心理科学进展 ›› 2021, Vol. 29 ›› Issue (7): 1216-1230.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2021.01216
收稿日期:
2020-08-08
出版日期:
2021-07-15
发布日期:
2021-05-24
通讯作者:
赵微
E-mail:zhaowei@snnu.edu.cn
基金资助:
JING Wei1, ZHANG Jie2, FU Jinxia1, TIAN Lin2, ZHAO Wei1()
Received:
2020-08-08
Online:
2021-07-15
Published:
2021-05-24
Contact:
ZHAO Wei
E-mail:zhaowei@snnu.edu.cn
摘要:
正常发展(Typically developing, TD)婴儿的先天面孔注意倾向稳定地存在于不同发展阶段不同刺激情景中, 且在生命第一年内呈现短暂下降后快速增强的发展趋势。TD婴幼儿的视觉注意在第4至6周之间发生由皮下控制向皮层控制的关键性转变之后, 伴随面孔视觉经验的不断积累, 逐渐形成的面孔特异性皮层网络对面孔的优先选择性反应逐渐增强。而孤独症谱系(Autism Spectrum Disorder, ASD)婴幼儿具备初始的面孔注意先天倾向, 但在面孔皮层发育关键期内逐渐偏离正常轨道, 在1岁左右表现出面孔注意障碍。该群体先天的感知注意损伤或社会动机缺失可能导致其在关键性转变期内的面孔视觉经验输入不足, 进而阻碍面孔特异性皮层网络的正常发展。未来研究可考虑采用生物遗传学方法和近红外脑成像技术探索新生儿面孔注意先天倾向的起源, 系统考察社会场景中感知觉特征和社会性特征对ASD高危婴儿面孔注意发展轨迹尤其是关键性转变期的影响作用。
中图分类号:
荆伟, 张婕, 付锦霞, 田琳, 赵微. (2021). 婴幼儿面孔注意偏向:先天倾向与发展轨迹——来自正常和孤独症婴幼儿的证据. 心理科学进展 , 29(7), 1216-1230.
JING Wei, ZHANG Jie, FU Jinxia, TIAN Lin, ZHAO Wei. (2021). Attention bias to faces in infants and toddlers: Inborn predispositions and developmental changes. Advances in Psychological Science, 29(7), 1216-1230.
[1] | 魏玲, 连榕, 余宥依, 孙超. (2017). 自闭症谱系障碍儿童对不同类型社交场景注意模式的眼动差异. 心理与行为研究, 15(4), 536-543. |
[2] | Amso, D., Haas, S., & Markant, J. (2014). An eye tracking investigation of developmental change in bottom-up attention orienting to faces in cluttered natural scenes. PLoS One, 9(1). http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0085701. |
[3] |
Amso, D., Haas, S., Tenenbaum, E., Markant, J., & Sheinkopf, S. J. (2014). Bottom-up attention orienting in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(3), 664-673. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1925-5.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1925-5 URL |
[4] |
Amso, D., & Scerif, G. (2015). The attentive brain: Insights from developmental cognitive neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(10), 606-619. http://doi.org/10.1038/nrn4025.
doi: 10.1038/nrn4025 URL |
[5] |
Arcaro, M. J., Schade, P. F., Vincent, J. L., Ponce, C. R., & Livingstone, M. S. (2017). Seeing faces is necessary for face-domain formation. Nature Neuroscience, 20(10), 1404-1412. http://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4635.
doi: 10.1038/nn.4635 URL |
[6] |
Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Ashwin, E., Knickmeyer, R. C., Taylor, K., & Hackett, G. (2009). Fetal testosterone and autistic traits. British Journal of Psychology, 100(1), 1-22. http://doi.org/10.1348/000712608X311731.
doi: 10.1348/000712608X311731 URL |
[7] |
Azzi, J. C. B., Sirigu, A., & Duhamel, J.-R. (2012). Modulation of value representation by social context in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(6), 2126-2131. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1111715109.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1111715109 URL |
[8] |
Bahrick, L. E., Todd, J. T., Castellanos, I., & Sorondo, B. M. (2016). Enhanced Attention to Speaking Faces versus Other Event Types Emerges Gradually across Infancy. Developmental Psychology, 52(11), 1705-1720. http://doi. org/10.1037/dev0000157.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000157 URL |
[9] |
Benning, S. D., Kovac, M., Campbell, A., Miller, S., Hanna, E. K., Damiano, C. R., ... Dichter, G. S. (2016). Late positive potential ERP responses to social and nonsocial stimuli in youth with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(9), 3068-3077. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2845-y.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2845-y URL |
[10] |
Benson, V., Piper, J., & Fletcher-Watson, S. (2009). Atypical saccadic scanning in autistic spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 47(4), 1178-1182. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.019.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.019 URL |
[11] |
Bradshaw, J., Klin, A., Evans, L., Klaiman, C., Saulnier, C., & Mccracken, C. (2019). Development of attention from birth to 5 months in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 32(2), 1-11. http://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579419000233.
doi: 10.1017/S0954579418001359 URL |
[12] |
Braukmann, R., Lloyd-Fox, S., Blasi, A., Johnson, M. H., Bekkering, H., Buitelaar, J. K., & Hunnius, S. (2018). Diminished socially selective neural processing in 5-month- old infants at high familial risk of autism. European Journal of Neuroscience, 47(6), 720-728. http://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13751.
doi: 10.1111/ejn.13751 URL |
[13] |
Bryson, S. E., Garon, N., Mcmullen, T., Brian, J., Zwaigenbaum, L., Armstrong, V. L., ... Szatmari, P. (2018). Impaired disengagement of attention and its relationship to emotional distress in infants at high-risk for autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 40(5), 487-501. http://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2017.1372368.
doi: 10.1080/13803395.2017.1372368 URL |
[14] |
Buiatti, M., Di Giorgio, E., Piazza, M., Polloni, C., Menna, G., Taddei, F., ... Vallortigara, G. (2018). A cortical route for face-like pattern processing in human newborns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(10), 4625-4630. http://doi.org/10.1101/414284.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1812419116 URL |
[15] |
Chawarska, K., Macari, S., & Shic, F. (2012). Context modulates attention to social scenes in toddlers with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(8), 903-913. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02538.x.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02538.x URL |
[16] |
Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E. S., & Schultz, R. T. (2012). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(4), 231-239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.02.007.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.02.007 URL |
[17] |
Chevallier, C., Parish-Morris, J., McVey, A., Rump, K. M., Sasson, N. J., Herrington, J. D., & Schultz, R. T. (2015). Measuring social attention and motivation in autism spectrum disorder using eye-tracking: Stimulus type matters. Autism Research, 8(5), 620-628. http://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1479.
doi: 10.1002/aur.1479 URL pmid: 26069030 |
[18] |
Constantino, J. N., Kennon-Mcgill, S., Weichselbaum, C., Marrus, N., Haider, A., Glowinski, A. L., ... Jones, W. (2017). Infant viewing of social scenes is under genetic control and is atypical in autism. Nature, 547(7663), 340-344. http://doi.org/10.1038/nature22999.
doi: 10.1038/nature22999 URL pmid: 28700580 |
[19] |
Deen, B., Richardson, H., Dilks, D. D., Takahashi, A., Keil, B., Wald, L. L., ... Saxe, R. (2017). Organization of high- level visual cortex in human infants. Nature Communications, 8(1),13995. http://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms13995.
doi: 10.1038/ncomms13995 URL |
[20] |
Di Giorgio, E., Frasnelli, E., Salva, O. R., Luisa, S. M., Puopolo, M., Tosoni, D., ... Vallortigara G. (2016). Difference in visual social predispositions between newborns at low-and high-risk for autism. Scientific Reports, 6,26395. http://doi.org/10.1038/srep26395.
doi: 10.1038/srep26395 URL |
[21] |
Di Giorgio, E., Turati, C., Altoè, G., & Simion, F. (2012). Face detection in complex visual displays: An eye-tracking study with 3- and 6-month-old infants and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(1), 66-77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.04.012.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.04.012 URL pmid: 22695758 |
[22] | Duan, H., Min, X., Fang, Y., Fan, L., Yang, X., & Zhai, G. (2019). Visual attention analysis and prediction on human faces for children with autism spectrum disorder. ACM Transactions on Multimedia Computing, Communications, and Applications, 15,90. http://doi.org/10.1145/3337066. |
[23] |
Elison, J. T., Paterson, S., Wolff, J. J., Reznick, J. S., Sasson, N. J., Gu, H., ... Piven, J. (2013). White matter microstructure and atypical visual orienting in 7-month-olds at risk for autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(8), 899-908. http://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12091150.
doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12091150 URL |
[24] |
Elison, J. T., Sasson, N. J., Turner-Brown, L. M., Dichter, G. S., & Bodfish, J. W. (2012). Age trends in visual exploration of social and nonsocial information in children with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(2), 842-851. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2011.11.005.
URL pmid: 22639682 |
[25] |
Elsabbagh, M., Fernandes, J., Webb, S. J., Dawson, G., Charman, T., & Johnson, M. H. (2013). Disengagement of visual attention in infancy is associated with emerging autism in toddlerhood. Biological Psychiatry, 74(3),189-194. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.11.030.
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.11.030 URL |
[26] |
Elsabbagh, M., Gliga, T., Pickles, A., Hudry, K., Charman, T., & Johnson, M. H. (2013). The development of face orienting mechanisms in infants at-risk for autism. Behavioural Brain Research, 251,147-154. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.07.030.
doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.07.030 URL |
[27] | End, A., & Gamer, M. (2017). Preferential processing of social features and their interplay with physical saliency in complex naturalistic scenes. Frontiers in Psychology, 8,418. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00418. |
[28] |
Frank, M. C., Amso, D., & Johnson, S. P. (2014). Visual search and attention to faces during early infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 118, 13-26. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.08.012.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.08.012 URL |
[29] |
Frank, M. C., Vul, E., & Johnson, S. P. (2009). Development of infants’ attention to faces during the first year. Cognition, 110(2),160-170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.11.010.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.11.010 URL |
[30] |
Frost-Karlsson, M., Galazka, M. A., Gillberg, C., Gillberg, C., Miniscalco, C., Billstedt, E., ... Johnels, J. A. (2019). Social scene perception in autism spectrum disorder: An eye-tracking and pupillometric study. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 41(10), 1024-1032. http://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2019.1646214.
doi: 10.1080/13803395.2019.1646214 URL |
[31] |
Gale, C. M., Eikeseth, S., & Klintwall, L. (2019). Children with Autism show Atypical Preference for Non-social Stimuli. Scientific Reports, 9(1),10355. http://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-46705-8.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-46705-8 URL |
[32] |
Gauthier, I., & Nelson, C. A. (2001). The development of face expertise. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11(2),219-224. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00200-2.
doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00200-2 URL |
[33] |
Gillespie-Smith, K., Boardman, J. P., Murray, I. C., Norman, J. E., O’Hare, A., & Fletcher-Watson, S. (2016). Multiple measures of fixation on social content in infancy: Evidence for a single social cognitive construct? Infancy, 21(2), 241-257. http://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12103.
URL pmid: 26949376 |
[34] |
Gillespie-Smith, K., Riby, D. M., Hancock, P. J. B., & Doherty-Sneddon, G. (2014). Children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) attend typically to faces and objects presented within their picture communication systems. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 58(5), 459-470. http://doi.org/10.1111/jir.12043.
doi: 10.1111/jir.12043 URL |
[35] |
Gliga, T., Elsabbagh, M., Andravizou, A., & Johnson, M. (2009). Faces attract infants' attention in complex displays. Infancy, 14(5), 550-562. http://doi.org/10.1080/15250000903144199.
doi: 10.1080/15250000903144199 URL |
[36] |
Gluckman, M., & Johnson, S. P. (2013). Attentional capture by social stimuli in young infants. Frontiers in Psychology, 4,527. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00527.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00527 URL pmid: 23966966 |
[37] | Golarai, G., Liberman, A., & Grill-Spector, K. (2015). Experience shapes the development of neural substrates of face processing in human ventral temporal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 27(2), 1229-1244. http://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv314. |
[38] |
Goren, C. C., Sarty, M., & Wu, P. Y. K. (1975). Visual following and pattern discrimination of face-like stimuli by newborn infants. Pediatrics, 56(4), 544-549.
pmid: 1165958 |
[39] |
Grand, R. L., Mondloch, C. J., Maurer, D., & Brent, H. P. (2001). Early visual experience and face processing. Nature, 410(6831),890. http://doi.org/10.1038/35073749.
URL pmid: 11309606 |
[40] |
Green, J., Pickles, A., Pasco, G., Bedford, R., Wan, M. W., Elsabbagh, M., ... Johnson, M. (2017). Randomised trial of a parent-mediated intervention for infants at high risk for autism: Longitudinal outcomes to age 3 years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(12), 1330-1340. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12728.
doi: 10.1111/jcpp.2017.58.issue-12 URL |
[41] |
Greene, D. J., Colich, N., Iacoboni, M., Zaidel, E., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Dapretto, M. (2011). Atypical neural networks for social orienting in autism spectrum disorders. Neuroimage, 56(1),354-362. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.031.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.031 URL |
[42] |
Guillon, Q., Rogé, B., Afzali, M. H., Baduel, S., Kruck, J., & Hadjikhani, N. (2016). Intact perception but abnormal orientation towards face-like objects in young children with ASD. Scientific Reports, 6,22119. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep22119.
doi: 10.1038/srep22119 URL |
[43] |
Hanley, M., McPhillips, M., Mulhern, G., & Riby, D. M. (2013). Spontaneous attention to faces in Asperger syndrome using ecologically valid static stimuli. Autism, 17(6), 754-761. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361312456746.
doi: 10.1177/1362361312456746 URL pmid: 22987893 |
[44] |
Hanley, M., Riby, D. M., Mccormack, T., Carty, C., Coyle, L., Crozier, N., ... Mcphillips, M. (2014). Attention during social interaction in children with autism: Comparison to specific language impairment, typical development, and links to social cognition. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8(7), 908-924. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2014.03.020.
doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2014.03.020 URL |
[45] |
Harrison, A. J., & Slane, M. M. (2020). Examining how types of object distractors distinctly compete for facial attention in autism spectrum disorder using eye tracking. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50(3), 924-934. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04315-3.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-04315-3 URL pmid: 31811617 |
[46] |
Hirai, M., Muramatsu, Y., & Nakamura, M. (2020). Developmental changes in orienting towards faces: A behavioral and eye-tracking study. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 44(2), 157-165. http://doi.org/10.1177/0165025419844031.
doi: 10.1177/0165025419844031 URL |
[47] | Ince, R. A. A., Jaworska, K., Gross, J., Panzeri, S., van Rijsbergen, N. J., Rousselet, G. A., & Schyns, P.G. (2016). The deceptively simple n170 reflects network information processing mechanisms involving visual feature coding and transfer across hemispheres. bioRxiv, 44065. http://doi.org/10.1101/044065. |
[48] |
Itier, R. J., & Taylor, M. J. (2004). Face recognition memory and configural processing: A developmental ERP study using upright, inverted, and Contrast-Reversed faces. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(3), 487-502. http://doi.org/10.1162/089892904322926818.
doi: 10.1162/089892904322926818 URL |
[49] |
Jayaraman, S., Fausey, C. M., & Smith, L. B. (2017). Why are faces denser in the visual experiences of younger than older infants. Developmental Psychology, 53(1), 38-49. http://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000230.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000230 URL pmid: 28026190 |
[50] |
Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H. D., & Morton, J. J. L. (1991). Newborns' preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition, 40(1-2),1-19. http://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(91)90045-6.
URL pmid: 1786670 |
[51] |
Johnson, M. H., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1991). Components of visual orienting in early infancy: Contingency learning, anticipatory looking, and disengaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3(4), 335-344. http://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1991.3.4.335.
doi: 10.1162/jocn.1991.3.4.335 URL |
[52] |
Johnson, M. H., Senju, A., & Tomalski, P. (2015). The two-process theory of face processing: Modifications based on two decades of data from infants and adults. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 50, 169-179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.009.
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.009 URL |
[53] |
Jones, E. J. H., Gliga, T., Bedford, R., Charman, T., & Johnson, M. H. (2014). Developmental pathways to autism: A review of prospective studies of infants at risk. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 39, 1-33. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.12.001.
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.12.001 URL |
[54] |
Jones, E. J. H., Venema, K., Earl, R. K., Lowy, R., Barnes, K., Estes, A., ... Webb, S. J. (2016). Reduced engagement with social stimuli in 6-month-old infants with later autism spectrum disorder: A longitudinal prospective study of infants at high familial risk. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 8(1),7. http://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-016-9139-8.
doi: 10.1186/s11689-016-9139-8 URL |
[55] |
Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2013). Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2-6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature, 504(7480), 427-431. http://doi.org/10.1038/nature12715.
doi: 10.1038/nature12715 URL |
[56] |
Joseph, J. E., Zhu, X., Gundran, A., Davies, F., Clark, J., Ruble, L., ... Bhatt, R. (2015). Typical and atypical neurodevelopment for face specialization: An fMRI study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(6), 1725-1741. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2330-4.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2330-4 URL pmid: 25479816 |
[57] |
Kang, E., Keifer, C. M., Levy, E. J., Foss-Feig, J. H., Mcpartland, J. C., & Lerner, M. D. (2017). Atypicality of the N170 event-related potential in autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 3(8), 657-666. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.11.003.
doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.11.003 URL |
[58] | Kanwisher, N. (2010). Functional specificity in the human brain: A window into the functional architecture of the mind. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(25), 11163-11170. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1005062107. |
[59] | Kelly, D. J., Duarte, S., Meary, D., Bindemann, M., & Pascalis, O. (2019). Infants rapidly detect human faces in complex naturalistic visual scenes. Developmental Science, 22(6),e12829. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12829. |
[60] |
Kleberg, J. L., Thorup, E., & Falck-Ytter, T. (2017). Reduced visual disengagement but intact phasic alerting in young children with autism. Autism Research, 10(3), 539-545. http://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1675.
doi: 10.1002/aur.1675 URL pmid: 27696688 |
[61] |
Kovarski, K., Mennella, R., Wong, S. M., Dunkley, B. T., Taylor, M. J., & Batty, M. (2019). Enhanced early visual responses during implicit emotional faces processing in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49(3), 871-886. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3787-3.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3787-3 URL pmid: 30374763 |
[62] |
Kwon, M., Setoodehnia, M., Baek, J., Luck, S. J., & Oakes, L. M. (2016). The development of visual search in infancy: Attention to faces versus salience. Developmental Psychology, 52(4), 537-555. http://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000080.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000080 URL |
[63] |
Leppanen, J. M. (2016). Using eye tracking to understand infants' attentional bias for faces. Child Development Perspectives, 10(3), 161-165. http://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12180.
doi: 10.1111/cdep.2016.10.issue-3 URL |
[64] |
Libertus, K., Landa, R., & Haworth, J. L. (2017). Development of attention to faces during the first 3 years: Influences of stimulus type. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1976. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01976.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01976 URL pmid: 29204130 |
[65] | Macari, S., Milgramm, A., Reed, J., Shic, F., Powell, K., Macris, D., & Chawarska, K. (2020). Context-Specific dyadic attention vulnerabilities during the first year in infants later developing autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 60(1), 166-175. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2019.12.012. |
[66] |
Macchi, C. V., Turati, C., & Simion, F. (2004). Can a nonspecific bias toward Top-Heavy patterns explain newborns' face preference? Psychological Science, 15(6), 379-383. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00688.x.
URL pmid: 15147490 |
[67] |
Macchi, C. V., Valenza, E., Simion, F., & Leo, I. (2008). Congruency as a nonspecific perceptual property contributing to newborns’ face preference. Child Development, 79(4), 807-820. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01160.x.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01160.x URL pmid: 18717891 |
[68] | Mares, I., Ewing, L., Farran, E. K., Smith, F. W., & Smith, M. L. (2020). Developmental changes in the processing of faces as revealed by EEG decoding. Neuroimage, 211, 116660. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116660. |
[69] |
McCleery, J. P., Akshoomoff, N., Dobkins, K. R., & Carver, L. J. (2009). Atypical face versus object processing and hemispheric asymmetries in 10-month-old infants at risk for autism. Biological Psychiatry, 66(10),950-957. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.031.
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.031 URL |
[70] |
Meissner, T. W., Prufer, H., Nordt, M., Semmelmann, K., & Weigelt, S. (2018). Development of face detection in preschool children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 42(4), 439-444. http://doi.org/10.1177/0165025417738058.
doi: 10.1177/0165025417738058 URL |
[71] |
Mo, S., Liang, L., Bardikoff, N., & Sabbagh, M. A. (2019). Shifting visual attention to social and non-social stimuli in autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 65, 56-64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2019.05.006.
doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2019.05.006 URL |
[72] | Moore, A., Wozniak, M., Yousef, A., Barnes, C. C., Cha, D., Courchesne, E., & Pierce, K. (2018). The geometric preference subtype in ASD: Identifying a consistent, early- emerging phenomenon through eye tracking. Molecular Autism, 9(1), 19. http://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-018-0202-z. |
[73] |
Morton, J. J. L., & Johnson, M. H. (1991). CONSPEC and CONLERN: A two-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review, 98(2), 164-181. http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.164.
URL pmid: 2047512 |
[74] | Nakano, T., Tanaka, K., Endo, Y., Yamane, Y., Yamamoto, T., Nakano, Y., ... Kitazawa, S. (2010). Atypical gaze patterns in children and adults with autism spectrum disorders dissociated from developmental changes in gaze behaviour. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 277 (1696),2935-2943. http://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.0587. |
[75] |
Neuhaus, E., Jones, E. J. H., Barnes, K., Sterling, L., Estes, A., Munson, J., ... Webb, S. J. (2016). The relationship between early neural responses to emotional faces at age 3 and later autism and anxiety symptoms in adolescents with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(7), 2450-2463. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2780-y.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2780-y URL pmid: 27055415 |
[76] | Noris, B., Nadel, J., Barker, M., Hadjikhani, N., & Billard, A. (2012). Investigating gaze of children with ASD in naturalistic settings. PLoS One, 7(9). http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0044144. |
[77] | Ozonoff, S., Iosif, A.-M., Baguio, F., Cook, I. C., Hill, M. M., Hutman, T., ... Young, G. S. (2010). A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(3), 256-266. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2009.11.009. |
[78] |
Ozonoff, S., Macari, S., Young, G. S., Goldring, S., Thompson, M., & Rogers, S. J. (2008). Atypical object exploration at 12 months of age is associated with autism in a prospective sample. Autism, 12(5), 457-472. http://doi.org/10.1177/1362361308096402.
doi: 10.1177/1362361308096402 URL pmid: 18805942 |
[79] |
Parish-Morris, J., Chevallier, C., Tonge, N., Letzen, J., Pandey, J., & Schultz, R. T. (2013). Visual attention to dynamic faces and objects is linked to face processing skills: A combined study of children with autism and controls. Frontiers in Psychology, 4,185. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00185.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00185 URL pmid: 23596436 |
[80] |
Peltola, M. J., Yrttiaho, S., & Leppanen, J. M. (2018). Infants' attention bias to faces as an early marker of social development. Developmental Science, 21(6),e12687. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12687.
doi: 10.1111/desc.2018.21.issue-6 URL |
[81] |
Pierce, K., Marinero, S., Hazin, R., McKenna, B., Barnes, C. C., & Malige, A. (2016). Eye tracking reveals abnormal visual preference for geometric images as an early biomarker of an autism spectrum disorder subtype associated with increased symptom severity. Biological Psychiatry, 79(8),657-666. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.03.032.
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.03.032 URL |
[82] |
Powell, L. J., Deen, B., & Saxe, R. (2018). Using individual functional channels of interest to study cortical development with fNIRS. Developmental Science, 21(4),e12595. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12595.
doi: 10.1111/desc.2018.21.issue-4 URL |
[83] |
Powell, L. J., Kosakowski, H. L., & Saxe, R. (2018). Social origins of cortical face areas. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(9), 752-763. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.06.009.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.06.009 URL |
[84] |
Reid, V. M., Dunn, K., Young, R. J., Amu, J., Donovan, T., & Reissland, N. (2017). The human fetus preferentially engages with face-like visual stimuli. Current Biology, 27(12), 1825-1828. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.044.
doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.044 URL |
[85] |
Reynolds, G. D. (2015). Infant visual attention and object recognition. Behavioural Brain Research, 285,34-43. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.01.015.
doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.01.015 URL |
[86] |
Reynolds, G. D., & Roth, K. C. (2018). The development of attentional biases for faces in infancy: A developmental systems perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 9,222. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00222.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00222 URL pmid: 29541043 |
[87] |
Riby, D. M., & Hancock, P. J. B. (2008). Do faces capture the attention of individuals with Williams syndrome or autism? Evidence from tracking eye movements. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(3), 421-431. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-008-0641-z.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0641-z URL |
[88] |
Riby, D. M., & Hancock, P. J. B. (2009). Looking at movies and cartoons: Eye-tracking evidence from Williams syndrome and autism. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 53(2), 169-181. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2788.2008.01142.x.
doi: 10.1111/jir.2009.53.issue-2 URL |
[89] |
Rozga, A., Hutman, T., Young, G. S., Rogers, S. J., Ozonoff, S., Dapretto, M., & Sigman, M. (2011). Behavioral profiles of affected and unaffected siblings of children with autism: Contribution of measures of mother-infant interaction and nonverbal communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(3), 287-301. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1051-6.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1051-6 URL |
[90] |
Sabatos-Devito, M., Schipul, S. E., Bulluck, J. C., Belger, A., & Baranek, G. T. (2016). Eye tracking reveals impaired attentional disengagement associated with sensory response patterns in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(4), 1319-1333. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2681-5.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2681-5 URL pmid: 26816345 |
[91] | Sasson, N. J., Elison, J. T., Turner-Brown, L., Dichter, G. S., & Bodfish, J. W. (2011). Brief report: Circumscribed attention in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(2), 242-247. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1038-3. |
[92] |
Sasson, N. J., & Touchstone, E. W. (2013). Visual attention to competing social and object images by preschool children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(3), 584-592. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1910-z.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1910-z URL |
[93] |
Sasson, N. J., Turner-Brown, L., Holtzclaw, T. N., Lam, K. S. L., & Bodfish, J. W. (2008). Children with autism demonstrate circumscribed attention during passive viewing of complex social and nonsocial picture arrays. Autism Research, 1(1), 31-42. http://doi.org/10.1002/aur.4.
doi: 10.1002/aur.4 URL pmid: 19360648 |
[94] |
Scherf, K. S., Behrmann, M., Humphreys, K., & Luna, B. (2007). Visual category-selectivity for faces, places and objects emerges along different developmental trajectories. Developmental Science, 10(4),F15-F30 http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00595.x.
doi: 10.1111/desc.2007.10.issue-4 URL |
[95] |
Shah, P., Gaule, A., Bird, G., & Cook, R. (2013). Robust orienting to protofacial stimuli in autism. Current Biology, 23(24), R1087-R1088. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.10.034.
doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.10.034 URL |
[96] |
Shah, P., Happe, F., Sowden, S., Cook, R. J., & Bird, G. (2015). Orienting toward face-like stimuli in early childhood. Child Development, 86(6), 1693-1700. http://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12441.
doi: 10.1111/cdev.2015.86.issue-6 URL |
[97] |
Shephard, E., Milosavljevic, B., Mason, L., Elsabbagh, M., Tye, C., Gliga, T., ... Johnson, M. H. (2020). Neural and behavioural indices of face processing in siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD): A longitudinal study from infancy to mid-childhood. Cortex, 127,162-179. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2020.02.008.
doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.02.008 URL |
[98] |
Shic, F., Wang, Q., Macari, S., & Chawarska, K. (2020). The role of limited salience of speech in selective attention to faces in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61(4), 459-469. http://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13118.
doi: 10.1111/jcpp.v61.4 URL |
[99] |
Shultz, S., Klin, A., & Jones, W. (2018). Neonatal transitions in social behavior and their implications for autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(5), 452-469. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.02.012.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.02.012 URL |
[100] | Simion, F., & Giorgio, E. D. (2015). Face perception and processing in early infancy: Inborn predispositions and developmental changes. Frontiers in Psychology, 6,969. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00969. |
[101] |
Simion, F., Valenza, E., Cassia, V. M., Turati, C., & Umilta, C. (2002). Newborns' preference for up-down asymmetrical configurations. Developmental Science, 5(4), 427-434. http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00237.
doi: 10.1111/desc.2002.5.issue-4 URL |
[102] | Simion, F., Valenza, E., Umiltà, C., & Barba, B. D (1998). Preferential orienting to faces in newborns: A temporal- nasal asymmetry. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 24(5), 1399-1405. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.24.5.1399. |
[103] |
Simpson, E. A., Maylott, S. E., Leonard, K., Lazo, R. J., & Jakobsen, K. V. (2019). Face detection in infants and adults: Effects of orientation and color. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 186, 17-32. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.05.001.
doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.05.001 URL |
[104] |
Sumner, E., Leonard, H. C., & Hill, E. L. (2018). Comparing attention to socially-relevant stimuli in autism spectrum disorder and developmental coordination disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46(8), 1717-1729. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-017-0393-3.
doi: 10.1007/s10802-017-0393-3 URL |
[105] |
Sysoeva, O. V., Constantino, J. N., & Anokhin, A. P. (2018). Event-related potential (ERP) correlates of face processing in verbal children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) and their first-degree relatives: A family study. Molecular Autism, 9(1),41. http://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-018-0220-x.
doi: 10.1186/s13229-018-0220-x URL |
[106] |
Thye, M. D., Bednarz, H. M., Herringshaw, A. J., Sartin, E. B., & Kana, R. K. (2017). The impact of atypical sensory processing on social impairments in autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 29,151-167. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.04.010.
doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.04.010 URL |
[107] |
Tsang, T., Johnson, S. P., Jeste, S. S., & Dapretto, M. (2019). Social complexity and the early social environment affect visual social attention to faces. Autism Research, 12(3), 445-457. http://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2060.
doi: 10.1002/aur.2019.12.issue-3 URL |
[108] |
Turati, C., Simion, F., Milani, I., & Umilta, C. (2002). Newborns' preference for faces: What is crucial? Developmental Psychology, 38(6), 875-882. http://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.6.875.
doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.38.6.875 URL |
[109] | Unruh, K. E., Sasson, N. J., Shafer, R. L., Whitten, A., Miller, S., Turner-Brown, L., & Bodfish, J. W. (2016). Social orienting and attention is influenced by the presence of competing nonsocial information in adolescents with autism. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 10,586. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2016.00586. |
[110] |
Valenza, E., Simion, F., Cassia, V. M., & Umilta, C. (1996). Face preference at birth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22(4), 892-903. http://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.22.4.892.
doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.22.4.892 URL |
[111] |
van Renswoude, D. R., Visser, I., Raijmakers, M. E. J., Tsang, T., & Johnson, S. P. (2019). Real-world scene perception in infants: What factors guide attention allocation? Infancy, 24(5), 693-717. http://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12308.
doi: 10.1111/infa.12308 URL pmid: 32677279 |
[112] |
Vinken, K., & Vogels, R. (2019). A behavioral face preference deficit in a monkey with an incomplete face patch system. Neuroimage, 189,415-424. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.01.043.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.01.043 URL |
[113] |
Wan, G., Kong, X., Sun, B., Yu, S., Tu, Y., Park, J., ... Kong, J. (2019). Applying eye tracking to identify autism spectrum disorder in children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49(1), 209-215. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3690-y.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3690-y URL |
[114] |
Wan, M. W., Green, J., Elsabbagh, M., Johnson, M. H., Charman, T., & Plummer, F. (2013). Quality of interaction between at-risk Infants and caregiver at 12-15 months is associated with 3-year autism outcome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(7), 763-771. http://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12032.
doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12032 URL |
[115] |
Wang, Q., Wall, C. A., Barney, E., Bradshaw, J., Macari, S., Chawarska, K., & Shic, F. (2020). Promoting social attention in 3-year-olds with ASD through gaze-contingent eye tracking. Autism Research, 13(1), 61-73. http://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2199.
doi: 10.1002/aur.v13.1 URL |
[116] |
Wang, S., Jiang, M., Duchesne, X. M., Laugeson, E. A., Kennedy, D. P., Adolphs, R., & Zhao, Q. (2015). Atypical visual saliency in autism spectrum disorder quantified through model-based eye tracking. Neuron, 88(3),604-616. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.042.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.042 URL |
[117] |
Webb, S. J., Neuhaus, E., & Faja, S. (2017). Face perception and learning in autism spectrum disorders. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(5), 970-986. http://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1151059.
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1151059 URL |
[118] |
Wilson, C. E., Brock, J., & Palermo, R. (2010). Attention to social stimuli and facial identity recognition skills in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 54(12), 1104-1115. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01340.x.
doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01340.x URL |
[119] |
Yamashiro, A., Sorcinelli, A., Rahman, T., Elbogen, R., Curtin, S., & Vouloumanos, A. (2019). Shifting preferences for primate faces in neurotypical infants and infants later diagnosed with ASD. Autism Research, 12(2), 249-262. http://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2043.
doi: 10.1002/aur.2043 URL pmid: 30561908 |
[120] |
Young, G. S., Merin, N., Rogers, S. J., & Ozonoff, S. (2009). Gaze behavior and affect at 6 months: Predicting clinical outcomes and language development in typically developing infants and infants at risk for autism. Developmental Science, 12(5), 798-814. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00833.x.
doi: 10.1111/desc.2009.12.issue-5 URL |
[121] |
Zhang, L., Yan, G., Zhou, L., Lan, Z., & Benson, V. (2020). The influence of irrelevant visual distractors on eye movement control in Chinese children with autism spectrum disorder: Evidence from the remote distractor paradigm. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50(2), 500-512. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04271-y.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-04271-y URL pmid: 31673908 |
[122] | Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S. W., Rogers, T., Roberts, W., Brian, J., & Szatmari, P. (2005). Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23(2-3),143-152. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.05.001. |
[1] | 刘敏, 胡洋, 刘巧云. 孤独症儿童潜在早期识别标志——发声异常及原因探析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(3): 635-647. |
[2] | 王琳, 王志丹, 王泓婧. 孤独症儿童动作发展障碍的神经机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(7): 1239-1250. |
[3] | 毕小彬, 范晓壮, 米文丽, 贺荟中. 高风险婴儿前瞻性纵向研究与孤独症谱系障碍早期识别[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(3): 443-455. |
[4] | 荆伟, 王庭照. 双通路理论视角下孤独症谱系障碍者的视线加工障碍[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(3): 508-521. |
[5] | 张青, 董书阳, 王争艳. 婴幼儿自我调节的发展机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(2): 230-241. |
[6] | 赵晓宁, 胡金生, 李松泽, 刘西, 刘琼阳, 吴娜. 基于眼动研究的孤独症谱系障碍早期预测[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(2): 301-311. |
[7] | 诸彦含, 赵玉兰, 周意勇, 吴江. 组织中的韧性:基于心理路径和系统路径的保护性资源建构[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(2): 357-369. |
[8] | 白晓宇, TawandaS.Mutusva, 祝卓宏. PEAK关系训练系统:孤独症语言障碍康复的新方法[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(11): 1896-1905. |
[9] | 康一奇, 种霞, 吴南. 自闭症早期预警:联合注意和共情的发生发展及影响因素[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(7): 1223-1231. |
[10] | 李涛涛, 胡金生, 王琦, 李骋诗, 李松泽, 何建青, 李辰洋, 刘淑清. 孤独症谱系障碍者的视听时间整合 *[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(6): 1031-1040. |
[11] | 巨兴达, 宋伟, 徐婧. CHRM3基因与孤独症谱系障碍[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(12): 2141-2152. |
[12] | 王分分, 祝卓宏. 言语行为的关系框架理论视角: 孤独症谱系障碍的新探索[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(8): 1321-1326. |
[13] | 王琦;胡金生;李骋诗;李松泽. 孤独症谱系障碍者的情绪韵律识别[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(9): 1377-1390. |
[14] | 李松泽; 胡金生; 李骋诗; 王琦; 刘淑清; 康晓东; 崔丽 . 孤独症谱系障碍者的视觉−空间工作记忆缺陷及脑机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016, 24(7): 1050-1064. |
[15] | 徐晓惠;李晶;朱莉琪. 婴幼儿对合作行为共享性特征的理解[J]. 心理科学进展, 2014, 22(9): 1404-1412. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||