心理科学进展 ›› 2021, Vol. 29 ›› Issue (1): 112-122.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2021.00112
收稿日期:
2020-06-09
出版日期:
2021-01-15
发布日期:
2020-11-23
通讯作者:
隋雪
E-mail:suixue88@163.com
基金资助:
Received:
2020-06-09
Online:
2021-01-15
Published:
2020-11-23
Contact:
SUI Xue
E-mail:suixue88@163.com
摘要:
词汇识别与记忆受到词汇之间语义联想关系的影响。鉴于自由联想方法建立词汇语义联想关系的局限性, 研究者们提出用共现联想法建立词汇之间的语义联想关系。本文对词汇共现频率视角下的语义联想效应及其神经机制的研究进行分析发现, 语义联想的丰富程度和关联程度影响词汇的加工; 与少语义联想词相比, 多语义联想词导致P200波幅增大和N400波幅减小; 语义联想加工涉及的脑区主要是额上回和左内侧颞叶区域。未来研究需要探讨自由联想法和共现联想法建立语义联想关系的差异, 基于共现联想法进一步地探究语义联想的行为规律和神经机制, 并从共现联想的角度开展汉语语义联想效应及神经机制的研究。
中图分类号:
李雨桐, 隋雪. (2021). 词汇共现频率视角下语义联想效应及其神经机制. 心理科学进展 , 29(1), 112-122.
LI Yutong, SUI Xue. (2021). Semantic association effect and its neural mechanism from the perspective of lexical co-occurrence frequency. Advances in Psychological Science, 29(1), 112-122.
[1] |
Baror, S., & Bar, M. (2016). Associative activation and its relation to exploration and exploitation in the brain. Psychological Science, 27(6), 776-789.
doi: 10.1177/0956797616634487 URL pmid: 27122295 |
[2] |
Bentin, S., McCarthy, G., & Wood, C. C. (1985). Event- related potentials, lexical decision and semantic priming. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 60(4), 343-355.
doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(85)90008-2 URL pmid: 2579801 |
[3] |
Biemann, C., & Riedl, M. (2013). Text: Now in 2D! A framework for lexical expansion with contextual similarity. Journal of Language Modelling, 1(1), 55-95.
doi: 10.15398/jlm.v1i1.60 URL |
[4] |
Braun, M., Jacobs, A. M., Hahne, A., Ricker, B., Hofmann, M. J., & Hutzler, F. (2006). Model-generated lexical activity predicts graded ERP amplitudes in lexical decision. Brain Research, 1073-1074, 431-439.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.12.078 URL pmid: 16464440 |
[5] |
Braun, M., Jacobs, A. M., Richlan, F., Hawelka, S., Hutzler, F., & Kronbichler, M. (2015). Many neighbors are not silent. fMRI evidence for global lexical activity in visual word recognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 423-423.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00423 URL pmid: 26257634 |
[6] |
Braun, M., Kronbichler, M., Richlan, F., Hawelka, S., Hutzler, F., & Jacobs, A. M. (2019). A model-guided dissociation between subcortical and cortical contributions to word recognition. Scientific Reports, 9, 4506.
doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-41011-9 URL pmid: 30872701 |
[7] |
Coane, J. H., McBride, D. M., Termonen, M. -L., & Cutting, J. C. (2016). Categorical and associative relations increase false memory relative to purely associative relations. Memory & cognition, 44(1), 37-49.
doi: 10.3758/s13421-015-0543-1 URL pmid: 26250805 |
[8] |
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108(1), 204-256.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.1.204 URL pmid: 11212628 |
[9] |
Dambacher, M., Kliegl, R., Hofmann, M. J., & Jacobs, A. M. (2006). Frequency and predictability effects on event-related potentials during reading. Brain Research, 1084(1), 89-103.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.02.010 URL pmid: 16545344 |
[10] |
Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58(1), 17-22.
doi: 10.1037/h0046671 URL pmid: 13664879 |
[11] |
Diana, R. A., Yonelinas, A. P., & Ranganath, C. (2007). Imaging recollection and familiarity in the medial temporal lobe: A three-component model. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(9), 379-386.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.001 URL |
[12] | Dunning, T. (1993). Accurate methods for the statistics of surprise and coincidence. Computational Linguistics, 19(1), 61-74. |
[13] |
Eichenbaum, H., Yonelinas, A. P., & Ranganath, C. (2007). The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 30(1), 123-152.
doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094328 URL |
[14] | Ellis, N. C., Frey, E., & Jalkanen, I. (2009). The psycholinguistic reality of collocation and semantic prosody (1): Lexical access. In U. Rõmer, & R. Schulze (Eds.), Studies in Corpus Linguistics, Exploring the Lexis-Grammar Interface(pp. 89-114). Amsterdam, Holand: John Benjamins. |
[15] | Franke, N., Roelke, A., Radach, R. R., & Hofmann, M. J. (2017, July). After braking comes hasting: reversed effects of indirect associations in 2nd and 4th graders. Paper presented at the meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, London, England. |
[16] |
Gallo, D. A. (2010). False memories and fantastic beliefs: 15 years of the DRM illusion. Memory & Cognition, 38(7), 833-848.
doi: 10.3758/MC.38.7.833 URL pmid: 20921097 |
[17] |
Geng, H., Qi, Y., Li, Y., Fan, S., Wu, Y., & Zhu, Y. (2007). Neurophysiological correlates of memory illusion in both encoding and retrieval phases. Brain Research, 1136, 154-168.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.12.027 URL pmid: 17239833 |
[18] |
Grainger, J., & Jacobs, A. M. (1994). A dual read-out model of word context effects in letter perception: Further investigations of the word superiority effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(6), 1158-1176.
doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.20.6.1158 URL |
[19] | Goldhahn, D., Eckart, T., Quasthoff, U. (2012). Building large monolingual dictionaries at the Leipzig Corpora Collection_ From 100 to 200 Languages. In N. Calzolari, K. Choukri, T. Declerck, M. Ugur Dogan, B. Maegaard, J. Mariani, J. Odijk, & S. Piperidis (Eds.), Proceedings of the Eighth International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC-2012) (pp. 3196-3200). European Language Resources Association (ELRA). |
[20] |
Grainger, J., & Jacobs, A. M. (1996). Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: A multiple read-out model. Psychological Review, 103(3), 518-565.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.103.3.518 URL pmid: 8759046 |
[21] |
Hawelka, S., Schuster, S., Gagl, B., & Hutzler, F. (2013). Beyond single syllables: The effect of first syllable frequency and orthographic similarity on eye movements during silent reading. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28(8), 1134-1153.
doi: 10.1080/01690965.2012.696665 URL |
[22] | Hebb, D. O. (Eds). (1949). The organization of behavior: a neuropsychological theory. New York: Wiley. |
[23] |
Hofmann, M. J., Biemann, C., Westbury, C., Murusidze, M., Conrad, M., & Jacobs, A. M. (2018). Simple co-occurrence statistics reproducibly predict association ratings. Cognitive Science, 42(7), 2287-2312.
doi: 10.1111/cogs.12662 URL pmid: 30098213 |
[24] |
Hofmann, M. J., & Jacobs, A. M. (2014). Interactive activation and competition models and semantic context: From behavioral to brain data. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 46, 85-104.
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.06.011 URL pmid: 24992217 |
[25] |
Hofmann, M. J., Kuchinke, L., Biemann, C., Tamm, S., & Jacobs, A. M. (2011). Remembering words in context as predicted by an associative read-out model. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 11.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00011 URL pmid: 21713185 |
[26] | Hofmann, M. J., Kuchinke, L., Tamm, S., Võ, M. L. H., & Jacobs, A. M. (2009). Affective processing within 1/10th of a second: High arousal is necessary for early facilitative processing of negative but not positive words. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9(4), 389-397. |
[27] |
Jared, D., Jouravlev, O., & Joanisse, M. F. (2017). The effect of semantic transparency on the processing of morphologically derived words: Evidence from decision latencies and event- related potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 43(3), 422-450.
doi: 10.1037/xlm0000316 URL |
[28] |
Jou, J., Arredondo, M. L., Li, C., Escamilla, E. E., & Zuniga, R. (2017). The effects of increasing semantic-associate list length on the Deese-Roediger-McDermott false recognition memory: Dual false-memory process in retrieval from sub- and supraspan lists. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(10), 2076-2093.
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1222446 URL pmid: 27606720 |
[29] |
Jou, J., Escamilla, E. E., Arredondo, M. L., Pena, L., Zuniga, R., Perez, M., & Garcia, C. (2018). The role of decision criterion in the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) false recognition memory: False memory falls and rises as a function of restriction on criterion setting. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(2), 499-521.
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1256416 URL pmid: 27808007 |
[30] |
Kuchinke, L., Fritzemeier, S., Hofmann, M. J., & Jacobs, A. M. (2013). Neural correlates of episodic memory: Associative memory and confidence drive hippocampus activations. Behavioural Brain Research, 254, 92-101.
doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.04.035 URL |
[31] |
Li, B. B., Taylor, J. R., Wang, W., Gao, C. J., & Guo, C. Y. (2017). Electrophysiological signals associated with fluency of different levels of processing reveal multiple contributions to recognition memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 53, 1-13.
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2017.05.001 URL pmid: 28558307 |
[32] |
Lothane, H. Z. (2018). Free association as the foundation of the psychoanalytic method and psychoanalysis as a historical science. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 38(6), 416-434.
doi: 10.1080/07351690.2018.1480225 URL |
[33] |
Lucas, M. (2000). Semantic priming without association: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7(4), 618-630.
doi: 10.3758/bf03212999 URL pmid: 11206202 |
[34] |
Mandera, P., Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2017). Explaining human performance in psycholinguistic tasks with models of semantic similarity based on prediction and counting: A review and empirical validation. Journal of Memory and Language, 92, 57-78.
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2016.04.001 URL |
[35] | Mcclelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception, part I: An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88(88), 580-596. |
[36] |
Meade, G., Grainger, J., & Holcomb, P. J. (2019). Task modulates ERP effects of orthographic neighborhood for pseudowords but not words. Neuropsychologia, 129, 385-396.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.02.014 URL pmid: 30797831 |
[37] |
Meade, G., Midgley, K. J., Dijkstra, T., & Holcomb, P. J. (2018). Cross-language neighborhood effects in learners indicative of an integrated lexicon. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 30(1), 70-85.
doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01184 URL pmid: 28880767 |
[38] |
Midgley, K. J., Holcomb, P. J., & Grainger, J. (2009). Masked repetition and translation priming in second language learners: A window on the time-course of form and meaning activation using ERPs. Psychophysiology, 46(3), 551-565.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00784.x URL pmid: 19298629 |
[39] | Mikolov, T., Chen, K., Corrado, G., & Dean, J. (2013, May). Efficient estimation of word representations in vector space. Paper presented at the meeting of the international conference on learning representations, Scottsdale, USA. |
[40] |
Molinaro, N., Conrad, M., Barber, H. A., & Carreiras, M. (2010). On the functional nature of the N400: Contrasting effects related to visual word recognition and contextual semantic integration. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(1), 1-7.
doi: 10.1080/17588920903373952 URL pmid: 24168241 |
[41] |
Müller, O., Duñabeitia, J. A., & Carreiras, M. (2010). Orthographic and associative neighborhood density effects: What is shared, what is different? Psychophysiology, 47(3), 455-466.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00960.x URL pmid: 20102534 |
[42] | Namaziandost, E., Shafiee, S., & Rasooyar, H. (2018). Paradigmatic relations and syntagmatic relations: Are they useful in learning grammatical structures? Journal of Applied Linguistics and Language Research, 5(5), 20-34. |
[43] |
Otgaar, H., Moldoveanu, G., Wang, J. Q., & Howe, M. L. (2017). Exploring the consequences of nonbelieved memories in the DRM paradigm. Memory, 25(7), 922-933.
doi: 10.1080/09658211.2016.1272701 URL pmid: 28029065 |
[44] |
Otgaar, H., Muris, P., Howe, M. L., & Merckelbach, H. (2017). What drives false memories in psychopathology? A case for associative activation. Clinical Psychological Science, 5(6), 1048-1069.
doi: 10.1177/2167702617724424 URL pmid: 29170722 |
[45] |
Park, J. L., & Donaldson, D. I. (2016). Investigating the relationship between implicit and explicit memory: Evidence that masked repetition priming speeds the onset of recollection. NeuroImage, 139, 8-16.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.06.013 URL |
[46] |
Perry, C., Ziegler, J. C., & Zorzi, M. (2007). Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: The CDP+ model of reading aloud. Psychological Review, 114(2), 273-315.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.273 URL pmid: 17500628 |
[47] |
Perry, C., Ziegler, J. C., & Zorzi, M. (2010). Beyond single syllables: Large-scale modeling of reading aloud with the connectionist dual process (CDP++) model. Cognitive Psychology, 61(2), 106-151.
doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.04.001 URL |
[48] |
Pleger, B., & Timmann, D. (2018). The role of the human cerebellum in linguistic prediction, word generation and verbal working memory: Evidence from brain imaging, non-invasive cerebellar stimulation and lesion studies. Neuropsychologia, 115, 204-210.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.03.012 URL pmid: 29530801 |
[49] |
Qu, Q. Q., Zhang, Q. F., & Damian, M. F. (2016). Tracking the time course of lexical access in orthographic production: An event-related potential study of word frequency effects in written picture naming. Brain and Language, 159, 118-126.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.06.008 URL pmid: 27393929 |
[50] | Rapp, R. (2002, September). The computation of word associations: comparing syntagmatic and paradigmatic approaches. Paper presented at the meeting of the 19th international conference on Computational linguistics, Taipei, China. |
[51] |
Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(4), 803-814.
doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.803 URL |
[52] |
Roelke, A., Franke, N., Biemann, C., Radach, R., Jacobs, A. M., & Hofmann, M. J. (2018). A novel co-occurrence-based approach to predict pure associative and semantic priming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(4), 1488-1493.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1453-6 URL pmid: 29546666 |
[53] |
Ross, D. A., Sadil, P., Wilson, D. M., & Cowell, R. A. (2017). Hippocampal engagement during recall depends on memory content. Cerebral Cortex, 28(8), 2685-2698.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx147 URL pmid: 28666344 |
[54] |
Schachter, J. (2018). Free association: From Freud to current use—the effects of training analysis on the use of free association. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 38(6), 457-467.
doi: 10.1080/07351690.2018.1480231 URL |
[55] |
Shin, J. -E., Suh, E. M., Eom, K., & Kim, H. S. (2018). What does “happiness” prompt in your mind? Culture, word choice, and experienced happiness. Journal of Happiness Studies, 19(3), 649-662.
doi: 10.1007/s10902-016-9836-8 URL |
[56] |
Siew, C. S. Q., & Vitevitch, M. S. (2016). Spoken word recognition and serial recall of words from components in the phonological network. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 42(3), 394-410.
doi: 10.1037/xlm0000139 URL |
[57] |
Smith, C. N., Wixted, J. T., & Squire, L. R. (2011). The hippocampus supports both recollection and familiarity when memories are strong. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(44), 15693-15702.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3438-11.2011 URL pmid: 22049412 |
[58] |
Stoodley, C. J., & Schmahmann, J. D. (2009). Functional topography in the human cerebellum: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuroimage, 44(2), 489-501.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.039 URL |
[59] |
Strozak, P., Abedzadeh, D., & Curran, T. (2016). Separating the FN400 and N400 potentials across recognition memory experiments. Brain Research, 1635, 41-60.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.01.015 URL pmid: 26776478 |
[60] |
Stuellein, N., Radach, R. R., Jacobs, A. M., & Hofmann, M. J. (2016). No one way ticket from orthography to semantics in recognition memory: N400 and P200 effects of associations. Brain Research, 1639, 88-98.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.02.029 URL pmid: 26921776 |
[61] | Trevor, A. H. (Eds).(2008). The psychology of language, from data to theory. Hove and New York: Psychology Press. |
[62] |
Voss, J. L., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). FN400 potentials are functionally identical to N400 potentials and reflect semantic processing during recognition testing. Psychophysiology, 48(4), 532-546.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01085.x URL |
[63] |
Wagenmakers, E. -J., Ratcliff, R., Gomez, P., & Mckoon, G. (2008). A diffusion model account of criterion shifts in the lexical decision task. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(1), 140-159.
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.04.006 URL |
[64] |
Wang, J. Q., Otgaar, H., Howe, M. L., & Zhou, C. (2019). A self-reference false memory effect in the DRM paradigm: Evidence from eastern and western samples. Memory & Cognition, 47(1), 76-86.
doi: 10.3758/s13421-018-0851-3 URL pmid: 30141171 |
[65] |
Wixted, J. T., Goldinger, S. D., Squire, L. R., Kuhn, J. R., Papesh, M. H., Smith, K. A., ... Steinmetz, P. N. (2018). Coding of episodic memory in the human hippocampus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(5), 1093-1098.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1716443115 URL |
[66] |
Wixted, J. T., & Squire, L. R. (2010). The role of the human hippocampus in familiarity-based and recollection-based recognition memory. Behavioural Brain Research, 215, 197-208.
doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.04.020 URL pmid: 20412819 |
[67] |
Woods, J. A., & Dewhurst, S. A. (2019). Putting false memories into context: The effects of odour contexts on correct and false recall. Memory, 27(3), 379-386.
doi: 10.1080/09658211.2018.1512632 URL pmid: 30139301 |
[68] | Zannino, G. D., Perri, R., Teghil, A., Caltagirone, C., & Carlesimo, G. A. (2018). Associative agreement as a predictor of naming ability in alzheimer's disease: A case for the semantic nature of associative links. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 11(261), 1-11. |
[69] |
Zhang, W. W., Gross, J., & Hayne, H. (2017). The effect of mood on false memory for emotional DRM word lists. Cognition and Emotion, 31(3), 526-537.
doi: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1138930 URL pmid: 26823096 |
[1] | 冯杰, 徐娟, 伍新春. 视觉经验缺失对盲人听觉词汇识别的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2021, 29(12): 2131-2146. |
[2] | 袁娟娟, 杨炀, 郑志伟, 刘萍萍. 图文位置和熟悉性对儿童词汇识别的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2019, 27(suppl.): 73-73. |
[3] | 滑慧敏, 顾俊娟, 林楠, 李兴珊. 视觉词汇识别中的字符位置编码[J]. 心理科学进展, 2017, 25(7): 1132-1138. |
[4] | 白学军;张慢慢;臧传丽;李馨;陈璐;闫国利. 词边界信息在中文词汇学习与识别中的作用:眼动研究的证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2014, 22(1): 1-8. |
[5] | 任桂琴;韩玉昌;刘颖. 句子语境中汉语词汇识别的即时加工过程[J]. 心理科学进展, 2012, 20(4): 493-503. |
[6] | 李馨;白学军;闫国利;臧传丽;梁菲菲. 空格在文本阅读中的作用[J]. 心理科学进展, 2010, 18(9): 1377-1385. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||