Acta Psychologica Sinica ›› 2021, Vol. 53 ›› Issue (3): 259-272.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00259
• Reports of Empirical Studies • Previous Articles Next Articles
ZUO Bin, DAI Yuee, WEN Fangfang(), GAO Jia, XIE Zhijie, HE Saifei
Received:
2019-09-18
Published:
2021-03-25
Online:
2021-01-27
Contact:
WEN Fangfang
E-mail:wenff@mail.ccnu.edu.cn
Supported by:
ZUO Bin, DAI Yuee, WEN Fangfang, GAO Jia, XIE Zhijie, HE Saifei. (2021). “You were what you eat”: Food-gender stereotypes and their impact on evaluation of impression. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 53(3), 259-272.
Add to citation manager EndNote|Ris|BibTeX
URL: https://journal.psych.ac.cn/acps/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00259
Preference | M | SD | 95% CI | t | Cohen's d |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Male food preference | 1.88 | 1.48 | [1.69, 2.08] | 19.26*** | 1.81 |
Female food preference | -2.24 | 1.50 | [-2.43, -2.05] | -22.71*** | 2.13 |
Table 1 Paired sample t test for preference of male and female for masculine and feminine foods
Preference | M | SD | 95% CI | t | Cohen's d |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Male food preference | 1.88 | 1.48 | [1.69, 2.08] | 19.26*** | 1.81 |
Female food preference | -2.24 | 1.50 | [-2.43, -2.05] | -22.71*** | 2.13 |
Target individual | M | SD | 95% CI | t | Cohen's d |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Stereotypical male | 0.20 | 0.81 | [0.09, 0.31] | 3.62*** | 0.22 |
Stereotypical female | -0.51 | 0.85 | [-0.62, -0.39] | -8.58*** | 0.55 |
Counter-Stereotypical male | -0.57 | 0.89 | [-0.69, -0.44] | -9.16*** | 0.56 |
Counter-Stereotypical female | 0.16 | 0.85 | [0.04 0.27] | 2.67** | 0.17 |
Table 2 Paired-sample t test results on competence vs warmth score on stereotypical and counter-stereotypical targets
Target individual | M | SD | 95% CI | t | Cohen's d |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Stereotypical male | 0.20 | 0.81 | [0.09, 0.31] | 3.62*** | 0.22 |
Stereotypical female | -0.51 | 0.85 | [-0.62, -0.39] | -8.58*** | 0.55 |
Counter-Stereotypical male | -0.57 | 0.89 | [-0.69, -0.44] | -9.16*** | 0.56 |
Counter-Stereotypical female | 0.16 | 0.85 | [0.04 0.27] | 2.67** | 0.17 |
Comparison object | M | SD | t | 95% CI | p | Cohen's d |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Male-Competence | ||||||
Consistent positive - Negative | -5.02 | 19.88 | -1.64 | [-11.22, 1.17] | 0.11 | 0.01 |
Inconsistent positive - Negative | 2.83 | 93.87 | 0.20 | [-26.42, 32.98] | 0.85 | 0.01 |
Consistent positive - Inconsistent positive | -5.63 | 21.36 | -1.71 | [-12.29, 1.02] | 0.10 | 0.02 |
Consistent negativity - Inconsistent negativity | 2.22 | 95.56 | 0.15 | [-27.56, 32.00] | 0.88 | 0.01 |
Male-Warmth | ||||||
Consistent positive - Negativity | -1.51 | 14.03 | -0.69 | [-5.94, 2.92] | 0.50 | 0.00 |
Inconsistent positive - Negative | -6.70 | 19.39 | -2.21* | [-12.82, -0.59] | 0.03 | 0.02 |
Consistent positive - Inconsistent positive | 1.65 | 19.61 | 0.54 | [-4.54, 7.83] | 0.59 | 0.00 |
Consistent negativity - Inconsistent negativity | -3.55 | 15.64 | -1.45 | [-8.49, 1.39] | 0.15 | 0.01 |
Female-Competence | ||||||
Consistent positive - Negativity | -21.71 | 132.38 | -1.10 | [-61.48, 18.06] | 0.28 | 0.06 |
Inconsistent positive - Negative | -0.12 | 13.59 | -0.06 | [-4.20, 3.97] | 0.95 | 0.00 |
Consistent positive - Inconsistent positive | -0.93 | 18.43 | -0.34 | [-6.47, 4.61] | 0.74 | 0.00 |
Consistent negativity - Inconsistent negativity | 20.66 | 133.67 | 1.04 | [-19.50, 60.82] | 0.31 | 0.06 |
Female-Warmth | ||||||
Consistent positive - Negativity | -3.65 | 24.27 | -1.02 | [-10.86, 3.56] | 0.31 | 0.01 |
Inconsistent positive - Negative | 0.86 | 14.72 | 0.40 | [-3.51, 5.23] | 0.70 | 0.00 |
Consistent positive - Inconsistent positive | -0.68 | 21.07 | -0.22 | [-6.93, 5.58] | 0.83 | 0.00 |
Consistent negativity - Inconsistent negativity | 3.83 | 14.02 | 1.85 | [-0.34, 7.99] | 0.07 | 0.01 |
Table 3 Paired sample t test for difference in reaction time
Comparison object | M | SD | t | 95% CI | p | Cohen's d |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Male-Competence | ||||||
Consistent positive - Negative | -5.02 | 19.88 | -1.64 | [-11.22, 1.17] | 0.11 | 0.01 |
Inconsistent positive - Negative | 2.83 | 93.87 | 0.20 | [-26.42, 32.98] | 0.85 | 0.01 |
Consistent positive - Inconsistent positive | -5.63 | 21.36 | -1.71 | [-12.29, 1.02] | 0.10 | 0.02 |
Consistent negativity - Inconsistent negativity | 2.22 | 95.56 | 0.15 | [-27.56, 32.00] | 0.88 | 0.01 |
Male-Warmth | ||||||
Consistent positive - Negativity | -1.51 | 14.03 | -0.69 | [-5.94, 2.92] | 0.50 | 0.00 |
Inconsistent positive - Negative | -6.70 | 19.39 | -2.21* | [-12.82, -0.59] | 0.03 | 0.02 |
Consistent positive - Inconsistent positive | 1.65 | 19.61 | 0.54 | [-4.54, 7.83] | 0.59 | 0.00 |
Consistent negativity - Inconsistent negativity | -3.55 | 15.64 | -1.45 | [-8.49, 1.39] | 0.15 | 0.01 |
Female-Competence | ||||||
Consistent positive - Negativity | -21.71 | 132.38 | -1.10 | [-61.48, 18.06] | 0.28 | 0.06 |
Inconsistent positive - Negative | -0.12 | 13.59 | -0.06 | [-4.20, 3.97] | 0.95 | 0.00 |
Consistent positive - Inconsistent positive | -0.93 | 18.43 | -0.34 | [-6.47, 4.61] | 0.74 | 0.00 |
Consistent negativity - Inconsistent negativity | 20.66 | 133.67 | 1.04 | [-19.50, 60.82] | 0.31 | 0.06 |
Female-Warmth | ||||||
Consistent positive - Negativity | -3.65 | 24.27 | -1.02 | [-10.86, 3.56] | 0.31 | 0.01 |
Inconsistent positive - Negative | 0.86 | 14.72 | 0.40 | [-3.51, 5.23] | 0.70 | 0.00 |
Consistent positive - Inconsistent positive | -0.68 | 21.07 | -0.22 | [-6.93, 5.58] | 0.83 | 0.00 |
Consistent negativity - Inconsistent negativity | 3.83 | 14.02 | 1.85 | [-0.34, 7.99] | 0.07 | 0.01 |
[1] |
Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Stewart, I., & Boles, S. (2010). A sketch of the implicit relational assessment procedure (irap) and the relational elaboration and coherence (rec) model. Psychological Record, 60, (3), 527-542.
doi: 10.1007/BF03395726 URL |
[2] |
Bian, L., Leslie, S. -J., & Cimpian, A. (2017). Gender stereotypes about intellectual competence emerge early and influence children's interests. Science, 355 (6323), 389-391.
doi: 10.1126/science.aah6524 URL pmid: 28126816 |
[3] |
Breen, A. B., & Karpinski, A. (2013). Implicit and explicit attitudes toward gay males and lesbians among heterosexual males and females. Journal of Social Psychology, 153 (3), 351-374.
doi: 10.1080/00224545.2012.739581 URL |
[4] | Cai, H. J., Huang, X. F., & Song, H. R. (2008). The relationship between sex-role and subjective well-being in China. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 40, (4), 474-486. |
[5] | Cai H., J., & Yang, Z. L. (2002). Using IAT to study the structure of gender self-concept. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 34, (2), 59-65. |
[6] | Cai, H. J., Zhou, Y., & Shi, Q. H. (2001). Implicit association test (IAT) and its application in the study of gender stereotypes. Journal of Social Psychology Research, (4), 6-11. |
[7] | Cambon, L., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2017). Compensation is for real: Evidence from existing groups in the context of actual relations. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 20 (6), 745-756. |
[8] |
Cambon, L., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2018). Two routes toward compensation: An investigation into the mechanisms of compensation for high-and low-status groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 77, 24-35.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2018.04.002 URL |
[9] |
Cavazza, N., Guidetti, M., & Butera, F. (2015). Ingredients of gender-based stereotypes about food. Indirect influence of food type, portion size and presentation on gendered intentions to eat. Appetite, 91, 266-272.
doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.04.068 URL pmid: 25934089 |
[10] |
Cavazza, N., Guidetti, M., & Butera, F. (2017). Portion size tells who i am, food type tells who you are: specific functions of amount and type of food in same- and opposite-sex dyadic eating contexts. Appetite, 112, 96-101.
doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.01.019 URL pmid: 28111085 |
[11] |
Dai, T. T., Zuo, B., & Wen, F. F. (2014). The compensation effect between warmth and competence in social cognition. Advances in Psychological Science, 22, (3), 502-511.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.00502 URL |
[12] |
Derous, E., Ryan, A. M., & Serlie, A. W. (2015). Double jeopardy upon resumé screening: when achmed is less employable than aïsha. Personnel Psychology, 68, (3), 659-696.
doi: 10.1111/peps.12078 URL |
[13] |
Drake, C. E., Primeaux, S., & Thomas, J. (2017). Comparing implicit gender stereotypes between women and men with the implicit relational assessment procedure. Gender Issues, 35, (7), 1-18.
doi: 10.1007/s12147-017-9208-7 URL |
[14] |
Eagly, A. H., Nater, C., Miller, D. I., Kaufmann, M., & Sczesny, S. (2020). Gender stereotypes have changed: A cross-temporal meta-analysis of U.S. public opinion polls from 1946 to 2018. American Psychologist, 75, (3), 301-315.
doi: 10.1037/amp0000494 URL |
[15] | Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (2012). Social role theory. (Unpublished master's thesis). Shaanxi Normal University, Xi'an, China. |
[16] |
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods, 39, (2), 175-191.
doi: 10.3758/BF03193146 URL |
[17] |
Finn, M., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Mcenteggart, C. (2018). Exploring the single-trial-type-dominance-effect in the irap: Developing a differential arbitrarily applicable relational responding effects (daarre) model. Psychological Record, 68 (8), 11-25.
doi: 10.1007/s40732-017-0262-z URL |
[18] | Fiske, S. T. (2004). Social beings: A core motives approach to social psychology. New York: Wiley. |
[19] |
Fiske, S. T. (2018). Stereotype content: warmth and competence endure. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27 (2), 67-73. doi: 10.1177/0963721417738825
doi: 10.1177/0963721417738825 URL pmid: 29755213 |
[20] |
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, (2), 77-83.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.005 URL |
[21] |
Fiske, S. T., Xu, J., Cuddy, A. C., & Glick, P. (1999). (Dis)Respecting versus(Dis) liking: Status and interdependence predict ambivalent stereotypes of competence and warmth. Journal of Social Issues, 55, (3), 473-489.
doi: 10.1111/josi.1999.55.issue-3 URL |
[22] |
Gal, D., & Wilkie, J. (2010). Real men don’t eat quiche. Regulation of gender-expressive choices by men. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 1, (4), 291-301.
doi: 10.1177/1948550610365003 URL |
[23] |
Galinsky, A. D., Hall, E. V., & Cuddy, A. J. C., . (2013). Gendered races: implications for interracial marriage, leadership selection, and athletic participation. Psychology Science, 24 (4), 498-506.
doi: 10.1177/0956797612457783 URL |
[24] |
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, (1), 4-27.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.102.1.4 URL pmid: 7878162 |
[25] |
Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Rudman, L. A., Farnham, S. D., Nosek, B. A., & Mellott, D. S. (2002). A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept. Psychological Review, 109, (1), 3-25.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.109.1.3 URL pmid: 11863040 |
[26] | Guan, J., & Cheng, J. T. (2011). Dimensionality and measure of stereotype content model and ifluence of involvement. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 19, (2), 184-188. |
[27] | He, Z. R. (2012). Gender consciousness in Laozi and its historical influence. Journal of South China Normal University (social science edition), (1), 107-113. |
[28] |
Hentschel, T., Heilman, M. E., & Peus, C. V. (2019). The multiple dimensions of gender stereotypes: A current look at men’s and women’s characterizations of others and themselves. Frontiers in Psychology, 10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00011.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.03037 URL pmid: 32116868 |
[29] |
Higgs, S., & Thomas, J. (2016). Social influences on eating. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 9, 1-6.
doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.10.005 URL |
[30] | Hofstede, G. (Eds.), Xu, L. S. (Review). (2008). Culture’s consequence: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations (2nd ed.). Shanghai, China: Shanghai Foreign Language Education Press. |
[31] |
Hoyt, W. D., Hamilton, S. B., & Rickard, K. M. (2003). The effects of dietary fat and caloric content on the body-size estimates of anorexic profile and normal college students. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 59, (1), 85-91.
doi: 10.1002/jclp.10119 URL pmid: 12508333 |
[32] | Jiang, X. H., Yang, F., Li, X. L., & Wang, M. F. (2016). Implicit gender stereotypes on warmth and competence dimensions in children. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 24 (4), 613-617. |
[33] |
Johnson, K. L., Freeman, J. B., & Pauker, K. (2012). Race is gendered: how covarying phenotypes and stereotypes bias sex categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, (1), 116-131.
doi: 10.1037/a0025335 URL |
[34] | Katz, D., & Braly, K. (1933). Racial stereotypes of one hundred college students. Journal of Abnormal & Social Psychology, 28 (3), 280-290. |
[35] |
Kimura, A., Wada, Y., Asakawa, A., Masuda, T., Goto, S. I., Dan, I., & Oka, T. (2012). Dish influences implicit gender-based food stereotypes among young Japanese adults. Appetite, 58, (3), 940-945.
doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.02.013 URL |
[36] |
Kimura, A., Wada, Y., Goto, S. -I., Tsuzuki, D., Cai, D. S., Oka, T., & Dan, I. (2009). Implicit gender-based food stereotypes. semantic priming experiments on young Japanese. Appetite, 52, (2), 521-524.
doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2008.11.002 URL |
[37] | Ma, F., & Liang, N. J. (2006). A study on implicit mathematics gender stereotype by SEB. Journal of Psychological Science, 29, (5), 1116-1118. |
[38] | Ma, F., & Liang, N. J. (2008). A study on implicit mathematics gender stereotype by IAT. Journal of Psychological Science, 31 (1), 35-39. |
[39] | Macrae, C. N., Stangor, C., & Hewstone, M. (Eds). (1996). Foundations of stereotypes and stereotyping. New York: Guilford. |
[40] |
Mooney, K. M., Detore, J., & Malloy, K. A. (1994). Perceptions of women related to food choice. Sex Roles, 31 (7-8), 433-442.
doi: 10.1007/BF01544199 URL |
[41] | Mooney, K. M., & Lorenz, E. (1997). The effects of food and gender on interpersonal perceptions. 36 (9-10), 639-653. |
[42] |
Mõttus, R., Mcneill, G., Jia, X. L., Craig, L. C. A., Starr, J. M., & Deary, I. J. (2013). The associations between personality, diet and body mass index in older people. Health Psychology, 32, (4), 353-360.
doi: 10.1037/a0025537 URL |
[43] | Murcott, A. (1983). The sociology of food and eating: essays on the sociological significance of food. Aldershot, England: Grower. |
[44] |
Nath, J. (2011). Gendered fare? A qualitative investigation of alternative food and masculinities. Journal of Sociology, 47 (3), 261-278.
doi: 10.1177/1440783310386828 URL |
[45] |
Oakes, M. E. (2004). Good foods gone bad: “infamous” nutrients diminish perceived vitamin and mineral content of foods. Appetite, 42, (3), 273-278.
doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2003.10.004 URL |
[46] | Okahana, H., & Zhou, E. (2018). Graduate enrollment and degrees: 2007 to 2017. Washington, DC: Council of Graduate Schools. |
[47] |
Park, B., Smith, J. A., & Correll, J. (2008). "Having it all" or "doing it all"? perceived trait attributes and behavioral obligations as a function of workload, parenthood, and gender. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, (7), 1156-1164.
doi: 10.1002/ejsp.v38:7 URL |
[48] |
Plaks, J. E., Stroessner, S. J., Dweck, C. S., & Sherman, J. W. (2001). Person theories and attention allocation: preferences for stereotypic versus counterstereotypic information. Journal of personality and social psychology, 80, (6), 876-893.
URL pmid: 11414372 |
[49] |
Pliner, P., & Chaiken, S. (1990). Eating, social motives, and self- presentation in women and men. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26, (3), 240-254.
doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(90)90037-M URL |
[50] | Pohlmann, A. (2014). Threatened at the table: Meat consumption, maleness and men's gender identities. Dissertations & Theses- Gradworks. University of Hawai'i at Manoa. |
[51] | Qian, M. Y., Luo, S. H., Zhang, G. J., Chen, P., & Yao, P. (1999). An elementary survey of sex stereotype. Chinese Journal of Applied Psychology, 5, (1), 14-19. |
[52] |
Raynor, D. A., & Levine, H. (2009). Associations between the five-factor model of personality and healt behaviors among college students. Journal of American College Health, 58, (1), 73-81.
doi: 10.3200/JACH.58.1.73-82 URL pmid: 19592356 |
[53] |
Rocco, P., Adams, R. B., Ursula, H., Kleck, R. E., & Leslie, Z. (2017). Age and gender differences in facial attractiveness, but not emotion resemblance, contribute to age and gender stereotypes. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1704.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01704 URL pmid: 29033881 |
[54] | Rothgerber, H. (2013). Real men don’t eat (vegetable) quiche: Masculinity and the justification of meat consumption. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 14, (4), 363-375. |
[55] |
Rozin, P., Hormes, J. M., Faith, M. S., & Wansink, B. (2012). Is meat male? a quantitative multimethod framework to establish metaphoric relationships. Journal of Consumer Research, 39, (3), 629-643.
doi: 10.1086/664970 URL |
[56] |
Schösler, H., Boer, J. D., Boersema, J. J., & Aiking, H. (2015). Meat and masculinity among young Chinese, Turkish and Dutch adults in the Netherlands. Appetite, 89 (6), 152-159.
doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.02.013 URL |
[57] | Song, J. J., Zuo, B., Wen, F. F., Tan, X., & Zhao, M. M. (2017). The effect of crossed categorization on stereotype-wealth × age group. Psychological Exploration, 37, (2), 155-160. |
[58] |
Stein, R. I., & Nemeroff, C. J. (1995). Moral overtones of food: Judgments of others based on what they eat. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21 (5), 480-490.
doi: 10.1177/0146167295215006 URL |
[59] | Su, H., Ren, X. P., Lu, K. W., & Zhang, H. (2016). The evolution of given names and times changes. Youth Studies, (3), 31-38. |
[60] |
Thomas, M. A. (2016). Are vegans the same as vegetarians? the effect of diet on perceptions of masculinity. Appetite, 97, 79-86.
doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.11.021 URL pmid: 26593101 |
[61] |
Tryon, M. S., Stanhope, K. L., Epel, E. S., Mason, A. E., Brown, R., Medici, V., … Laugero, K. D. (2015). Excessive sugar consumption may be a difficult habit to break: A view from the brain and body. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 100, (6), 2239-2247.
doi: 10.1210/jc.2014-4353 URL pmid: 25879513 |
[62] |
Vandello, J. A., Bosson, J. K., Cohen, D., Burnaford, R. M., & Weaver, J. R. (2008). Precarious manhood. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 95, (6), 1325-1339.
doi: 10.1037/a0012453 URL pmid: 19025286 |
[63] |
Vartanian, L. R. (2015). Impression management and food intake. Current directions in research. Appetite, 86, 74-80.
doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.08.021 URL pmid: 25149198 |
[64] |
Vartanian, L. R., Herman, C. P., & Polivy, J. (2007). Consumption stereotypes and impression management: How you are what you eat. Appetite, 48, (3), 265-277.
doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2006.10.008 URL |
[65] | Wang, P., & Wang, K. (2009). Implicit relational assessment procedures: A new method for measuring implicit attitude. Psychological Science, 32, (3), 669-671. |
[66] | Wang, X., Geng, L., Qin, J., & Yao, S. (2016). The potential relationship between spicy taste and risk seeking. Judgment & Decision Making, 11, (6), 547-553. |
[67] | Wen, F. F., & Zuo, B. (2019). Concepts, cues and influential mechanisms of social categorization. Journal of Psychological Science, 42, (2), 141-147. |
[68] |
White, K., & Dahl, D. W. (2006). To be or not be? The influence of dissociative reference groups on consumer preferences. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 16, (4), 404-413.
doi: 10.1207/s15327663jcp1604_11 URL |
[69] |
Wolffhechel, K., Fagertun, J., Jacobsen, U. P., Majewski, W., Hemmingsen, A. S., Larsen, C. L., … Jarmer, H. (2014). Interpretation of appearance: The effect of facial features on first impressions and personality. Plos One, 9, (9), e107721. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0107721.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0107721 URL pmid: 25233221 |
[70] | Yu, H. Y., & Liang, N. J. (2005). Stereotypic explanatory bias (SEB)——A new index to show people's implicit attitudes. Psychological Science, 28 (1), 42-44. |
[71] |
Zarate, M. A., & Smith, E. R. (1990). Person categorization and stereotyping. Social Cognition, 8, (2), 161-185.
doi: 10.1521/soco.1990.8.2.161 URL |
[72] |
Zellner, D. A., Loaiza, S., Gonzalez, Z., Pita, J., Morales, J., Pecora, D., & Wolf, A. (2006). Food selection changes under stress. Physiology and Behavior, 87, (4), 789-793.
doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.01.014 URL pmid: 16519909 |
[73] | Zhang, Q., & Wang, M. F. (2014). The implicit mixed gender stereotypes regarding warmth and competence dimensions in college students: evidences from IAT. Journal of Shandong Normal University (Humnnities and Social Sciences), 59 (5), 126-130. |
[74] | Zuo, B. (2015). Stereotype content and form. Wuhan: Central China Normal University Press. |
[75] | Zuo, B., & Liu, X. (2006). The researches of implicit gender stereotype based on IAT and SEB. Psychological Development and Education. 22 (4), 57-63. |
[1] | CHEN Sijing, PU Xueli, ZHU Yue, WANG Hao, LIU Jianwei. The impact of normative misperception on food waste in dining out: Mechanism analyses and countermeasures [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2021, 53(8): 904-918. |
[2] | ZUO Bin, LIU Chen, WEN Fangfang, TAN Xiao, XIE Zhijie. The impact of gender orientation of names on individuals’ evaluation of impressions and interpersonal interaction [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2021, 53(4): 387-399. |
[3] | HAN Shangfeng, LI Yue, LIU Shen, XU Qiang, TAN Qun, ZHANG Lin. Beauty is in the eye of the beholder: The halo effect and generalization effect in the facial attractiveness evaluation [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(4): 363-376. |
[4] | HE Ning; ZHU Yunli. Self-love and other-love: Research on the relationships among narcissism, empathy and implicit altruism [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2016, 48(2): 199-210. |
[5] | CUI Yichen, WANG Pei, TAN Chenhao. The influence of implicit theories of personality on processing strategy of person impressions [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2016, 48(12): 1538-1550. |
[6] | FU Xinyuan; LU Zhiyuan; KOU Yu. Effects of a Stranger’s Presence and Behavior on Moral Hypocrisy [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(8): 1058-1066. |
[7] | QIAN Miao; ZHOU Lixia; LU Tiantian; WENG Mengxing; FU Genyue. Theoretical Construction and Empirical Research on A Preschooler-friendly Implicit Association Test [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(7): 903-913. |
[8] | YAN Bihua; JI Ming; ZHAO Xiaojun; TU Jinlu; YOU Xuqun. Predictive Effect of Implicit Safety Attitudes on Safety Performance in Aviation Safety Culture [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(1): 119-128. |
[9] | CHEN Shujuan; WANG Pei; LIANG Yajun. Perceiving Groups and Persons: Inverse Base-Rate Effect [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2014, 46(11): 1772-1781. |
[10] | YUAN Denghua; FU Chunjiang; LUO Siming. Formation and Change of Brand Impression: Based on Dual Process Model [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2014, 46(10): 1534-1548. |
[11] | GUAN Mu-Zhen,LIU Xu-Feng,MIAO Dan-Min,LV Jing,HONG Xia,YANG Hai. A Comparison of Violent Attitude Between Passionate Criminals and Recidivists [J]. , 2010, 42(05): 599-606. |
[12] | Chi-yue CHIU,Zhi-Min ZOU,Sheng-Dong LIN. Culture and Socially Desirable Responding: An Individual-in-Society Perspective [J]. , 2010, 42(01): 48-55. |
[13] | Cai-Huajian. THE EFFECT OF IMPLICIT SELF-ESTEEM AND THE RELATIONSHIP OF EXPLICIT SELF-ESTEEM AND IMPLICIT SELF-ESTEEM [J]. , 2003, 35(06): 796-801. |
[14] | Song Guangwen,Chen Qishan. THE INFLUENCE OF IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT ON CHANGE OF ATTITUDE AFTER FORCED COMPLIANCE [J]. , 2003, 35(03): 397-403. |
[15] | Cai Huajian, Yang Zhiliang (Department of Psychology, East China Normal University, Shanghai 200062). USING IAT TO STUDY THE STRUCTURE OF GENDER SELF-CONCEPT [J]. , 2002, 34(02): 59-65. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||