版社. |
[7] | 张素愚, 王修欣, 杜秀芳. (2018). 任务框架和序列趋势对趋势阻尼的影响. 心理科学, 41(1), 31-37. |
[8] |
Attali, Y. (2013). Perceived hotness affects behavior of basketball players and coaches. Psychological Science, 24(7), 1151-1156.
doi: 10.1177/0956797612468452 pmid: 23630221 |
[9] |
Brenner, L. A., Griffin, D. W., & Koehler, D. J. (2012). A case-based model of probability and pricing judgments: Biases in buying and selling uncertainty. Management Science, 58(1), 159-178.
doi: 10.1287/mnsc.1110.1429 URL |
[10] | Briki, W., & Markman, K. D. (2018). Psychological momentum: The phenomenology of goal pursuit. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 12(9), 1-14. |
[11] |
Briki, W., Markman, K. D., Coudevylle, G. R., Sinnapah, S., & Hue, O. (2015). Momentum sequence and environmental climate influence levels of perceived PM within a sport competition. European Journal of Sport Science, 16(3), 350-357.
doi: 10.1080/17461391.2015.1062566 URL |
[12] |
Briki, W., & Zoudji, B. (2019). Gaining or losing team ball possession: The dynamics of momentum perception and strategic choice in football coaches. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1019.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01019 URL |
[13] |
Caruso, E. M., van Boven, L., Chin, M., & Ward, A. (2013). The temporal Doppler Effect: When the future feels closer than the past. Psychological Science, 24(4), 530-536.
doi: 10.1177/0956797612458804 URL |
[14] | Collins, R. N., & Mandel, D. R. (2019). Cultivating credibility with probability words and numbers. Judgment and Decision Making, 14(6), 683-695. |
[15] |
Erlandsson, A., Hohle, S. M., Løhre, E., & Västfjäll, D. (2018). The rise and fall of scary numbers: The effect of perceived trends on future estimates, severity ratings, and help-allocations in a cancer context. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 48(11), 618-633.
doi: 10.1111/jasp.2018.48.issue-11 URL |
[16] |
Ersner-Hershfield, H., Galinsky, A. D., Kray, L. J., & King, B. G. (2010). Company, country, connections: Counterfactual origins increase organizational commitment, patriotism, and social investment. Psychological Science, 21(10), 1479-1486.
doi: 10.1177/0956797610382123 pmid: 20817783 |
[17] |
Fehr-Duda, H., & Epper, T. (2012). Probability and risk: Foundations and economic implications of probability- dependent risk preferences. Annual Review of Economics, 4(1), 567-593.
doi: 10.1146/economics.2012.4.issue-1 URL |
[18] |
Ferson, S., O'Rawe, J., Antonenko, A., Siegrist, J., Mickley, J., Luhmann, C. C., ... Finkel, A. M. (2015). Natural language of uncertainty: Numeric hedge words. International Journal of Approximate Reasoning, 57, 19-39.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijar.2014.11.003 URL |
[19] |
Fessel, F., & Roese, N. J. (2011). Hindsight bias, visual aids, and legal decision making: Timing is everything. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 5(4), 180-193
doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00343.x URL |
[20] |
Gallistel, C. R., Krishan, M., Liu, Y., Miller, R., & Latham, P. E. (2014). The perception of probability. Psychological Review, 121(1), 96-123.
doi: 10.1037/a0035232 pmid: 24490790 |
[21] |
Guenther, C. L., & Kokotajlo, C. (2017). Psychological momentum and risky decision-making. Journal of Theoretical Social Psychology, 1(2), 43-51.
doi: 10.1002/jts5.2017.1.issue-2 URL |
[22] | Hohle, S. M., & Teigen, K. H. (2015). Forecasting forecasts: The trend effect. Judgment and Decision Making, 10(5), 416-428. |
[23] |
Hohle, S. M., & Teigen, K. H. (2018). More than 50% or less than 70% chance: Pragmatic implications of single-bound probability estimates. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 31(1), 138-150.
doi: 10.1002/bdm.v31.1 URL |
[24] |
Hohle, S. M., & Teigen, K. H. (2019). When probabilities change: Perceptions and implications of trends in uncertain climate forecasts. Journal of Risk Research, 22(5), 555-569.
doi: 10.1080/13669877.2018.1459801 URL |
[25] |
Howe, L. C., MacInnis, B., Krosnick, J. A., Markowitz, E. M., & Socolow, R. (2019). Acknowledging uncertainty impacts public acceptance of climate scientists’ predictions. Nature Climate Change, 9(11), 863-867.
doi: 10.1038/s41558-019-0587-5 URL |
[26] |
Hubbard, T. L. (2015). The varieties of momentum-like experience. Psychological Bulletin, 141(6), 1081-1119.
doi: 10.1037/bul0000016 URL |
[27] |
Hubbard, T. L. (2017a). Toward a general theory of momentum-like effects. Behavioural Processes, 141(8), 50-66.
doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.02.019 URL |
[28] |
Hubbard, T. L. (2017b). Momentum in music: Musical succession as physical motion. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 27(1), 14-30.
doi: 10.1037/pmu0000171 URL |
[29] |
Hubbard, T. L. (2019). Momentum-like effects and the dynamics of perception, cognition, and action. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 81, 2155-2170
doi: 10.3758/s13414-019-01770-z URL |
[30] |
Hubbard, T. L. (2020). Representational gravity: Empirical findings and theoretical implications. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27(1), 36-55.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-019-01660-3 URL |
[31] |
Isaac, M. S., & Brough, A. R. (2014). Judging a part by the size of its whole: The category size bias in probability judgments. Journal of Consumer Research, 41(2), 310-325.
doi: 10.1086/676126 URL |
[32] |
Iso‐Ahola, S. E., & Dotson, C. O. (2014). Psychological momentum: Why success breeds success. Review of General Psychology, 18(1), 19-33.
doi: 10.1037/a0036406 URL |
[33] | Jordan, J. S., Cialdella, V., Schloesser, D. S., & Bai, J. (2018). Forms of bias in cognitive science:Moving beyond perception, action, and cognition. In T. L. Hubbard (Ed.). Spatial biases in perception and cognition (pp.350-365). New York: Cambridge University Press |
[34] |
Kappes, H. B., & Morewedge, C. K. (2016). Mental simulation as substitute for experience. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10(7), 405-420.
doi: 10.1111/spc3.v10.7 URL |
[35] |
Kause, A., Townsend, T., & Gaissmaier, W. (2019). Framing climate uncertainty: Frame choices reveal and influence climate change beliefs. Weather, Climate, and Society, 11(1), 199-215.
doi: 10.1175/WCAS-D-18-0002.1 URL |
[36] |
Lewandowsky, S. (2011). Popular consensus: Climate change is set to continue. Psychological Science, 22(4), 460-463.
doi: 10.1177/0956797611402515 pmid: 21415241 |
[37] |
Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological bulletin, 127(2), 267-286.
pmid: 11316014 |
[38] |
Løhre, E. (2018). Stronger, sooner, and more certain climate change: A link between certainty and outcome strength in revised forecasts. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(12), 2531-2547.
doi: 10.1177/1747021817746062 URL |
[39] |
Løhre, E., Sobkow, A., Hohle, S. M., & Teigen, K. H. (2019). Framing experts' (dis) agreements about uncertain environmental events. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 32(5), 564-578.
doi: 10.1002/bdm.v32.5 URL |
[40] |
Løhre, E., & Teigen, K. H. (2017). Probabilities associated with precise and vague forecasts. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 30(5), 1014-1026.
doi: 10.1002/bdm.v30.5 URL |
[41] |
Maglio, S. J., & Polman, E. (2014). Spatial orientation shrinks and expands psychological distance. Psychological Science, 25(7), 1345-1352.
doi: 10.1177/0956797614530571 pmid: 24815609 |
[42] |
Maglio, S. J., & Polman, E. (2016). Revising probability estimates: Why increasing likelihood means increasing impact. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111(2), 141-158.
doi: 10.1037/pspa0000058 pmid: 27281350 |
[43] |
Markman, K. D., & Guenther, C. L. (2007). Psychological momentum: Intuitive physics and naive beliefs. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(6), 800-812.
pmid: 17488872 |
[44] | Nevin, J. A. (2015). Behavioral Momentum: A Scientific Metaphor. CreateSpaceIndependent Publishing, San Bernadino. |
[45] |
Pettit, N. C., Sivanathan, N., Gladstone, E., & Carson Marr, J. (2013). Rising stars and sinking ships: Consequences of status momentum. Psychological Science, 24(8), 1579-1584.
doi: 10.1177/0956797612473120 URL |
[46] |
Regenwetter, M., & Davis‐Stober, C. P. (2018). The role of independence and stationarity in probabilistic models of binary choice. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 31(1), 100-114.
doi: 10.1002/bdm.2037 pmid: 29805199 |
[47] |
Reh, S., Tröster, C., & van Quaquebeke, N. (2018). Keeping (future) rivals down: Temporal social comparison predicts coworker social undermining via future status threat and envy. Journal of Applied Psychology, 103(4), 399-415.
doi: 10.1037/apl0000281 URL |
[48] | Roese, N. J., & Epstude, K. (2017). The functional theory of counterfactual thinking:New evidence, new Challenges, new insights. In J. M. Olson (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 56). (pp. 1-79). Academic Press Inc. |
[49] |
Shen, L., & Hsee, C. K. (2017). Numerical nudging: Using an accelerating score to enhance performance. Psychological Science, 28(8), 1077-1086.
doi: 10.1177/0956797617700497 URL |
[50] |
Suter, R. S., Pachur, T., & Hertwig, R. (2016). How affect shapes risky choice: Distorted probability weighting versus probability neglect. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 29(4), 437-449.
doi: 10.1002/bdm.v29.4 URL |
[51] |
Teigen, K. H., Halberg, A. M., & Fostervold, K. I. (2007). Single limit interval estimates as reference points. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21(3), 383-406.
doi: 10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0720 URL |
[52] | Teigen, K. H., Løhre, E., & Hohle, S. M. (2018). The boundary effect: Perceived post hoc accuracy of prediction intervals. Judgment and Decision Making, 13(4), 309-321. |
[53] | Tetlock, P. E., & Gardner, D. (2015). Superforecasting: The art and science of prediction. Random House. |
[54] |
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1992). Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 5(4), 297-323.
doi: 10.1007/BF00122574 URL |
[55] | Witt, J. K. (2018). Spatial biases from action. In T. L. Hubbard (Ed.). Spatial biases in perception and cognition (pp.307-323). New York: Cambridge University Press. |
[1] | ZAHNG Yuyan, XU Liying, YU Feng, DING Xiaojun, WU Jiahua, ZHAO Liang. A three-dimensional motivation model of algorithm aversion [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(5): 1093-1105. |
[2] | LUAN Mo, WU Shuang. How does decision making process signal social status? A maximizing decision making perspective [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(10): 2194-2205. |
[3] | REN Xiaoyun, DUAN Jinyun, FENG Chengzhi. Individuals vs groups: Advice-taking in decision making [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2021, 29(3): 549-559. |
[4] | YANG Ling, LIU Wenxin, ZHANG Yang, ZHANG Jianxun, NIU Lulin. The external validity of delay discounting in the field of substance addiction [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2021, 29(1): 140-149. |
[5] | XU Fu-ming, HUANG Long, ZHANG Hui, XIANG Peng, LIU Teng-fei, LI Ya-hong. Psychological mechanisms and management strategies of behavioral poverty trap: Based on the dual perspectives of cognition and motivation [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(5): 681-691. |
[6] | HE Qinghua, LI Dandan. Brain maturation and improvement in intertemporal choice in Chinese children and adolescents [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(3): 381-389. |
[7] | PENG Jiaxi, ZHAO Lumimg, FANG Peng, CAO Yunfei, MIAO Danmin, XIAO Wei. The effect mechanism of sleep deprivation on risky decision making [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(11): 1789-1799. |
[8] | MA Yu, XIAO Chenjie, CHE Jingshang, WANG Haixia, LI Aimei. Ego depletion impedes rational decision making: Mechanisms and boundary conditions [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(11): 1911-1925. |
[9] | WU Xiao-Ju, CHEN Jun-Fang, FU Jia-Hui, LI Shu, LIANG Zhu-Yuan. Time preferences for public health and health behavior [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2020, 28(11): 1926-1938. |
[10] | Yiling Zhou, Mowei Shen, Hui Chen. Affecting Postdictive Effect of Choice by Altering Attention [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(suppl.): 37-37. |
[11] | LIAO Jiajun, LI Hong, WU Yin. Testosterone and human decision-making [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(9): 1607-1621. |
[12] | Jingyi LU, Xuesong SHANG. Making decisions for others: Multi-dimensional psychological mechanisms and decision feelings [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(9): 1545-1552. |
[13] | XIN Xiaoyang, XU Chenhong, CHEN Hongyu, LI Ying. Quantum models for decision making [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(8): 1365-1373. |
[14] | ZHANG Qian, CHEN Linlin, YANG Qun. The effects of facial features on judicial decision making [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(4): 698-709. |
[15] | LIU Yunzhi, YANG Ziyan, WANG Yuqi, CHEN Jun, CAI Huajian. The concept of future self-continuity and its effects [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(12): 2161-2169. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||