心理科学进展 ›› 2022, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (8): 1794-1803.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.01794
收稿日期:
2021-06-09
出版日期:
2022-08-15
发布日期:
2022-06-23
通讯作者:
周蕾
E-mail:zhoulei@gdut.edu.cn
基金资助:
LIN Jiao-Min1, LI Ai-Mei2, ZHOU Ya-Ran2, HE Jun-Hong1, ZHOU Lei1()
Received:
2021-06-09
Online:
2022-08-15
Published:
2022-06-23
Contact:
ZHOU Lei
E-mail:zhoulei@gdut.edu.cn
摘要:
眼动追踪技术因干扰性低、客观性强、获取数据丰富而准确等优点, 被广泛应用于行为决策研究。通过操纵眼动过程, 可使决策结果按照预期的方向改变, 建立决策过程和结果的因果链, 为决策的干预研究提供思路。基于外源性眼动操纵、注视追随操纵这两类研究, 介绍了行为决策领域眼动操纵的基本方法及原理、常见操纵指标、操纵效果, 并分析和讨论了不同操纵类别的优缺点。在实验设计中考虑决策者策略及偏好的差异, 结合计算建模等方法进一步丰富操纵指标和分析方法, 并将该方法的优势拓展到其它领域是眼动操纵研究未来的重要方向。
中图分类号:
林浇敏, 李爱梅, 周雅然, 何军红, 周蕾. (2022). 眼动操纵技术在决策研究中的应用前景:改变决策行为. 心理科学进展 , 30(8), 1794-1803.
LIN Jiao-Min, LI Ai-Mei, ZHOU Ya-Ran, HE Jun-Hong, ZHOU Lei. (2022). The prospect of gaze manipulation technology in decision-making research: Altering decision-making. Advances in Psychological Science, 30(8), 1794-1803.
操纵类别 | 具体方法 | 原理 | 主要操纵的 眼动指标 | 主要研究 领域 | 参考文献 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
外源性眼动操纵 | 外部线索 | 通过运用外部线索引导实验者注视目标选项 | 注视点数量 | 消费决策 | Vriens et al., |
物理特征 | 通过改变选项的物理属性:如显著性(亮度、透明度、颜色等)、标签、尺寸等, 引导实验者注视目标选项 | 注视点数量 | 消费决策 | Peschel et al., Zuschke, | |
呈现时长 | 通过延长选项的呈现时间引导实验者注视目标选项 | 注视时长 | 认知决策 | Shimojo et al., Nittono & Wada, | |
消费决策 | Armel et al., | ||||
跨期决策 | Fisher, | ||||
注视追随 操纵 | 末次注视 | 当实验者的末次注视位于目标选项时, 中断眼动过程 | 末次注视点 | 消费决策 | Liu et al., 2020 |
认知决策 | Morii & Sakagami, | ||||
道德决策 | Ghaffari & Fiedler, | ||||
注视时长 | 当实验者注视选项(维度)达到一定时长时, 中断眼动过程 | 注视时长 | 跨期决策 | Fisher, Liu et al., 2021 | |
风险决策 | Sui et al., | ||||
认知决策 | 亓立东 等, Newell & Le Pelley, Tavares et al., | ||||
道德决策 | Pärnamets et al., |
表1 操纵研究方法总结
操纵类别 | 具体方法 | 原理 | 主要操纵的 眼动指标 | 主要研究 领域 | 参考文献 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
外源性眼动操纵 | 外部线索 | 通过运用外部线索引导实验者注视目标选项 | 注视点数量 | 消费决策 | Vriens et al., |
物理特征 | 通过改变选项的物理属性:如显著性(亮度、透明度、颜色等)、标签、尺寸等, 引导实验者注视目标选项 | 注视点数量 | 消费决策 | Peschel et al., Zuschke, | |
呈现时长 | 通过延长选项的呈现时间引导实验者注视目标选项 | 注视时长 | 认知决策 | Shimojo et al., Nittono & Wada, | |
消费决策 | Armel et al., | ||||
跨期决策 | Fisher, | ||||
注视追随 操纵 | 末次注视 | 当实验者的末次注视位于目标选项时, 中断眼动过程 | 末次注视点 | 消费决策 | Liu et al., 2020 |
认知决策 | Morii & Sakagami, | ||||
道德决策 | Ghaffari & Fiedler, | ||||
注视时长 | 当实验者注视选项(维度)达到一定时长时, 中断眼动过程 | 注视时长 | 跨期决策 | Fisher, Liu et al., 2021 | |
风险决策 | Sui et al., | ||||
认知决策 | 亓立东 等, Newell & Le Pelley, Tavares et al., | ||||
道德决策 | Pärnamets et al., |
[1] | 黄龙, 徐富明, 胡笑羽. (2020). 眼动轨迹匹配法: 一种研究决策过程的新方法. 心理科学进展, 28(9), 1454-1461. |
[2] | 亓立东, 李雨桐, 隋雪. (2021). 注视反映及影响决策偏好的眼动研究. 心理科学, 44(4), 786-792. |
[3] | 魏子晗, 李兴珊. (2015). 决策过程的追踪:基于眼动的证据. 心理科学进展, 23(12), 2029-2041. |
[4] | 张伟, 徐建平. (2016). 应聘者非言语信息对面试官评分的影响: 解释、测量和控制. 心理科学进展, 24(8), 1319-1328. |
[5] | 张阳阳. (2016). 跨期决策是否基于“折扣计算”:来自过程检验的证据(博士学位论文). 中国科学院大学, 北京. |
[6] | 周蕾, 李爱梅, 张磊, 李纾, 梁竹苑. (2019). 风险决策和跨期决策的过程比较: 以确定效应和即刻效应为例. 心理学报, 51(3), 337-352. |
[7] | Armel K. C., Beaumel A., & Rangel A. (2008). Biasing simple choices by manipulating relative visual attention. Judgment and Decision Making, 3(5), 396-403. |
[8] | Atkinson R. C., & Shiffrin R. M. (1968). Human memory:A proposed system and its control processes. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.), Psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 2, pp. 89-195). New York, NY: Academic Press. |
[9] |
Ballco P., de-Magistris T., & Caputo V. (2019). Consumer preferences for nutritional claims: An exploration of attention and choice based on an eye-tracking choice experiment. Food Research International, 116, 37-48.
doi: 10.1016/j.foodres.2018.12.031 URL |
[10] |
Bhatia S., & He L. (2021). Machine-generated theories of human decision-making. Science, 372(6547), 1150-1151.
doi: 10.1126/science.abi7668 URL |
[11] |
Chuk T., Chan A. B., Shimojo S., & Hsiao J. H. (2020). Eye movement analysis with switching hidden Markov models. Behavior Research Methods, 52(3), 1026-1043.
doi: 10.3758/s13428-019-01298-y pmid: 31712999 |
[12] |
Cleland J., Chu J., Lim S., Low J., Low-Beer N., & Kwek T. K. (2020). COVID 19: Designing and conducting an online mini-multiple interview (MMI) in a dynamic landscape. Medical Teacher, 42(7), 776-780.
doi: 10.1080/0142159X.2020.1762851 URL |
[13] |
Cowan N. (1993). Activation, attention, and short-term memory. Memory & Cognition, 21(2), 162-167.
doi: 10.3758/BF03202728 URL |
[14] |
Dannenberg P., Fuchs M., Riedler T., & Wiedemann C. (2020). Digital transition by COVID-19 pandemic? The German food online retail. Tijdschrift Voor Economische En Sociale Geografie, 111(3), 543-560.
doi: 10.1111/tesg.12453 URL |
[15] |
Edwards W. (1954). The theory of decision making. Psychological Bulletin, 51(4), 380-417.
doi: 10.1037/h0053870 URL |
[16] | Eivazi S., & Bednarik R. (2011). Predicting problem- solving behavior and performance levels from visual attention data. In Y. Nakano, C. Conati & T. Bader (Eds.), Proceedings of 2nd Workshop on Eye Gaze in Intelligent Human Machine Interaction (pp. 9-16). California: ACM Press. |
[17] |
Fisher G. (2021). Intertemporal choices are causally influenced by fluctuations in visual attention. Management Science, 67(8), 4961-4981.
doi: 10.1287/mnsc.2020.3732 URL |
[18] |
Ghaffari M., & Fiedler S. (2018). The power of attention: Using eye gaze to predict other-regarding and moral choices. Psychological Science, 29(11), 1878-1889.
doi: 10.1177/0956797618799301 pmid: 30295569 |
[19] |
Gigerenzer G., & Gaissmaier W. (2011). Heuristic decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 451-482.
doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145346 pmid: 21126183 |
[20] |
Glöckner A., & Herbold A.-K. (2011). An eye-tracking study on information processing in risky decisions: Evidence for compensatory strategies based on automatic processes. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 24(1), 71-98.
doi: 10.1002/bdm.684 URL |
[21] | Horstmann N., Ahlgrimm A., & Glöckner A. (2009). How distinct are intuition and deliberation? An eye-tracking analysis of instruction-induced decision modes. Judgment and Decision Making, 4(5), 335-354. |
[22] |
Huddleston P. T., Behe B. K., Driesener C., & Minahan S. (2018). Inside-outside: Using eye-tracking to investigate search-choice processes in the retail environment. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 43, 85-93.
doi: 10.1016/j.jretconser.2018.03.006 URL |
[23] |
Jones R. E., & Abdelfattah K. R. (2020). Virtual interviews in the era of COVID-19: A primer for applicants. Journal of Surgical Education, 77(4), 733-734.
doi: 10.1016/j.jsurg.2020.03.020 URL |
[24] | Khaw M. W., Li Z., & Woodford M. (2018). Temporal discounting and search habits: Evidence for a task- dependent relationship. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(2102). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02102. |
[25] |
Krajbich I., Armel C., & Rangel A. (2010). Visual fixations and the computation and comparison of value in simple choice. Nature Neuroscience, 13(10), 1292-1298.
doi: 10.1038/nn.2635 pmid: 20835253 |
[26] |
Krajbich I., & Rangel A. (2011). Multialternative drift- diffusion model predicts the relationship between visual fixations and choice in value-based decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(33), 13852-13857.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1101328108 pmid: 21808009 |
[27] | Krol M., & Krol M. (2017). A novel approach to studying strategic decisions with eye-tracking and machine learning. Judgment and Decision Making, 12(6), 596. |
[28] | Kunar M. A., Watson D. G., Tsetsos K., & Chater N. (2017). The influence of attention on value integration. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 79(6), 1615-1627. |
[29] |
Lahey J. N., & Oxley D. (2016). The power of eye tracking in economics experiments. American Economic Review, 106(5), 309-313.
doi: 10.1257/aer.p20161009 URL |
[30] |
Liu H. Z., Lyu X. K., Wei Z. H., Mo W. L., Luo J. R., & Su X. Y. (2021). Exploiting the dynamics of eye gaze to bias intertemporal choice. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 34(3), 419-431.
doi: 10.1002/bdm.2219 URL |
[31] | Liu H. Z., Zhou Y. B., Wei Z. H., & Jiang C. M. (2020). The power of last fixation: Biasing simple choices by gaze- contingent manipulation. Acta Psychologica, 208, 103106. |
[32] |
Milosavljevic M., Navalpakkam V., Koch C., & Rangel A. (2012). Relative visual saliency differences induce sizable bias in consumer choice. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22(1), 67-74.
doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2011.10.002 URL |
[33] | Morii M., & Sakagami T. (2015). The effect of gaze- contingent stimulus elimination on preference judgments. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(1351). |
[34] |
Newell B. R., & Le Pelley M. E. (2018). Perceptual but not complex moral judgments can be biased by exploiting the dynamics of eye-gaze. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(3), 409-417.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000386 URL |
[35] |
Nittono H., & Wada Y. (2009). Gaze shifts do not affect preference judgments of graphic patterns. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 109(1), 79-94.
doi: 10.2466/pms.109.1.79-94 URL |
[36] |
Noton D., & Stark L. (1971). Scanpaths in saccadic eye movements while viewing and recognizing patterns. Vision Research, 11(9), 929-942.
pmid: 5133265 |
[37] |
Orquin J. L., Chrobot N., & Grunert K. G. (2018). Guiding decision makers’ eye movements with (un)predictable object locations. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 31(3), 341-354.
doi: 10.1002/bdm.2060 URL |
[38] |
Orquin J. L., & Loose S. M. (2013). Attention and choice: A review on eye movements in decision making. Acta Psychologica, 144(1), 190-206.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.06.003 pmid: 23845447 |
[39] |
Pärnamets P., Johansson P., Hall L., Balkenius C., Spivey M. J., & Richardson D. C. (2015). Biasing moral decisions by exploiting the dynamics of eye gaze. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(13), 4170-4175.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1415250112 pmid: 25775604 |
[40] |
Peschel A. O., Orquin J. L., & Loose S. M. (2019). Increasing consumers' attention capture and food choice through bottom-up effects. Appetite, 132, 1-7.
doi: S0195-6663(18)30872-9 pmid: 30248439 |
[41] | Raptis G. E., Katsini C., Belk M., Fidas C., Samaras G., & Avouris N. (2017). Using eye gaze data and visual activities to infer human cognitive styles:Method and feasibility studies. In M. Bielikova & E. Herder (Eds.), Proceedings of the 25th conference on User Modeling, Adaptation and Personalization (pp. 164-173). New York, ACM Press. |
[42] |
Russo J. E., & Dosher B. A. (1983). Strategies for multiattribute binary choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9(4), 676-696.
doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.9.4.676 URL |
[43] |
Samuelson P. A. (1937). A note on measurement of utility. The Review of Economic Studies, 4(2), 155-161.
doi: 10.2307/2967612 URL |
[44] |
Scheibehenne B., & Pachur T. (2015). Using Bayesian hierarchical parameter estimation to assess the generalizability of cognitive models of choice. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 22(2), 391-407.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0684-4 pmid: 25134469 |
[45] |
Scholten M., & Read D. (2010). The psychology of intertemporal tradeoffs. Psychological Review, 117(3), 925-944.
doi: 10.1037/a0019619 pmid: 20658858 |
[46] |
Schulte-Mecklenbeck M., Kühberger A., Gagl B., & Hutzler F. (2017). Inducing thought processes: Bringing process measures and cognitive processes closer together. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 30(5), 1001-1013.
doi: 10.1002/bdm.2007 URL |
[47] | Schulte-Mecklenbeck M., Kühberger A., & Ranyard R. (2011). The role of process data in the development and testing of process models of judgment and decision making. Judgment and Decision Making, 6(8), 733-739. |
[48] |
Shimojo S., Simion C., Shimojo E., & Scheier C. (2003). Gaze bias both reflects and influences preference. Nature Neuroscience, 6(12), 1317-1322.
doi: 10.1038/nn1150 URL |
[49] |
Shojaeizadeh M., Djamasbi S., Paffenroth R. C., & Trapp A. C. (2019). Detecting task demand via an eye tracking machine learning system. Decision Support Systems, 116, 91-101.
doi: 10.1016/j.dss.2018.10.012 |
[50] | Simon H. (1982) Models of bounded rationality: Behavioral economics and business organization. MIT Press. |
[51] | Sui X. Y., Liu H. Z., & Rao L. L. (2020). The timing of gaze-contingent decision prompts influences risky choice. Cognition, 195, 104077. |
[52] |
Su Y., Rao L.-L., Sun H.-Y., Du X.-L., Li X., & Li S. (2013). Is making a risky choice based on a weighting and adding process? An eye-tracking investigation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 39(6), 1765-1780.
doi: 10.1037/a0032861 URL |
[53] | Tavares G., Perona P., & Rangel A. (2017). The attentional drift diffusion model of simple perceptual decision-making. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 11(468). |
[54] |
Uggeldahl K., Jacobsen C., Lundhede T. H., & Olsen S. B. (2016). Choice certainty in discrete choice experiments: Will eye tracking provide useful measures? Journal of Choice Modelling, 20, 35-48.
doi: 10.1016/j.jocm.2016.09.002 URL |
[55] |
van der Laan L. N., Hooge I. T. C., de Ridder D. T. D., Viergever M. A., & Smeets P. A. M. (2015). Do you like what you see? The role of first fixation and total fixation duration in consumer choice. Food Quality and Preference, 39, 46-55.
doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2014.06.015 URL |
[56] |
Vriens M., Vidden C., & Schomaker J. (2020). What I see is what I want: Top-down attention biasing choice behavior. Journal of Business Research, 111, 262-269.
doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2019.09.001 URL |
[57] |
Weber E. U., & Johnson E. J. (2009). Mindful judgment and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 53-85.
doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163633 pmid: 18798706 |
[58] | Zhang, Y-Y., Zhou, L., Li, S., & Liang Z-Y. (2022). Computation of subjective value does not always elicit alternative-based information searching in intertemporal choice. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2274 |
[59] | Zhou L., Liang Z-Y., Li S., & Zhang L. (2019, August). Similarity in processes of risky choice and intertemporal choice: Based on equivalence conversion. Paper presented at the meeting of Subjective Probability, Utility, and Decision Making, Amsterdam, Netherlands. |
[60] | Zhou L., Zhang L., Su Y., & Liang Z. Y. (2019). Is zero void? Attentional mechanism of hidden-zero effect in risky decision-making. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/pmhsa |
[61] | Zhou L., Zhang Y. Y., Li S., & Liang Z. Y. (2018). New paradigms for the old question: Challenging the expectation rule held by risky decision-making theories. Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology, 12, e17. |
[62] | Zhou L., Zhang Y. Y., Wang Z. J., Rao L. L., Wang W., Li S., Li X. S., & Liang Z. Y. (2016). A scanpath analysis of the risky decision-making process. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 29(2-3), 169-182. |
[63] |
Zuschke N. (2020). The impact of task complexity and task motivation on in-store marketing effectiveness: An eye tracking analysis. Journal of Business Research, 116, 337-350.
doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2020.05.009 URL |
[1] | 凌斌. 行为决策中的选择性信息呈现:基于多重理论整合的视角[J]. 心理科学进展, 2013, 21(11): 2036-2046. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||