Acta Psychologica Sinica ›› 2024, Vol. 56 ›› Issue (4): 469-481.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2024.00469
• Reports of Empirical Studies • Previous Articles Next Articles
WANG Fuxing(), HUANG Yu, ZHANG Yang, ZHU Wanling, LENG Xiaoxue
Received:
2023-03-29
Published:
2024-04-25
Online:
2024-02-02
Contact:
Fuxing Wang, E-mail: Supported by:
WANG Fuxing, HUANG Yu, ZHANG Yang, ZHU Wanling, LENG Xiaoxue. (2024). Prompts affect the learning-by-teaching process and learning outcome in multimedia learning*. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 56(4), 469-481.
Add to citation manager EndNote|Ris|BibTeX
URL: https://journal.psych.ac.cn/acps/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2024.00469
Figure 2. Learning materials (left picture shows relevant basic knowledge; middle picture shows humoral immunity; right picture shows cellular immunity).
Outcome variables | Teaching | Restudy | F | t | p | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | |||||
Prior knowledge | Pretest | 3.82 | 1.52 | 4.64 | 1.06 | 5.51 | — | 0.023 |
Learning outcome | Retention | 14.63 | 3.08 | 12.11 | 3.70 | 10.83 | — | 0.002 |
Transfer | 16.22 | 4.63 | 13.65 | 4.37 | 5.18 | — | 0.027 | |
Subjective feelings | Mental effort | 7.50 | 1.11 | 7.29 | 1.19 | — | 0.70 | 0.487 |
Difficulty | 6.54 | 1.38 | 5.57 | 1.37 | — | 2.63 | 0.011 | |
Interest | 5.39 | 1.71 | 5.61 | 1.93 | — | ?0.44 | 0.662 | |
Motivation | 6.21 | 2.08 | 6.29 | 1.68 | — | ?0.14 | 0.888 |
Table 1 Descriptive statistics and difference test results of each condition in Experiment 1
Outcome variables | Teaching | Restudy | F | t | p | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | |||||
Prior knowledge | Pretest | 3.82 | 1.52 | 4.64 | 1.06 | 5.51 | — | 0.023 |
Learning outcome | Retention | 14.63 | 3.08 | 12.11 | 3.70 | 10.83 | — | 0.002 |
Transfer | 16.22 | 4.63 | 13.65 | 4.37 | 5.18 | — | 0.027 | |
Subjective feelings | Mental effort | 7.50 | 1.11 | 7.29 | 1.19 | — | 0.70 | 0.487 |
Difficulty | 6.54 | 1.38 | 5.57 | 1.37 | — | 2.63 | 0.011 | |
Interest | 5.39 | 1.71 | 5.61 | 1.93 | — | ?0.44 | 0.662 | |
Motivation | 6.21 | 2.08 | 6.29 | 1.68 | — | ?0.14 | 0.888 |
Figure 4. Four conditions in Experiment 2. Note. Picture A is the picture prompt group, Picture B is the text prompt group, Picture C is the keywords prompt group, and Picture D is the no prompt group.
Outcome variables | Picture | Text | Keyword | Control | F | p | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |||
Pretest | 4.76 | 1.30 | 4.21 | 1.34 | 4.17 | 1.28 | 4.21 | 1.48 | 1.24 | 0.300 |
Retention | 16.36 | 2.23 | 13.79 | 3.00 | 16.34 | 2.44 | 14.09 | 3.18 | 7.70 | <.001 |
Transfer | 17.21 | 4.34 | 14.94 | 5.15 | 18.97 | 4.35 | 14.87 | 4.59 | 7.04 | <.001 |
Idea units | 17.59 | 2.44 | 15.57 | 2.85 | 17.86 | 2.08 | 14.64 | 3.48 | 8.58 | <.001 |
Elaborations | 0.88 | 1.13 | 0.89 | 1.17 | 0.90 | 1.18 | 0.30 | 0.57 | 2.36 | 0.076 |
Monitoring | 2.38 | 1.93 | 2.05 | 1.36 | 3.12 | 1.86 | 1.57 | 1.37 | 4.77 | 0.004 |
Mental effort | 7.38 | 0.90 | 7.29 | 0.90 | 7.14 | 1.25 | 7.36 | 1.13 | 0.23 | 0.876 |
Difficulty | 6.10 | 1.63 | 6.46 | 1.58 | 6.17 | 1.37 | 6.46 | 1.50 | 0.41 | 0.746 |
Interest | 5.28 | 1.85 | 4.64 | 1.37 | 4.93 | 1.94 | 5.32 | 1.87 | 0.94 | 0.425 |
Motivation | 6.28 | 1.44 | 5.75 | 1.65 | 5.93 | 1.56 | 6.39 | 1.81 | 0.94 | 0.425 |
Table 2 Descriptive statistics and ANOVA results in Experiment 2
Outcome variables | Picture | Text | Keyword | Control | F | p | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |||
Pretest | 4.76 | 1.30 | 4.21 | 1.34 | 4.17 | 1.28 | 4.21 | 1.48 | 1.24 | 0.300 |
Retention | 16.36 | 2.23 | 13.79 | 3.00 | 16.34 | 2.44 | 14.09 | 3.18 | 7.70 | <.001 |
Transfer | 17.21 | 4.34 | 14.94 | 5.15 | 18.97 | 4.35 | 14.87 | 4.59 | 7.04 | <.001 |
Idea units | 17.59 | 2.44 | 15.57 | 2.85 | 17.86 | 2.08 | 14.64 | 3.48 | 8.58 | <.001 |
Elaborations | 0.88 | 1.13 | 0.89 | 1.17 | 0.90 | 1.18 | 0.30 | 0.57 | 2.36 | 0.076 |
Monitoring | 2.38 | 1.93 | 2.05 | 1.36 | 3.12 | 1.86 | 1.57 | 1.37 | 4.77 | 0.004 |
Mental effort | 7.38 | 0.90 | 7.29 | 0.90 | 7.14 | 1.25 | 7.36 | 1.13 | 0.23 | 0.876 |
Difficulty | 6.10 | 1.63 | 6.46 | 1.58 | 6.17 | 1.37 | 6.46 | 1.50 | 0.41 | 0.746 |
Interest | 5.28 | 1.85 | 4.64 | 1.37 | 4.93 | 1.94 | 5.32 | 1.87 | 0.94 | 0.425 |
Motivation | 6.28 | 1.44 | 5.75 | 1.65 | 5.93 | 1.56 | 6.39 | 1.81 | 0.94 | 0.425 |
Figure 6. The mediating role of idea units in Experiment 2. Note. The values in the figure are standardized regression coefficients, the same below; * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001
Figure 7. Three conditions in Experiment 3. Note. Picture A is the generated prompt group, Picture B is the keyword prompts group, and Picture C is the no prompt group.
Outcome variable | Generate prompts | Keyword prompts | No prompts | F | p | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |||
Pretest | 4.21 | 2.23 | 4.90 | 1.40 | 4.52 | 1.48 | 1.14 | 0.324 |
Retention | 16.00 | 2.73 | 16.21 | 2.86 | 13.10 | 3.21 | 10.96 | <.001 |
Transfer | 19.72 | 3.49 | 17.31 | 5.12 | 14.35 | 5.59 | 10.02 | <.001 |
Idea units | 17.66 | 2.13 | 17.86 | 1.68 | 15.17 | 3.01 | 12.23 | <.001 |
Elaborations | 0.83 | 0.97 | 0.48 | 0.69 | 0.38 | 0.86 | 2.39 | 0.098 |
Monitoring | 2.69 | 2.00 | 2.07 | 1.65 | 1.91 | 1.30 | 1.78 | 0.176 |
Mental effort | 7.55 | 0.95 | 7.21 | 1.29 | 7.10 | 1.26 | 2.28 | 0.109 |
Difficulty | 5052 | 1.64 | 6.00 | 1.10 | 6.10 | 0.82 | 1.21 | 0.304 |
Interest | 5.97 | 1.96 | 5.90 | 1.59 | 5.34 | 1.65 | 1.22 | 0.302 |
Motivation | 6.55 | 1.86 | 6.24 | 1.77 | 6.76 | 1.70 | 0.79 | 0.456 |
Table 3 Descriptive statistics and ANOVA results in Experiment 3
Outcome variable | Generate prompts | Keyword prompts | No prompts | F | p | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |||
Pretest | 4.21 | 2.23 | 4.90 | 1.40 | 4.52 | 1.48 | 1.14 | 0.324 |
Retention | 16.00 | 2.73 | 16.21 | 2.86 | 13.10 | 3.21 | 10.96 | <.001 |
Transfer | 19.72 | 3.49 | 17.31 | 5.12 | 14.35 | 5.59 | 10.02 | <.001 |
Idea units | 17.66 | 2.13 | 17.86 | 1.68 | 15.17 | 3.01 | 12.23 | <.001 |
Elaborations | 0.83 | 0.97 | 0.48 | 0.69 | 0.38 | 0.86 | 2.39 | 0.098 |
Monitoring | 2.69 | 2.00 | 2.07 | 1.65 | 1.91 | 1.30 | 1.78 | 0.176 |
Mental effort | 7.55 | 0.95 | 7.21 | 1.29 | 7.10 | 1.26 | 2.28 | 0.109 |
Difficulty | 5052 | 1.64 | 6.00 | 1.10 | 6.10 | 0.82 | 1.21 | 0.304 |
Interest | 5.97 | 1.96 | 5.90 | 1.59 | 5.34 | 1.65 | 1.22 | 0.302 |
Motivation | 6.55 | 1.86 | 6.24 | 1.77 | 6.76 | 1.70 | 0.79 | 0.456 |
Figure 9. The mediating role of idea units in Experiment 3. Note. The values in the figure are standardized regression coefficients, the same below; * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001
[1] |
Adesope, OO, & Nesbit, JC (2013). Animated and static concept maps enhance learning from spoken narration. Learning and Instruction, 27, 1-10.
doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.02.002 URL |
[2] |
Butler, AC, & Roediger, HL (2007). Testing improves long-term retention in a simulated classroom setting. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19 (4 - 5), 514-527.
doi: 10.1080/09541440701326097 URL |
[3] |
Cheng, M., Kuang, Z., Leng, X., Zhang, Y., Wang, F. (2023). Teaching to promote learning: the impact of learners’ learning by teaching on learning. Advances in Psychological Science, 31 (5), 769-782.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2023.00769 URL |
[4] |
Eitel, A., Scheiter, K., Schüler, A., Nyström, M., & Holmqvist, K. (2013). How a picture facilitates the process of learning from text: Evidence for scaffolding. Learning and Instruction, 28, 48-63.
doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.05.002 URL |
[5] |
Fiorella, L. (2022). Learning by explaining after pauses in video lectures: Are provided visuals a scaffold or a crutch? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 36 (5), 1142-1149.
doi: 10.1002/acp.v36.5 URL |
[6] |
Fiorella, L., & Kuhlmann, S. (2020). Creating drawings enhances learning by teaching. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112 (4), 811-822.
doi: 10.1037/edu0000392 URL |
[7] |
Fiorella, L., & Mayer, RE (2013). The relative benefits of learning by teaching and teaching expectancy. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38 (4), 281-288.
doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2013.06.001 URL |
[8] | Fiorella, L., & Mayer, RE (2015). Learning as a generative activity: Eight learning strategies that promote understanding. Cambridge University Press. |
[9] |
Fiorella, L., & Mayer, RE (2016). Eight ways to promote generative learning. Educational Psychology Review, 28 (4), 717-741.
doi: 10.1007/s10648-015-9348-9 URL |
[10] | Fiorella, L., & Mayer, RE (2022). The generative activity principle in multimedia learning. In R. E. Mayer & L. Fiorella (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (3rd ed., pp. 339-350). Cambridge University Press. |
[11] |
Fiorella, L., Pyres, M., & Hebert, R. (2021). Explaining and drawing activities for learning from multimedia: The role of sequencing and scaffolding. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 35 (6), 1574-1584.
doi: 10.1002/acp.v35.6 URL |
[12] |
Fiorella, L., Stull, AT, Kuhlmann, S., & Mayer, RE (2020). Fostering generative learning from video lessons: Benefits of instructor-generated drawings and learner-generated explanations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112 (5), 895-906.
doi: 10.1037/edu0000408 URL |
[13] |
Gallagher, S. E., Dulain, M., Mahony, N., Kehoe, C., McCarthy, F., & Morgan, G. (2017). Instructor-provided summary infographics to support online learning. Educational Media International, 54 (2), 129-147.
doi: 10.1080/09523987.2017.1362795 URL |
[14] |
Harris, PL, Koenig, MA, Corriveau, KH, & Jaswal, VK (2018). Cognitive foundations of learning from testimony. Annual Review of Psychology, 69 (1), 251-273.
doi: 10.1146/psych.2018.69.issue-1 URL |
[15] |
Hiller, S., Rumann, S., Berthold, K., & Roelle, J. (2020). Example-based learning: Should learners receive closed-book or open-book self-explanation prompts? Instructional Science, 48, 623-649.
doi: 10.1007/s11251-020-09523-4 |
[16] |
Hoogerheide, V., Deijkers, L., Loyens, SM, Heijltjes, A., & van Gog, T. (2016). Gaining from explaining: Learning improves from explaining to fictitious others on video, not from writing to them. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 44-45, 95-106.
doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2016.02.005 URL |
[17] |
Hoogerheide, V., Loyens, SM, & van Gog, T. (2014). Effects of creating video-based modeling examples on learning and transfer. Learning and Instruction, 33, 108-119.
doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2014.04.005 URL |
[18] |
Hoogerheide, V., Renkl, A., Fiorella, L., Paas, F., & van Gog, T. (2019). Enhancing example-based learning: Teaching on video increases arousal and improves problem-solving performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111 (1), 45-56.
doi: 10.1037/edu0000272 |
[19] |
Horovitz, T., & Mayer, R. E. (2021). Learning with human and virtual instructors who display happy or bored emotions in video lectures. Computers in Human Behavior, 119, 106724.
doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2021.106724 URL |
[20] |
Jacob, L., Lachner, A., & Scheiter, K. (2020). Learning by explaining orally or in written form? Text complexity matters. Learning and Instruction, 68, 101344.
doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2020.101344 URL |
[21] |
Kang, SHK, McDermott, KB, & Roediger, HL (2007). Test format and corrective feedback modify the effect of testing on long-term retention. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19 (4-5), 528-558.
doi: 10.1080/09541440601056620 URL |
[22] |
Karpicke, J. D., & Aue, W. R. (2015). The testing effect is alive and well with complex materials. Educational Psychology Review, 27 (2), 317-326.
doi: 10.1007/s10648-015-9309-3 URL |
[23] |
Karpicke, JD, & Blunt, JR (2011). Retrieval practice produces more learning than elaborative studying with concept mapping. Science, 331 (6018), 772-775.
doi: 10.1126/science.1199327 pmid: 21252317 |
[24] |
Karpicke, J. D., Blunt, J. R., & Smith, M. A. (2016). Retrieval-based learning: Positive effects of retrieval practice in elementary school children. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 350.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00350 pmid: 27014156 |
[25] |
Karpicke, J. D., & Grimaldi, P. J. (2012). Retrieval-based learning: A perspective for enhancing meaningful learning. Educational Psychology Review, 24 (3), 401-418.
doi: 10.1007/s10648-012-9202-2 URL |
[26] | Karpicke, J. D., Lehman, M., & Aue, W. R. (2014). Retrieval-based learning:An episodic context account. In BH Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 237-284). San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press. |
[27] |
Kobayashi, K. (2019). Learning by preparing-to-teach and teaching: A meta-analysis. Japanese Psychological Research, 61 (3), 192-203.
doi: 10.1111/jpr.12221 |
[28] |
Koh, A. W. L., Lee, S. C., & Lim, S. W. H. (2018). The learning benefits of teaching: A retrieval practice hypothesis. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 32 (3), 401-410.
doi: 10.1002/acp.v32.3 URL |
[29] | Kuhlmann, S., & Fiorella, L. (2022). Effects of instructor-provided visuals on learner-generated explanations. Educational Psychology, 42 (9), 1068-1088. |
[30] |
Lachner, A., Jacob, L., & Hoogerheide, V. (2021). Learning by writing explanations: Is explaining to a fictitious student more effective than self-explaining? Learning and Instruction, 74, 101438.
doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2020.101438 URL |
[31] |
Leopold, C., & Leutner, D. (2012). Science text comprehension: Drawing, main idea selection, and summarizing as learning strategies. Learning and Instruction, 22 (1), 16-26.
doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.05.005 URL |
[32] | Mayer, R. E. (2009). Multimedia learning (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press. |
[33] | Mayer, R. E. (2014). The cognitive theory of multimedia learning. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (2nd ed., pp. 43-71). Cambridge University Press. |
[34] | Mayer, R. E. (2020). Multimedia learning (3rd ed.). Cambridge University Press. |
[35] | Mayer, R. E. (2022). Cognitive theory of multimedia learning. In RE Mayer & L. Fiorella (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (3rd ed., pp. 57-72). Cambridge University Press. |
[36] |
McCabe, J. A. (2015). Learning the brain in introductory psychology: Examining the generation effect for mnemonics and examples. Teaching of Psychology, 42 (3), 203-210.
doi: 10.1177/0098628315587617 URL |
[37] |
Paas, F. (1992). Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skills in statistics: A cognitive load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84 (4), 429-434.
doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.84.4.429 URL |
[38] |
Pi, Z., Zhang, Y., Zhou, W., Xu, K., Chen, Y., Yang, J., & Zhao, Q. (2021). Learning by explaining to oneself and a peer enhances learners' theta and alpha oscillations while watching video lectures. British Journal of Educational Technology, 52 (2), 659-679.
doi: 10.1111/bjet.v52.2 URL |
[39] |
Ponce, H. R., Mayer, R. E., & Méndez, E. E. (2022). Effects of learner-generated highlighting and instructor-provided highlighting on learning from text: A meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 34, 989-1024.
doi: 10.1007/s10648-021-09654-1 |
[40] |
Rittle-Johnson, B., & Loehr, A. M. (2017). Eliciting explanations: Constraints on when self-explanation aids learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24 (5), 1501-1510.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1079-5 URL |
[41] |
Roelle, J., & Berthold, K. (2017). Effects of incorporating retrieval into learning tasks: The complexity of the tasks matters. Learning and Instruction, 49, 142-156.
doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2017.01.008 URL |
[42] |
Roelle, J., & Nückles, M. (2019). Generative learning versus retrieval practice in learning from text: The cohesion and elaboration of the text matters. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111 (8), 1341-1361.
doi: 10.1037/edu0000345 URL |
[43] |
Roscoe, R. D. (2014). Self-monitoring and knowledge-building in learning by teaching. Instructional Science, 42, 327-351.
doi: 10.1007/s11251-013-9283-4 URL |
[44] |
Rowland, C. A. (2014). The effect of testing versus restudy on retention: A meta-analytic review of the testing effect. Psychological Bulletin, 140 (6), 1432-1463.
doi: 10.1037/a0037559 pmid: 25150680 |
[45] |
Sibley, L., Fiorella, L., & Lachner, A. (2022). It's better when I see it: Students benefit more from open-book than closed-book teaching. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 36 (6), 1347-1355.
doi: 10.1002/acp.v36.6 URL |
[46] |
Smith, M. A., Blunt, J. R., Whiffen, J. W., & Karpicke, J. D. (2016). Does providing prompts during retrieval practice improve learning? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 30 (4), 544-553.
doi: 10.1002/acp.v30.4 URL |
[47] |
Sweller, J. (2010). Element interactivity and intrinsic, extraneous and germane cognitive load. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 123-138.
doi: 10.1007/s10648-010-9128-5 URL |
[48] |
Sweller, J., van Merriënboer, J. J, & Paas, F. (2019). Cognitive architecture and instructional design: 20 years later. Educational Psychology Review, 31, 261-292.
doi: 10.1007/s10648-019-09465-5 |
[49] |
Waldeyer, J., Heitmann, S., Moning, J., & Roelle, J. (2020). Can generative learning tasks be optimized by incorporation of retrieval practice? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 9 (3), 355-369.
doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2020.05.001 URL |
[50] |
Wittrock, M. C. (1974). Learning as a generative process. Educational Psychologist, 11 (2), 87-95.
doi: 10.1080/00461527409529129 URL |
[51] |
Wittrock, M. C. (2010). Learning as a generative process. Educational Psychologist, 45 (1), 40-45.
doi: 10.1080/00461520903433554 URL |
[52] |
Zhang, Q., & Fiorella, L. (2019). Role of generated and provided visuals in supporting learning from scientific text. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 59, 101808.
doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2019.101808 URL |
[53] | Zhang, Q., & Fiorella, L. (2021). Learning by drawing: When is it worth the time and effort? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 66 (3), 101990. |
[1] | MA Xiaofeng, LI Tiantian, JIA Ruihong, WEI Jie. The forward testing effect in spatial route learning [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2022, 54(12): 1433-1442. |
[2] | ZHOU Aibao; YANG Tiancheng; CHENG Chen; MA Xiaofeng; ZHAO Jing. Retrieval Practice Produces More Learning in Multiple-list Tests with Higher-Order Skills [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(7): 928-938. |
[3] | ZHOU Aibao;MA Xiaofeng;Li Jing;CUI Dan. The Advantage Effect of Retrieval Practice on Memory Retention and Transfer: Based on Explanation of Cognitive Load Theory [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2013, 45(8): 849-859. |
[4] | MU De-Fang,SONG Yao-Wu,CHEN Ying-H. Mechanism of Retrieval Inhibition in Directed Forgetting: Retrieval Success Produces Inhibition [J]. , 2009, 41(01): 26-34. |
[5] | Lin Chongde,Li Tsingan,Li Hongyu. The Effect of Mnemonic Key-letters Method on Chinese Children at risk in English Vocabulary Learning [J]. , 2004, 36(04): 482-490. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||