心理科学进展 ›› 2021, Vol. 29 ›› Issue (12): 2224-2235.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2021.02224
收稿日期:
2020-09-17
发布日期:
2021-10-26
通讯作者:
熊冠星
E-mail:xiongguanxing@163.com
基金资助:
SUN Hai-Long1, AN Xin-Ru2, XIONG Guan-Xing3()
Received:
2020-09-17
Published:
2021-10-26
Contact:
XIONG Guan-Xing
E-mail:xiongguanxing@163.com
摘要:
多时点结果跨期选择涉及两个及两个以上多个时点结果的权衡, 正日益成为跨期选择的研究重点。现有研究主要从两时点结果和序列结果两方面深入探讨了多时点结果跨期选择中的诸多决策效应, 包括:隐藏的零效应、象征效应、额外金额效应、前端金额效应以及序列效应, 并运用序列模型和权衡模型对上述决策效应进行阐释。未来还应深入探索同时涉及损益结果混合的多时点结果跨期选择, 运用决策过程追踪技术揭示决策效应的过程机制。
中图分类号:
孙海龙, 安薪如, 熊冠星. (2021). 多时点结果跨期选择的决策效应及其心理机制. 心理科学进展 , 29(12), 2224-2235.
SUN Hai-Long, AN Xin-Ru, XIONG Guan-Xing. (2021). Decision effect in multiple dated intertemporal choice and its psychological mechanisms. Advances in Psychological Science, 29(12), 2224-2235.
时点 | T1 | T2 | T3 | T4 | T5 | 决策效应 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
单时点 | 选项1 | X1 | —— | ||||
选项2 | X2 | ||||||
两时点结果 | |||||||
零 | 选项1 | X1 | 0 | 隐藏的零效应 (Hidden zero effect) | |||
选项2 | 0 | X2 | |||||
位置(前) | 选项1 | m | X1 | 象征效应 (Mere token effect) | |||
选项2 | m | X2 | |||||
位置(中) | 选项1 | X1 | m | 额外金钱效应 (Extra dated-money effect) | |||
选项2 | m | X2 | |||||
位置(后) | 选项1 | X1 | m | ||||
选项2 | X2 | m | |||||
位置(相同) | 选项1 | X1+m | 前端金额效应 (Front-end amount effect) | ||||
选项2 | m | X2 |
表1 两时点结果跨期选择中的决策效应
时点 | T1 | T2 | T3 | T4 | T5 | 决策效应 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
单时点 | 选项1 | X1 | —— | ||||
选项2 | X2 | ||||||
两时点结果 | |||||||
零 | 选项1 | X1 | 0 | 隐藏的零效应 (Hidden zero effect) | |||
选项2 | 0 | X2 | |||||
位置(前) | 选项1 | m | X1 | 象征效应 (Mere token effect) | |||
选项2 | m | X2 | |||||
位置(中) | 选项1 | X1 | m | 额外金钱效应 (Extra dated-money effect) | |||
选项2 | m | X2 | |||||
位置(后) | 选项1 | X1 | m | ||||
选项2 | X2 | m | |||||
位置(相同) | 选项1 | X1+m | 前端金额效应 (Front-end amount effect) | ||||
选项2 | m | X2 |
模型 | 理论 基础 | 基本内容 | 两时点结果 | 序列结果领域 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
相容证据 | 相斥(未证实) | 相容证据 | 相斥(未证实) | |||
权衡 模型 | 基于 维度 | 人们需要比较价值维度和时间维度之间的差异:在多个结果的累积效用与效用累积的时长之间加以权衡。其中时长受到实际延迟时间和延迟时间内结果的累积效用的影响。 | 隐藏的零效应 象征效应 额外金钱效应 前端金额效应 | —— | 序列效应 | |
序列 模型 | 基于 选项 | 人们需要权衡各选项累积效用大小。对于多时点跨期而言, 每个选项的效用由三部分构成:时间折扣效用、改善效用和延展效用。获益情境下递增序列的效用大于递减序列。 | 隐藏的零效应 象征效应 额外金钱效应 前端金额效应 | 序列效应 | —— |
表2 多时点结果跨期选择中决策效应的理论模型
模型 | 理论 基础 | 基本内容 | 两时点结果 | 序列结果领域 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
相容证据 | 相斥(未证实) | 相容证据 | 相斥(未证实) | |||
权衡 模型 | 基于 维度 | 人们需要比较价值维度和时间维度之间的差异:在多个结果的累积效用与效用累积的时长之间加以权衡。其中时长受到实际延迟时间和延迟时间内结果的累积效用的影响。 | 隐藏的零效应 象征效应 额外金钱效应 前端金额效应 | —— | 序列效应 | |
序列 模型 | 基于 选项 | 人们需要权衡各选项累积效用大小。对于多时点跨期而言, 每个选项的效用由三部分构成:时间折扣效用、改善效用和延展效用。获益情境下递增序列的效用大于递减序列。 | 隐藏的零效应 象征效应 额外金钱效应 前端金额效应 | 序列效应 | —— |
[1] | 江程铭, 谢铠杰, 何铨. (2018). 跨期选择研究:从单时点结果到多时点结果. 心理科学进展, 26(5), 922-927. |
[2] | 李纾. (2016). 决策心理:齐当别之道. 上海: 华东师范大学出版社. |
[3] | 苏寅, 毕研玲, 李纾, 饶俪琳. (2010). "避害"还是"趋害"? 心理科学, (2), 252-254. |
[4] | 孙海龙. (2018). 序列类型对跨期决策的影响:基于心理动量的视角 (博士学位论文)? 暨南大学, 广州. |
[5] | 孙红月, 江程铭. (2016). 跨期决策是基于选项还是基于维度? 心理科学进展, 24(3), 431-437. |
[6] | 孙红月, 鲁盼, 蒋元萍. (2021). 跨期决策中的负折扣现象及其机制. 心理科学进展, 29(7), 1291-1299. |
[7] |
Abdellaoui, M., Attema, A. E., & Bleichrodt, H.(2009). Intertemporal tradeoffs for gains and losses: An experimental measurement of discounted utility. The Economic Journal, 120(545), 845-866. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0297.2009.02308.
doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0297.2009.02308.x URL |
[8] | Abdellaoui, M., Bleichrodt, H., & L'Haridon, O.(2013). Sign-dependence in intertemporal choice. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 47(3), 225-253. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11166-013-9181-9 |
[9] |
Bartels, D. M., & Urminsky, O.(2011). On intertemporal selfishness: How the perceived instability of identity underlies impatient consumption. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(1), 182-198. https://doi.org/10.1086/658339
doi: 10.1086/658339 URL |
[10] |
Bartels, D. M., & Urminsky, O.(2015). To know and to care: How awareness and valuation of the future jointly shape consumer spending. Journal of Consumer Research, 41(6), 1469-1485. https://doi.org/10.1086/680670
doi: 10.1086/680670 URL |
[11] | Białaszek, W., Ostaszewski, P., & Marcowski, P.(2016). Positive time preference for monetary sequences of rewards : The role of temporal spacing. Studia Psychologiczne, 54(2) 25-33. https://doi.org/10.2478/v1067-010-0154-9 |
[12] | Duffy, S., & Smith, J.(2013). Preference for increasing wages: How do people value various streams of income? Judgment and Decision Making, 8(1), 74-90. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1631845 |
[13] | Duffy, S., Smith, J., & Woods, K.(2015). How does the preference for increasing payments depend on the size and source of the payments? Management Science Letters, 5(12), 1071-1080. https://doi.org/10.5267/j.msl.2015.10.007 |
[14] |
Faralla, V., Novarese, M., & Ardizzone, A.(2017). Framing effects in intertemporal choice: A nudge experiment. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 71, 13-25.
doi: 10.1016/j.socec.2017.09.002 URL |
[15] |
Franco-Watkins, A. M., Mattson, R. E., & Jackson, M. D.(2016). Now or later? Attentional processing and intertemporal choice. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 29(2-3), 206-217.
doi: 10.1002/bdm.v29.1 URL |
[16] |
Frederick, S., Loewenstein, G., & O'Donoghue, T.(2002). Time discounting and time preference: A critical review. Journal of Economic Literature, 40(2), 351-401.
doi: 10.1257/jel.40.2.351 URL |
[17] |
Gathergood, J.(2012). Self-control, financial literacy and consumer over-indebtedness. Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(3), 590-602. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2011.11.006
doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2011.11.006 URL |
[18] |
Guyse, J. L., Keller, L. R., & Huynh, C. H.(2020). Valuing sequences of lives lost or saved over time: Preference for uniform sequences. Decision Analysis, 17(1), 24-38.
doi: 10.1287/deca.2019.0397 URL |
[19] |
Harvey, C. M.(1994). The reasonableness of non-constant discounting. Journal of Public Economics, 53(1), 31-51.
doi: 10.1016/0047-2727(94)90012-4 URL |
[20] |
Hoelzl, E., Kamleitner, B., & Kirchler, E.(2011). Loan repayment plans as sequences of instalments. Journal of Economic Psychology, 32(4), 621-631.
doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2011.02.002 URL |
[21] |
Li, S.(2004). A behavioral choice model when computational ability matters. Applied Intelligence, 20(2), 147-163.
doi: 10.1023/B:APIN.0000013337.01711.c7 URL |
[22] | Jiang, C. M., Hu, F. P., & Zhu, L. F.(2014). Introducing upfront losses as well as gains decreases impatience in intertemporal choices with rewards. Judgment and Decision Making, 9(4), 297-302. |
[23] | Loewenstein, G., & Frederick, S.(2008). Conflicting motives in evaluations of sequences. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 37(2), 221-235. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11166-008-9051-z |
[24] | Loewenstein, G., & Prelec, D.(1991). Negative time preference. American Economic Review, American Economic Association. |
[25] |
Loewenstein, G., & Prelec, D.(1992). Anomalies in intertemporal choice: Evidence and an interpretation. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 107(2), 573-597.
doi: 10.2307/2118482 URL |
[26] |
Loewenstein, G. F., & Prelec, D.(1993). Preferences for sequences of outcomes. Psychological Review, 100(1), 91-108. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.100.1.91
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.100.1.91 URL |
[27] |
Magen, E., Dweck, C. S., & Gross, J. J.(2008). The hidden-zero effect: Representing a single choice as an extended sequence reduces impulsive choice. Psychological Science, 19(7), 648-649. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02137.x.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02137.x URL pmid: 18727778 |
[28] |
Mazur, J. E.(1984). Tests of an equivalence rule for fixed and variable reinforcer delays. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 10(4), 426-436.
doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.10.4.426 URL |
[29] |
O'Hora, D., Carey, R., Kervick, A., Crowley, D., & Dabrowski, M.(2016). Decisions in motion: Decision dynamics during intertemporal choice reflect subjective evaluation of delayed rewards. Science Report, 6, 20740. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep20740
doi: 10.1038/srep20740 URL |
[30] |
Radu, P. T., Yi, R., Bickel, W. K., Gross, J. J., & McClure, S. M.(2011). A mechanism for reducing delay discounting by altering temporal attention. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 96(3), 363-385. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2011.96-363
doi: 10.1901/jeab.2011.96-363 URL |
[31] |
Rambaud, S. C., Pascual, J. L.,& de los Ángeles del Pino Álvarez, M.(2019). Preferences over sequences of payments: A new validation of the q-exponential discounting. Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and Its Applications, 515, 332-345. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physa.2018.09.169
doi: 10.1016/j.physa.2018.09.169 URL |
[32] | Rao, L.-L., & Li, S.(2011). New paradoxes in intertemporal choice. Judgment and Decision Making, 6(2), 122-129. |
[33] |
Read, D., Olivola, C. Y., & Hardisty, D. J.(2017). The value of nothing: Asymmetric attention to opportunity costs drives intertemporal decision making. Management Science, 63(12), 3999-4446. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2016.2547
doi: 10.1287/mnsc.2016.2540 URL |
[34] |
Read, D., & Powell, M.(2002). Reasons for sequence preferences. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15(5), 433-460. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.429
doi: 10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0771 URL |
[35] |
Read, D., & Scholten, M.(2012). Tradeoffs between sequences: Weighing accumulated outcomes against outcome-adjusted delays. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(6), 1675-1688. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028216
doi: 10.1037/a0028216 URL |
[36] | Reeck, C., Wall, D., & Johnson, E. J.(2017). Search predicts and changes patience in intertemporal choice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(45), 11890-11895. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1707040114 |
[37] | Reyes-Huerta, H.E., Valerio, C. & Carranza, R. Is the Value of Future Rewards Independent of Gains and Losses Concurrent with Intertemporal Choices?. Psychological Record 71, 149-156 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/ s40732-020-00424-z |
[38] |
Scholten, M., & Read, D.(2010). The psychology of intertemporal tradeoffs. Psychological Review, 117(3), 925-944. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019619
doi: 10.1037/a0019619 URL pmid: 20658858 |
[39] |
Scholten, M., Read, D., & Sanborn, A.(2016). Cumulative weighing of time in intertemporal tradeoffs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(9), 1177-1205. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000198.supp
doi: 10.1037/xge0000198 URL |
[40] |
Schoemann, M., Lüken, M., Grage, T., Kieslich, P. J., & Scherbaum, S.(2019). Validating mouse-tracking: How design factors influence action dynamics in intertemporal decision making. Behavior Research Methods, 51(5), 2356-2377
doi: 10.3758/s13428-018-1179-4 pmid: 30684228 |
[41] |
Schulte-Mecklenbeck, M., Kühberger, A., Gagl, B., & Hutzler, F.(2017). Inducing thought processes: Bringing process measures and cognitive processes closer together. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 30(5), 1001-1013. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2007
doi: 10.1002/bdm.v30.5 URL |
[42] | Shen, S. C., Huang, Y. N., Jiang, C. M., & Li, S.(2019). Can asymmetric subjective opportunity cost effect explain impatience in intertemporal choice? A replication study. Judgment and Decision Making, 14(2), 214-222. |
[43] | Stillman, P. E., Krajbich, I., & Ferguson, M. J.(2020). Using dynamic monitoring of choices to predict and understand risk preferences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 117(50), 31738-31747. |
[44] |
Sun, H. L., Li, A. M., Shen, S. C., Xiong, G. X., Rao, L.L., Zheng, R., … Li, S.(2020). Early departure, early revival: A “free from care” account of negative temporal discounting. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 16(2), 103-116. https://doi.org/10.5709/acp-0289-0
doi: 10.5709/acp-0289-0 URL |
[45] | Sun, H. Y., & Jiang, C. M.(2015). Introducing money at any time can reduce discounting in intertemporal choices with rewards: An extension of the upfront money effect. Judgment and Decision Making, 10(6), 564-570. |
[46] |
Urminsky, O., & Kivetz, R.(2011). Scope insensitivity and the “mere token” effect. Journal of Marketing Research, 48(2), 282-295. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.48.2.282
doi: 10.1509/jmkr.48.2.282 URL |
[47] |
Wu, C. Y., & He, G. B.(2012). The effects of time perspective and salience of possible monetary losses on intertemporal choice. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 40(10), 1645-1654
doi: 10.2224/sbp.2012.40.10.1645 URL |
[48] | Zhao, C. X., Jiang, C. M., Zhou, L., Li, S., Rao, L. L., & Zheng, R.(2015). The hidden opportunity cost of time effect on intertemporal choice. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(March),1-7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00311 |
[1] | 杨权, 尹述飞, 胡兴. 跨期选择中的年龄差异及其机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(6): 987-993. |
[2] | 何清华, 李丹丹. 中国儿童青少年跨期决策的发展与脑发育机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(3): 381-389. |
[3] | 江程铭, 谢铠杰, 何铨. 跨期选择研究:从单时点结果到多时点结果[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(5): 922-927. |
[4] | 张顺民;冯廷勇. 拖延的认知神经机制与基因:行为−脑−基因的多角度研究[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(3): 393-403. |
[5] | 任天虹;胡志善;孙红月;刘扬;李纾. 选择与坚持:跨期选择与延迟满足之比较[J]. 心理科学进展, 2015, 23(2): 303-315. |
[6] | 彭娟;冯廷勇. 成瘾人群跨期选择的神经机制及干预方法[J]. 心理科学进展, 2014, 22(5): 810-821. |
[7] | 梁竹苑;刘欢. 跨期选择的性质探索[J]. 心理科学进展, 2011, 19(7): 959-966. |
[8] | 甘怡群. 未来取向应对的双阶段序列模型及其时间透视机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2011, 19(11): 1583-1587. |
[9] | 孙彦. 风险条件下的跨期选择[J]. 心理科学进展, 2011, 19(1): 28-34. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||