Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2022, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (7): 1604-1611.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.01604
• Regular Articles • Previous Articles Next Articles
Received:
2021-08-26
Online:
2022-07-15
Published:
2022-05-17
Contact:
WANG Xiuxin
E-mail:xxwang@qfnu.edu.cn
CLC Number:
WANG Xiuxin, SHEN Yifan. How to cope with the threat to moral self? The perspectives of memory bias in moral contexts[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(7): 1604-1611.
[1] | 甘绍平. (2017). 常人道德的尺度. 道德与文明, (3), 30-36. |
[2] | 关旭旭, 王红波. (2021). 抑制引起的遗忘及其神经机制. 心理科学进展, 29(4), 665-676. |
[3] | 李宏翰, 于娟. (2013). 道德自我受胁及其应对机制. 广西师范大学学报(哲学社会科学版), 49(1), 147-153. |
[4] | 李荆广, 郭秀艳. (2009). 记忆研究的功能取向. 心理科学进展, 17(5), 923-930. |
[5] | 刘传军, 廖江群. (2021). 道德困境研究的范式沿革及其理论价值. 心理科学进展, 29(8), 1508-1520. |
[6] | 刘新明, 朱滢. (2002). 记忆的自我参照效应. 心理科学进展, 10(2), 121-126. |
[7] | 莫映桃, 高申春. (2011). 记忆的社会建构论阐释. 心理研究, 4(3), 21-27. |
[8] | 王珏, 吴明证, 孙晓玲. (2016). 道德失调的自我调节策略. 心理科学, 39(6), 1473-1478. |
[9] | 伍麟, 白仲琪. (2019). 为何不道德行为容易重复发生?——基于心理学的解释. 华中师范大学学报(人文社会科学版), 58(2), 184-192. |
[10] |
Anderson, M. C., & Hanslmayr, S. (2014). Neural mechanisms of motivated forgetting. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(6), 279-292.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.03.002 pmid: 24747000 |
[11] |
Barkan, R., Ayal, S., & Dan, A. (2015). Ethical dissonance, justifications, and moral behavior. Current Opinion in Psychology, 6, 157-161.
doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.08.001 URL |
[12] |
Barneron, M., & Yaniv, I. (2020). Advice-giving under conflict of interest: Context enhances self-serving behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 91, 104046.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2020.104046 URL |
[13] |
Carlson, R. W., Maréchal, M. A., Oud, B., Fehr, E., & Crockett, M. J. (2020). Motivated misremembering of selfish decisions. Nature Communications, 11, 2100.
doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-15602-4 pmid: 32350253 |
[14] |
Dana, J., Weber, R. A., & Kuang, J. X. (2007). Exploiting moral wiggle room: Experiments demonstrating an illusory preference for fairness. Economic Theory, 33(1), 67-80.
doi: 10.1007/s00199-006-0153-z URL |
[15] |
Durbin, K. A., Mitchell, K. J., & Johnson, M. K. (2017). Source memory that encoding was self-referential: The influence of stimulus characteristics. Memory, 25(9), 1191-1200.
doi: 10.1080/09658211.2017.1282517 pmid: 28276984 |
[16] |
Evans, C., Ehlers, A., Mezey, G., & Clark, D. M. (2007a). Intrusive memories and ruminations related to violent crime among young offenders: Phenomenological characteristics. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 20(2), 183-196.
doi: 10.1002/jts.20204 URL |
[17] |
Evans, C., Ehlers, A., Mezey, G., & Clark, D. M. (2007b). Intrusive memories in perpetrators of violent crime: Emotions and cognitions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75(1), 134-144.
doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.75.1.134 URL |
[18] |
Galeotti, F., Saucet, C., & Villeval, M. C. (2020). Unethical amnesia responds more to instrumental than to hedonic motives. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 117(41), 25423-25428.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.2011291117 URL |
[19] |
Gino, F. (2015). Understanding ordinary unethical behavior: Why people who value morality act immorally. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 3, 107-111.
doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.03.001 URL |
[20] |
Hu, X., Bergström, Z. M., Bodenhausen, G. V., & Rosenfeld, J. P. (2015). Suppressing unwanted autobiographical memories reduces their automatic influences: Evidence from electrophysiology and an implicit autobiographical memory test. Psychological Science, 26(7), 1098-1106.
doi: 10.1177/0956797615575734 URL |
[21] |
Hu, X., Bergström, Z. M., Gagnepain, P., & Anderson, M. C. (2017). Suppressing unwanted memories reduces their unintended influences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(2), 197-206.
doi: 10.1177/0963721417689881 URL |
[22] |
Huang, S., Stanley, M. L., & de Brigard, F. (2020). The phenomenology of remembering our moral transgressions. Memory and Cognition, 48(2), 277-286.
doi: 10.3758/s13421-019-01009-0 URL |
[23] |
Hofmann, W., Wisneski, D. C., Brandt, M. J., & Skitka, L. J. (2014). Morality in everyday life. Science, 345(6202), 1340-1343.
doi: 10.1126/science.1251560 URL |
[24] |
Klein, N., & Epley, N. (2016). Maybe holier, but definitely less evil, than you: Bounded self-righteousness in social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110(5), 660-674.
doi: 10.1037/pspa0000050 URL |
[25] |
Klein, N., & Epley, N. (2017). Less evil than you: Bounded self-righteousness in character inferences, emotional reactions, and behavioral extremes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(8), 1202-1212.
doi: 10.1177/0146167217711918 URL |
[26] |
Klein, S. B., & Loftus, J. (1988). The nature of self-referent encoding: The contributions of elaborative and organizational processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(1), 5-11.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.55.1.5 URL |
[27] |
Kouchaki, M., & Gino, F. (2016). Memories of unethical actions become obfuscated over time. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(22), 6166-6171.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1523586113 URL |
[28] |
Mazar, N., Amir, O., & Ariely, D. (2008). The dishonesty of honest people: A theory of self-concept maintenance. Journal of Marketing Research, 45(6), 633-644.
doi: 10.1509/jmkr.45.6.633 URL |
[29] |
Mcdonald, R. I., & Crandall, C. S. (2015). Social norms and social influence. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 3, 147-151.
doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.04.006 URL |
[30] |
Otto, P. E., & Bolle, F. (2015). Exploiting one’s power with a guilty conscience: An experimental investigation of self-serving biases. Journal of Economic Psychology, 51, 79-89.
doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2015.08.005 URL |
[31] |
Rigney, A. E., Schnyer, D. M., Hu, X., & Beer, J. S. (2021). Mechanisms of a spotless self-image: Navigating negative, self-relevant feedback. Self and Identity, 20(8), 1057-1076.
doi: 10.1080/15298868.2020.1806918 URL |
[32] |
Ritchie, T. D., Sedikides, C., & Skowronski, J. J. (2017). Does a person selectively recall the good or the bad from their personal past? It depends on the recall target and the person's favourability of self-views. Memory, 25(8), 934-944.
doi: 10.1080/09658211.2016.1233984 URL |
[33] |
Rode, J., & Menestrel, M. L. (2011). The influence of decision power on distributive fairness. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 79(3), 246-255.
doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2011.02.003 URL |
[34] |
Rom, S. C., & Conway, P. (2018). The strategic moral self: Self-presentation shapes moral dilemma judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 74, 24-37.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.08.003 URL |
[35] |
Rowell, S. F., & Jaswal, V. K. (2021). I remember being nice: Self-enhancement memory bias in middle childhood. Memory, 29(2), 261-269.
doi: 10.1080/09658211.2021.1877307 URL |
[36] | Rubin, D. C. (1986). Autobiographical memory. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. |
[37] |
Saucet, C., & Villeval, M. C. (2019). Motivated memory in dictator games. Games and Economic Behavior, 117, 250-279.
doi: 10.1016/j.geb.2019.05.011 URL |
[38] | Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., & Cai, H. (2015). On the panculturality of self-enhancement and self-protection motivation: The case for the universality of self-esteem. Advances in Motivation Science, 2, 185-241. |
[39] |
Sedikides, C., Green, J. D., Saunders, J., Skowronski, J. J., & Zengel, B. (2016). Mnemic neglect: Selective amnesia of one’s faults. European Review of Social Psychology, 27(1), 1-62.
doi: 10.1080/10463283.2016.1183913 URL |
[40] |
Shalvi, S., Gino, F., Barkan, R., & Ayal, S. (2015). Self-serving justifications: Doing wrong and feeling moral. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(2), 125-130.
doi: 10.1177/0963721414553264 URL |
[41] |
Shu, L. L., & Gino, F. (2012). Sweeping dishonesty under the rug: How unethical actions lead to forgetting of moral rules. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(6), 1164-1177.
doi: 10.1037/a0028381 URL |
[42] |
Stanley, M. L., & de Brigard, F. (2019). Moral memories and the belief in the good self. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(4), 387-391.
doi: 10.1177/0963721419847990 URL |
[43] |
Stanley, M. L., Yang, B. W., & de Brigard, F. (2018). No evidence for unethical amnesia for imagined actions: A failed replication and extension. Memory and Cognition, 46(5), 787-795.
doi: 10.3758/s13421-018-0803-y URL |
[44] |
Tappin, B. M., & McKay, R. T. (2017). The illusion of moral superiority. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8(6), 623-631.
doi: 10.1177/1948550616673878 pmid: 29081899 |
[45] |
Tasimi, A., & Johnson, M. K. (2015). A self-serving bias in children’s memories? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(3), 528-533.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000072 URL |
[46] |
Turk, D. J., Cunningham, S. J., & Macrae, C. N. (2008). Self-memory biases in explicit and incidental encoding of trait adjectives. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(3), 1040-1045.
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.02.004 URL |
[47] |
Zhang, Y., Pan, Z., Li, K., & Guo, Y. (2018). Self-serving bias in memories: Selectively forgetting the connection between negative information and the self. Experimental Psychology, 65(4), 236-244.
doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000409 URL |
[48] |
Zhu, Y., Zhang, L., Fan, J., & Han, S. H. (2007). Neural basis of cultural influence on self-representation. NeuroImage, 34(3), 1310-1316.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.047 URL |
[49] |
Zlatev, J. J., Kupor, D. M., Laurin, K., & Miller, D. T. (2020). Being “Good” or “Good Enough”: Prosocial Risk and the Structure of Moral Self-Regard. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 118(2), 242-253.
doi: 10.1037/pspa0000175 URL |
[1] | WANG Caiyu, Zheng Xiaoxu, YU Qiuting, LEI Li. The dilemma of green consumption: identity construction or environmental concern? [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(8): 1507-1520. |
[2] | ZHANG Hongwei;LI Ye. Moral Behavior under Two Kinds of Moral Self-regulation Mechanisms [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2014, 22(7): 1178-1187. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||