Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2019, Vol. 27 ›› Issue (4): 700-710.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2019.00700
• Regular Articles • Previous Articles Next Articles
WANG Yue1, GONG Yuanchao1,2,3, LI Ying1()
Received:
2018-06-05
Online:
2019-04-15
Published:
2019-02-22
Contact:
LI Ying
E-mail:liying@zzu.edu.cn
CLC Number:
WANG Yue, GONG Yuanchao, LI Ying. The mental mechanism of immoral contagion and its effects[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(4): 700-710.
[1] | 黄玺, 梁宏宇, 李放, 陈世民, 王巍欣, 林妙莲, 郑雪 . ( 2018). 道德提升感: 一种提升道德情操的积极道德情绪. 心理科学进展, 26, (7), 1253-1263. |
[2] | 黄好, 罗禹, 冯廷勇, 李红 . ( 2010). 厌恶加工的神经基础. 心理科学进展, 18, (9), 1449-1457. |
[3] | 李鸿展 . ( 2014). 近墨者黑: 不道德传染对道德判断的影响 (硕士学位论文). 广西师范大学. |
[4] |
李琼, 刘力 . ( 2011). 低地位群体的外群体偏好. 心理科学进展, 19, (7), 1061-1068.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2011.01061 URL |
[5] | 廖敏 . ( 2012). 道家文化的当代心理学价值及其实现. 社会心理科学,( 9), 107-114. |
[6] |
王晓辰, 高欣洁, 郭攀博 . ( 2018). 亲组织不道德行为的多层次模型. 心理科学进展, 26, (6), 1111-1120.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.01111 URL |
[7] | 吴宝沛, 张雷 . ( 2012). 厌恶与道德判断的关系. 心理科学进展, 20, (2), 309-316. |
[8] | 夏天生, 徐雪梅, 李贺, 俞梦霞, 莫雷 . ( 2018). 洗掉不愉快的经历, 留住幸福的时刻——回忆情绪事件对人们清洗/保留倾向的影响. 心理科学, 41, (1), 105-111. |
[9] | 徐细雄, 刘星 . ( 2013). 放权改革、薪酬管制与企业高管腐败. 管理世界, 3, 119-132. |
[10] | 阎书昌 . ( 2011). 身体洁净与道德. 心理科学进展, 19, (8), 1242-1248. |
[11] |
叶浩生 . ( 2011). 有关具身认知思潮的理论心理学思考. 心理学报, 43, (5), 589-598.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2011.00589 URL |
[12] |
叶红燕, 张凤华 . ( 2015). 从具身视角看道德判断. 心理科学进展, 23, (8), 1480-1488.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2015.01480 URL |
[13] |
Albas D., &Albas C. ( 1989). Modern magic: The case of examinations. The Sociological Quarterly, 30, (4), 603-613.
doi: 10.1111/j.1533-8525.1989.tb01537.x URL |
[14] |
Bem D.J . ( 1972). Constructing cross-situational consistencies in behavior: Some thoughts on Alker's critique of Mischel. Journal of Personality, 40, (1), 17-26.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1972.tb00645.x URL pmid: 5013145 |
[15] |
Caldwell B. M., Harenski C. L., Harenski K. A., Fede S. J., Steele V. R., Koenigs M. R., & Kiehl K. A . ( 2015). Abnormal frontostriatal activity in recently abstinent cocaine users during implicit moral processing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 565.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00565 URL pmid: 4608360 |
[16] | Carlston D .( 2010). Models of implicit and explicit mental representation. In B. Gawronski & K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory and applications. (pp. 38-61). Guilford Press. |
[17] | Chapman H. A., Kim D. A., Susskind J. M., & Anderson A. K . ( 2009). In bad taste: Evidence for the oral origins of moral disgust. Science, 323, (5918), 1222-1226. |
[18] |
Choi I., Nisbett R. E., & Norenzayan A . ( 1998). Causal attribution across cultures: Variation and universality. Psychological Bulletin, 125, (1), 47-63.
doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.125.1.47 URL |
[19] |
Danovitch J., &Bloom P. ( 2009). Children's extension of disgust to physical and moral events. Emotion, 9, (1), 107-112.
doi: 10.1037/a0014113 URL pmid: 19186922 |
[20] | Diener E., &Seligman M. E.P . ( 2004). Beyond money toward an economy of well-being. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5, (1), 1-31. |
[21] | Ferguson M. A., &ranscombe B, N. R .( 2014) . The social psychology of collective guilt. In C. von Scheve, & M. Salmela (Eds.), Collective emotions: Perspectives from psychology, philosophy, and sociology (pp. 251-265) . Oxford, UK: Oxford Uni-versity Press. |
[22] |
Fowler J.H., &Christakis N.A . ( 2009). Dynamic spread of happiness in a large social network: Longitudinal analysis of the Framingham heart study social network. BMJ: British Medical Journal, 338, (7685), 23-27.
doi: 10.1136/bmj.a2338 URL |
[23] |
Eskine K. J., Kacinik N. A., & Webster G. D . ( 2012). The bitter truth about morality: Virtue, not vice, makes a bland beverage taste nice. Plos One, 7, (7), e41159.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0041159 URL pmid: 3399822 |
[24] |
Eskine K. J., Novreske A., & Richards M . ( 2013). Moral contagion effects in everyday interpersonal encounters. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, (5), 947-950.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.04.009 URL |
[25] |
Gámez E., Díaz J. M., & Marreo H . ( 2011). The uncertain universality of the Macbeth effect with a Spanish sample. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 14, (1), 156-162.
doi: 10.5209/rev_SJOP.2011.v14.n1.13 URL pmid: 21568173 |
[26] | Gausel N. ( 2012). Facing in-group immorality: Differentiating expressed shame from expressed guilt. Review of European Studies, 4, (4), 1-7. |
[27] |
Giner-Sorolla R., Kupfer T., & Sabo J . ( 2018). What makes moral disgust special? An integrative functional review. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 57, 223-289.
doi: 10.1016/bs.aesp.2017.10.001 URL |
[28] |
Gino F., Gu J., & Zhong C-B . ( 2009). Contagion or restitution? When bad apples can motivate ethical behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, (6), 1299-1302.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.07.014 URL |
[29] | Graham J., Haidt J., Koleva S., Motyl M., Iyer R., Wojcik S. P., & Ditto P. H.. ( 2013). Moral foundations theory: The pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. In P. Devine & A. Plant (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 47, pp. 55-130). Burlington, MA: Academic Press. |
[30] |
Grappi S., Romani S., & Bagozzi R. P . ( 2013). Consumer response to corporate irresponsible behavior: Moral emotions and virtues. Journal of Business Research, 66, (10), 1814-1821.
doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2013.02.002 URL |
[31] |
Greene J., &Haidt A.J . ( 2002). How (and where) does moral judgment work?. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, (12), 517-523.
doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)02011-9 URL pmid: 12475712 |
[32] |
Greene J. D., Morelli S. A., Lowenberg K., Nystrom L. E., & Cohen J. D . ( 2008). Cognitive load selectively interferes with utilitarian moral judgment. Cognition, 107, (3), 1144-1154.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.11.004 URL pmid: 18158145 |
[33] |
Haidt J., Rozin P., Mccauley C., & Imada S . ( 1997). Body, psyche, and culture: the relationship between disgust and morality. Psychology and Developing Societies, 9, (1), 107-131.
doi: 10.1177/097133369700900105 URL |
[34] |
Hanna M.B., &Marcus H. ( 2016). Individual differences in moral development: Does intelligence really affect children’s moral reasoning and moral emotions?. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1961.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01961 URL pmid: 28066287 |
[35] |
Hardy S.A., &Carlo G. ( 2011). Moral identity: What is it, how does it develop, and is it linked to moral action?. Child Development Perspectives, 5, (3), 212-218.
doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00189.x URL |
[36] |
Harenski C.L., &Hamann S. ( 2006). Neural correlates of regulating negative emotions related to moral violations. Neuroimage, 30, (1), 313-324.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.034 URL pmid: 16249098 |
[37] |
Hejmadi A., Rozin P., & Siegal M . ( 2004). Once in contact, always in contact: Contagious essence and conceptions of purification in American and Hindu Indian children. Developmental Psychology, 40, (4), 467-476.
doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.4.467 URL pmid: 15238036 |
[38] |
Helzer E.G., &Pizarro D.A . ( 2011). Dirty liberals!: Reminders of physical cleanliness influence moral and political attitudes. Psychological Science, 22, (4), 517-522.
doi: 10.1177/0956797611402514 URL pmid: 21421934 |
[39] |
Ho Y-H., &Lin C-Y. ( 2014). The moral judgment relationship between leaders and followers: A comparative study across the Taiwan strait. Journal of Business Ethics, 134, (2), 299-310.
doi: 10.1007/s10551-014-2431-1 URL |
[40] |
Hood B. M., Gjersoe N. L., Donnelly K., Byers A., & Itajkura S . ( 2011). Moral contagion attitudes towards potential organ transplants in British and Japanese adults. Journal of Cognition & Culture, 11, (3/4), 269-286.
doi: 10.1163/156853711X591251 URL |
[41] | Inbar Y., &Pizarro D. ( 2013). Pollution and purity in moral and political judgment. Advances in Experimental Moral Psychology, 111-129. |
[42] |
Inbar Y., Pizarro D. A., Knobe J., & Bloom P . ( 2009). Disgust sensitivity predicts intuitive disapproval of gays. Emotion, 9, (3), 435-439.
doi: 10.1037/a0015960 URL pmid: 19485621 |
[43] |
Jong sung L., Dae hee K., & Braunstein-minkove J . ( 2016). Coping with athlete endorsers' immoral behavior: Roles of athlete identification and moral emotions on moral reasoning strategies. Journal of Sport Management, 30, (2), 176-191.
doi: 10.1123/jsm.2015-0341 URL |
[44] |
Koleva S. P., Graham J., Iyer R., Ditto P. H., & Haidt J . ( 2012). Tracing the threads: How five moral concerns (especially purity) help explain culture war attitudes. Journal of Research in Personality, 46, (2), 184-194.
doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2012.01.006 URL |
[45] |
Lakens D .( 2014). Grounding social embodiment. Social Cognition, 32, 168-183.
doi: 10.1521/soco.2014.32.supp.168 URL |
[46] |
Lee A., &Ji L.J . ( 2014). Moving away from a bad past and toward a good future: Feelings influence the metaphorical understanding of time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, (1), 21-26.
doi: 10.1037/a0032233 URL pmid: 23506086 |
[47] | Lee S. W.S., &Schwarz N. ( 2010). Washing away postdecisional dissonance. Science, 328, (5979), 709-709. |
[48] | Leffel G.M . ( 2008). Who cares? Generativity and the moral emotions, part 2: A "social intuitionist model" of moral motivation. Journal of Psychology & Theology, 36, (3), 182-201. |
[49] |
Legare C. H., Wellman H. M., & Gelman S. A . ( 2009). Evidence for an explanation advantage in naïve biological reasoning. Cognitive Psychology, 58, (2), 177-194.
doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2008.06.002 URL pmid: 18710700 |
[50] |
Leutgeb V., Wabnegger A., Leitner M., Zussner T., Scharmüller W., Klug D., & Schienle A . ( 2016). Altered cerebellar-amygdala connectivity in violent offenders: A resting-state fMRI study. Neuroscience Letters, 610, 160-164.
doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2015.10.063 URL pmid: 26523791 |
[51] |
Levav J., &McGraw A.P . ( 2009). Emotional accounting: How feelings about money influence consumer choice. Journal of Marketing Research, 46, (1), 66-80.
doi: 10.1509/jmkr.46.1.66 URL |
[52] | Marczyk J. .( 2015). Moral alliance strategies theory. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 1, (2), 77-90. |
[53] |
Martin J. W., &Cushman F .(in press). The adaptive logic of moral luck. A Companion to Experimental Philosophy.
doi: 10.1002/9781118661666.ch12 URL |
[54] | Mauss M. ( 1972). A general theory of magic (R. Brain, Trans.). New York: W. W. Norton. ( Original work published 1902) |
[55] |
Mcdermott E., &Langdon P.E . ( 2016). The moral reasoning abilities of men and women with intellectual disabilities who have a history of criminal offending behaviour. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 21, (1), 25-40.
doi: 10.1111/lcrp.12051 URL |
[56] |
Moll J., de Oliveira-Souza R., Moll F. T., Ignácio F. A., Bramati I. E., Caparelli-Dáquer E. M., & Eslinqer P. J . ( 2005). The moral affiliations of disgust: A functional MRI study. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 18, (1), 68-78.
doi: 10.1097/01.wnn.0000152236.46475.a7 URL pmid: 15761278 |
[57] | Nemeroff C., & Rozin P. ( 2000) . The makings of the magical mind: The nature and function of sympathetic magical thinking In K S Rosengren, C N Johnson, & P L Harris (Eds), Imagining the impossible: Magical, scientific, and religious thinking in children (pp 1-34) New York: Cambridge University Press . |
[58] |
Nemeroff C., &Rozin P. ( 1994). The contagion concept in adult thinking in the United States: Transmission of germs and of interpersonal influence. Ethos, 22, (2), 158-186.
doi: 10.1525/eth.1994.22.2.02a00020 URL |
[59] |
Newman G.E., &Bloom P. ( 2014). Physical contact influences how much people pay at celebrity auctions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111, (10), 3705-3708.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1313637111 URL pmid: 24567388 |
[60] |
Olatunji B. O., Cisler J., McKay D., & Phillips M. L . ( 2010). Is disgust associated with psychopathology? Emerging research in the anxiety disorders. Psychiatry Research, 175, (1-2), 1-10.
doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2009.04.007 URL pmid: 19969378 |
[61] |
Paxton J.M., &Greene J.D . ( 2010). Moral reasoning: Hints and allegations. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2, (3), 511-527.
doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01096.x URL pmid: 25163874 |
[62] |
Pham M.T . ( 2006). Emotion and rationality: A critical review and interpretation of empirical evidence. Review of General Psychology, 11, (2), 155-178.
doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.11.2.155 URL |
[63] |
Pivetti M., Camodeca M., & Rapino M . ( 2016). Shame, guilt, and anger: Their cognitive, physiological, and behavioral correlates. Current Psychology, 35, (4), 690-699.
doi: 10.1007/s12144-015-9339-5 URL |
[64] | Rai T.S., &Fiske A.P . ( 2011). Moral psychology is relationship regulation: Moral motives for unity, hierarchy, equality, and proportionality. Psychological Review, 118, (1), 57-75. |
[65] | Rohrmann S., &Hopp H. ( 2008). Cardiovascular indicators of disgust. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 68, (3), 201-208. |
[66] | Rozin P. ( 1999). The process of moralization. Psychological Science, 10, (3), 218-221. |
[67] | Rozin P., Haidt J., & Fincher K . ( 2009). From oral to moral. Science, 323, (5918), 1179-1180. |
[68] | Rozin P., Haidt J., & Mc Cauley C . ( 2000). Disgust. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions( 2nd ed., pp. 637-653). New York: Guilford. |
[69] |
Rozin P., Markwith M., & Mccauley C . ( 1994). Sensitivity to indirect contacts with other persons: AIDS aversion as a composite of aversion to strangers, infection, moral taint, and misfortune. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, (3), 495-504.
doi: 10.1037//0021-843X.103.3.495 URL pmid: 7930049 |
[70] | Rozin P., Markwith M., & Nemeroff C . ( 1992). Magical contagion beliefs and fear of AIDS. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22, (14), 1081-1092. |
[71] | Rozin P., &Royzman E.B . ( 2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, (4), 296-320. |
[72] |
Sanfey A. G., Rilling J. K., Aronson J. A., Nystrom L. E., & Cohen J. D . ( 2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the Ultimatum Game. Science, 300, (5626), 1755-1758.
doi: 10.1126/science.1082976 URL pmid: 12805551 |
[73] |
Schaich Borg J., Lieberman D., & Kiehl K. A . ( 2008). Infection, incest, and iniquity: Investigating the neural correlates of disgust and morality. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, (9), 1529-1546.
doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20109 URL pmid: 3969035 |
[74] |
Schnall S., Haidt J., Clore G. L., & Jordan A. H . ( 2008). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, (8), 1096-1109.
doi: 10.1177/0146167208317771 URL pmid: 18505801 |
[75] | Schur A . ( 2008). Beyond moral evangelism: On the rejection of punishment in Late Tolstoy. Tolstoy Studies Journal, 20, 38. |
[76] |
Selterman D., Moors A. C., & Koleva S . ( 2018). Moral judgment toward relationship betrayals and those who commit them. Personal Relationships, 25 (1), 65-86.
doi: 10.1111/pere.12228 URL |
[77] |
Sharvit K., Brambilla M., Babush M., & Colucci F. P . ( 2015). To feel or not to feel when my group harms others? The regulation of collective guilt as motivated reasoning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41, (9), 1223-1235.
doi: 10.1177/0146167215592843 URL pmid: 26130597 |
[78] |
Sherman G.D., &Clore G.L . ( 2010). The color of sin: White and black are perceptual symbols of moral purity and pollution. Psychological Science, 20, (8), 1019-1025.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02403.x URL pmid: 19619180 |
[79] |
Simpson J., Carter S., Anthony S. H., & Overton P. G . ( 2006). Is disgust a homogeneous emotion?. Motivation and Emotion, 30, (1), 31-41.
doi: 10.1007/s11031-006-9005-1 URL |
[80] | Smetana J.G . ( 1995). Morality in context: Abstractions, ambiguities and applications. In R. Vasta (Ed.), Annals of child development ( Vol 10, pp. 83-130). London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. |
[81] |
Smith E.R . ( 1993). Chapter 13-Social identity and social emotions: Toward new conceptualizations of prejudice. Affect Cognition and Stereotyping, 297-315.
doi: 10.1016/B978-0-08-088579-7.50017-X URL |
[82] |
Smith E. R., Seger C. R., & Mackie D. M . ( 2007). Can emotions be truly group level? Evidence regarding four conceptual criteria. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, (3), 431-446.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.3.431 URL pmid: 17723058 |
[83] | Stellar J.E., &Willer R. ( 2013). The corruption of value: Negative moral associations diminish the value of money. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5, (1), 60-66. |
[84] |
Tangney J. P., Stuewig J., & Mashek D. J . ( 2006). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 345-372.
doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070145 pmid: 16953797 |
[85] |
Tapp C., &Occhipinti S. ( 2016). The essence of crime: Contagious transmission from those who have committed moral transgressions. British Journal of Social Psychology, 55, (4), 756-772.
doi: 10.1111/bjso.12153 URL pmid: 27480621 |
[86] |
Tasimi A., &Gelman S.A . ( 2016). Dirty money: The role of moral history in economic judgments. Cognitive Science, 41, (S3), 523-544.
doi: 10.1111/cogs.12464 URL pmid: 28000966 |
[87] |
Travis R., &Fergus T.A . ( 2015). The potentiating effect of disgust sensitivity on the relationship between disgust propensity and mental contamination. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 6, 114-119.
doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2015.06.007 URL |
[88] |
Umphress E. E., Bingham J. B., & Mitchell M. S . ( 2010). Unethical behavior in the name of the company: The moderating effect of organizational identification and positive reciprocity beliefs on unethical pro-organizational behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, (4), 769-780.
doi: 10.1037/a0019214 URL pmid: 20604596 |
[89] |
Xie W., Yu B., Zhou X., Sedikides C., & Vohs K. D . ( 2014). Money, moral transgressions, and blame. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24, (3), 299-306.
doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2013.12.002 URL |
[90] |
Ying X. P., Luo J., Chiu C. Y., Wu Y. H., Xu Y., & Fan J . ( 2018). Functional dissociation of the posterior and anterior insula in moral disgust. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 860.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00860 URL |
[91] | Zanin A. C., Bisel R. S., & Adame E. A . ( 2016). Supervisor moral talk contagion and trust-in-supervisor: mitigating the workplace moral mum effect. Management Communication Quarterly, 30, (2), 147-163. |
[92] |
Zhang X., Guo Q., Zhang Y., Lou L., & Ding D . ( 2015). Different timing features in brain processing of core and moral disgust pictures: An event-related potentials study. Plos One, 10, (5), e0128531.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0128531 URL pmid: 4444107 |
[93] | Zhong C.B., &Liljenquist K. ( 2006). Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science, 313, (5792), 1451-1452. |
[94] | Zhong C-B., Strejcek B., & Sivanathan N . ( 2010). A clean self can render harsh moral judgment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, (5), 859-862. |
[1] | XIAO Tingwei, DONG Jie, LIANG Fei, WANG Fushun, LI Yang. The relationship between disgust and suicidal behavior [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(1): 87-98. |
[2] | ZHANG Xia, LEI Yi, WANG Fushun. The influence of oxytocin, progesterone and estrogen on disgust and its neurophysiological mechanism [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(1): 85-97. |
[3] | DU Weiqiang. Disgust and consumer behavior [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(11): 1929-1938. |
[4] | WU Bao-Pei;CHANG Lei. On the Relationship Between Disgust and Moral Judgment [J]. , 2012, 20(2): 309-316. |
[5] | WU Bao-Pei;CHANG Lei. Psychological Defense against Disease: How Humans Cope with Pathogen Threat [J]. , 2011, 19(3): 410-419. |
[6] | HUANG Hao;LUO Yu;FENG Ting-Yong;LI Hong. The Neural Basis of Disgust Processing [J]. , 2010, 18(9): 1449-1457. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||