Acta Psychologica Sinica ›› 2024, Vol. 56 ›› Issue (12): 1706-1717.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2024.01706
• Reports of Empirical Studies • Previous Articles Next Articles
WANG Chengcheng1,2,3, ZHAO Yufei1, SHENG Yingying2,3, ZHAO Qingbai2,3, XIAO Mengshi1(), HAN Lei1()
Published:
2024-12-25
Online:
2024-11-04
Contact:
XIAO Mengshi,HAN Lei
E-mail:mengshi_x@163.com;hanlei-0333@163.com
WANG Chengcheng, ZHAO Yufei, SHENG Yingying, ZHAO Qingbai, XIAO Mengshi, HAN Lei. (2024). Occurrence stage of SNARC effect. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 56(12), 1706-1717.
Response hand | Left arrow | Right arrow | Bidirectional arrow | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Small number | Large number | Small number | Large number | Small number | Large number | |
Left hand | 99.06 ± 2.00 | 98.75 ± 3.56 | 96.56 ± 4.89 | 96.25 ± 4.60 | 98.54 ± 2.01 | 99.17 ± 2.35 |
Right hand | 95.73 ± 5.04 | 96.67 ± 3.68 | 98.75 ± 2.35 | 99.27 ± 1.60 | 98.23 ± 2.29 | 98.65 ± 3.32 |
Table 1 The accuracy rate of each level in Experiment 1a (M ± SD; %)
Response hand | Left arrow | Right arrow | Bidirectional arrow | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Small number | Large number | Small number | Large number | Small number | Large number | |
Left hand | 99.06 ± 2.00 | 98.75 ± 3.56 | 96.56 ± 4.89 | 96.25 ± 4.60 | 98.54 ± 2.01 | 99.17 ± 2.35 |
Right hand | 95.73 ± 5.04 | 96.67 ± 3.68 | 98.75 ± 2.35 | 99.27 ± 1.60 | 98.23 ± 2.29 | 98.65 ± 3.32 |
Response hand | Left arrow | Right arrow | Bidirectional arrow | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Small number | Large number | Small number | Large number | Small number | Large number | |
Left hand | 490 ± 61 | 505 ± 67 | 540 ± 67 | 548 ± 64 | 525 ± 71 | 520 ± 67 |
Right hand | 533 ± 68 | 543 ± 72 | 507 ± 66 | 501 ± 76 | 520 ± 75 | 516 ± 78 |
Table 2 The reaction time of each level in Experiment 1a (M ± SD; %)
Response hand | Left arrow | Right arrow | Bidirectional arrow | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Small number | Large number | Small number | Large number | Small number | Large number | |
Left hand | 490 ± 61 | 505 ± 67 | 540 ± 67 | 548 ± 64 | 525 ± 71 | 520 ± 67 |
Right hand | 533 ± 68 | 543 ± 72 | 507 ± 66 | 501 ± 76 | 520 ± 75 | 516 ± 78 |
Response hand | Up arrow | Down arrow | Bidirectional arrow | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Small number | Large number | Small number | Large number | Small number | Large number | |
Left hand | 98.33 ± 2.80 | 98.13 ± 4.11 | 97.60 ± 3.25 | 97.81 ± 3.77 | 98.13 ± 2.98 | 98.23 ± 3.25 |
Right hand | 97.08 ± 4.74 | 98.65 ± 3.18 | 98.13 ± 3.40 | 98.23 ± 2.97 | 97.19 ± 3.70 | 98.44 ± 2.62 |
Table 3 The accuracy rate of each level in Experiment 1b (M ± SD; %)
Response hand | Up arrow | Down arrow | Bidirectional arrow | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Small number | Large number | Small number | Large number | Small number | Large number | |
Left hand | 98.33 ± 2.80 | 98.13 ± 4.11 | 97.60 ± 3.25 | 97.81 ± 3.77 | 98.13 ± 2.98 | 98.23 ± 3.25 |
Right hand | 97.08 ± 4.74 | 98.65 ± 3.18 | 98.13 ± 3.40 | 98.23 ± 2.97 | 97.19 ± 3.70 | 98.44 ± 2.62 |
Response hand | Up arrow | Down arrow | Bidirectional arrow | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Small number | Large number | Small number | Large number | Small number | Large number | |
Left hand | 489 ± 74 | 513 ± 87 | 492 ± 81 | 518 ± 85 | 489 ± 77 | 511 ± 81 |
Right hand | 505 ± 80 | 486 ± 81 | 495 ± 80 | 477 ± 78 | 505 ± 75 | 483 ± 79 |
Table 4 The reaction time of each level in Experiment 1b (M ± SD; %)
Response hand | Up arrow | Down arrow | Bidirectional arrow | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Small number | Large number | Small number | Large number | Small number | Large number | |
Left hand | 489 ± 74 | 513 ± 87 | 492 ± 81 | 518 ± 85 | 489 ± 77 | 511 ± 81 |
Right hand | 505 ± 80 | 486 ± 81 | 495 ± 80 | 477 ± 78 | 505 ± 75 | 483 ± 79 |
Response Hand | Small numbers | Large numbers | Special characters | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Left arrow | Right arrow | Left arrow | Right arrow | Left arrow | Right arrow | |
Left hand | 99.25 ± 2.13 | 99.13 ± 2.75 | 99.00 ± 2.58 | 97.38 ± 3.92 | 98.50 ± 3.24 | 98.00 ± 2.95 |
Right hand | 98.88 ± 2.11 | 98.25 ± 3.11 | 98.75 ± 2.72 | 99.25 ± 2.13 | 98.63 ± 2.99 | 99.13 ± 1.92 |
Table 5 The accuracy rate of each level in Experiment 2 (M ± SD; %)
Response Hand | Small numbers | Large numbers | Special characters | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Left arrow | Right arrow | Left arrow | Right arrow | Left arrow | Right arrow | |
Left hand | 99.25 ± 2.13 | 99.13 ± 2.75 | 99.00 ± 2.58 | 97.38 ± 3.92 | 98.50 ± 3.24 | 98.00 ± 2.95 |
Right hand | 98.88 ± 2.11 | 98.25 ± 3.11 | 98.75 ± 2.72 | 99.25 ± 2.13 | 98.63 ± 2.99 | 99.13 ± 1.92 |
Response Hand | Small numbers | Large numbers | Special characters | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Left arrow | Right arrow | Left arrow | Right arrow | Left arrow | Right arrow | |
Left hand | 388 ± 47 | 452 ± 78 | 383 ± 45 | 443 ± 69 | 383 ± 44 | 447 ± 76 |
Right hand | 437 ± 73 | 385 ± 48 | 433 ± 71 | 381 ± 51 | 440 ± 79 | 382 ± 50 |
Table 6 The reaction time of each level in Experiment 2 (M ± SD; ms)
Response Hand | Small numbers | Large numbers | Special characters | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Left arrow | Right arrow | Left arrow | Right arrow | Left arrow | Right arrow | |
Left hand | 388 ± 47 | 452 ± 78 | 383 ± 45 | 443 ± 69 | 383 ± 44 | 447 ± 76 |
Right hand | 437 ± 73 | 385 ± 48 | 433 ± 71 | 381 ± 51 | 440 ± 79 | 382 ± 50 |
Figure 8. Two-stage processing model (Yan et al., 2022). Note. The solid line represents the processing path of task related information, the dashed line represents the processing path of task unrelated information, and the lightning arrow represents that any form of interference factor applied to any link in the SNARC effect generation chain will affect the generation of the effect. Figure 8-A represents a magnitude comparison task, where magnitude information is input as task related information and parity information is input as task unrelated information; Figure 8-B represents the parity determination task, where parity information is input as task related information and size information is input as task unrelated information.
[1] |
Casarotti, M., Michielin, M., Zorzi, M. & Umiltà, C. (2007). Temporal order judgment reveals how number magnitude affects visuospatial attention. Cognition, 102(1), 101-117.
pmid: 17046735 |
[2] |
Craft, J. L., & Simon, J. R. (1970). Processing symbolic information from a visual display: Interference from an irrelevant directional cue. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83, 415-420.
pmid: 4098174 |
[3] | Dehaene, S., & Akhavein, R. (1995). Attention, automaticity, and levels of representation in number processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(2), 314-326. |
[4] | Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraus, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 122(3), 371-396. |
[5] | Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (1990). Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. Journal of experimental psychology: Human perception and performance, 16(3), 626-641. |
[6] | Dodd, M. D., Stigchel, S. V. D., Leghari, M. A., Fung, G., & Kingstone, A. (2008). Attentional SNARC: there’s something special about numbers (let us count the ways). Cognition, 108(3), 810-818. |
[7] |
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149-1160.
doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149 pmid: 19897823 |
[8] |
Fias, W., Lauwereyns, J., & Lammertyn, J. (2001). Irrelevant digits affect feature-based attention depending on the overlap of neural circuits. Cognitive Brain Research, 12(3), 415-423.
pmid: 11689301 |
[9] |
Fischer, M. H., Castel, A. D., Dodd, M. D., & Pratt, J. (2003). Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 6(6), 555-556.
pmid: 12754517 |
[10] |
Fischer, M. H., Warlop, N., Hill, R. L., & Fias, W. (2004). Oculomotor bias induced by number perception. Experimental Psychology, 51(2), 91-97.
pmid: 15114901 |
[11] | Gevers, W., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2005). Towards a common processing architecture underlying Simon and SNARC effects. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17(5), 659-673. |
[12] |
Gevers, W., Ratinckx, E., De Baene, W., & Fias, W. (2006). Further evidence that the SNARC effect is processed along a dual-route architecture. Experimental Psychology, 53(1), 58-68.
pmid: 16610273 |
[13] | Gevers, W., Santens, S., Dhooge, E., Chen, Q., Bossche, L., & Fias, W. (2010). Verbal-spatial and visuospatial coding of number-space interactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(1), 180-190. |
[14] | Gevers, W., Verguts, T., Reynvoet, B., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2006). Numbers and space: A computational model of the SNARC effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(1), 32-44. |
[15] | He, H., Yu, H. H., & Liu, J. P. (2015). Study on number processing with Global-Local aspects of compound visual stimulations. Psychological Exploration, 35(6), 499-501. |
[16] | Hedge, A., & Marsh, N. W. A. (1975). The effect of irrelevant spatial correspondences on two-choice response-time. Act Psychologica, 39, 427-439. |
[17] | Hu, C. P., Kong, X. Z., Wagenmakers, E.-J., Ly, A., & Peng, K. P. (2018). The Bayes factor and its implementation in JASP: A practical primer. Advances in Psychological Science, 26(6), 951-965. |
[18] | Jeffreys, H. (1961). Theory of probability (3rd Ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. |
[19] | Kang, W., Yang, M., & Wang, L. (2013). SNARC Effect: Current Research, Theories and Suggestions. Journal of Psychological Science, 36(5), 1242-1248. |
[20] |
Keus, I. M., Jenks, K. M., & Schwarz, W. (2005). Psychophysiological evidence that the SNARC effect has its functional locus in a response selection stage. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(1), 48-56.
pmid: 15922157 |
[21] | Keus, I. M., & Schwarz, W. (2005). Searching for the functional locus of the SNARC effect: Evidence for a response-related origin. Memory and Cognition, 33(4), 681-695. |
[22] |
Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility-a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2), 253-270.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.97.2.253 pmid: 2186425 |
[23] | Li, Q., Nan, W., Wang, K., & Liu, X. (2014). Independent processing of stimulus-stimulus and stimulus-response conficts. PLoS ONE, 9(2), e89249. |
[24] | Liu, C., & Fu, X. L. (2004). The influence of attention on the effects of number magnitude in number comparison task. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 36(3), 307-314. |
[25] | Lu, C. H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 2, 174-207. |
[26] | Luo, T., Qiu, R. Y., Chen, B., & Fu, S. M. (2018). The stimulus representation of unconscious information and its temporal characteristics. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 50(5), 473-482. |
[27] | Luo, C., & Proctor, R. W. (2021). Word-and arrow-based Simon effects emerge for eccentrically presented location words and arrows. Psychological Research, 85, 816-827. |
[28] |
Nan, W. Z., Yan, L. Z., Yang, G. C., Liu, X., & Fu, S. M. (2021). Two processing stages of the SNARC effect. Psychological Research, 86, 375-385.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-021-01506-5 pmid: 33847782 |
[29] | Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 353-383. |
[30] |
Salillas, E., Elyagoubi, R., & Semenza, C. (2008). Sensory and cognitive processes of shifts of spatial attention induced by numbers: An ERP study. Cortex, 44(4), 406-413.
doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.006 pmid: 18387572 |
[31] | Scerrati, E., Lugli, L., Nicoletti, R., & Umilta, C. (2017). Comparing Stroop-like and Simon effects on perceptual features. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 17815. |
[32] |
Schwarz, W., & Keus, I. M. (2004). Moving the eyes along the mental number line: Comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and number responses. Perception and Psychophysics, 66(4), 651-664.
pmid: 15311664 |
[33] | Shi, X. L. (2010). Spatial representations of numbers in paradigm of Global-Local processing(Unpublished master’s thesis). Soochow University. |
[34] |
Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51(3), 300-304.
pmid: 6045637 |
[35] |
Simon, J. R. (1968). Effect of ear stimulated on reaction time and movement time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 78, 344-346.
pmid: 5722451 |
[36] |
Simon, J. R., E Small, A. M., Jr. (1969). Processing auditory information: Interference from an irrelevant cue. Journal of Applied Psychology, 53, 433-435.
pmid: 5366316 |
[37] | Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276-315. |
[38] | Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643-662. |
[39] | Tlauka, M. (2002). The processing of numbers in choice-reaction tasks. Australian Journal of Psychology, 54, 94-98. |
[40] | Wagenmakers, E.-J., Love, J., Marsman, M., Jamil, T., Ly, A., Verhagen, J., … van Doorn, J. (2017). Bayesian inference for psychology. Part II: Example applications with JASP. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25, 58-76. |
[41] | Wang, Q. Q., Shi, W. D., & Ye, J. (2018). The impact effect of the task switch on the SNARC effect. Chinese Journal of Applied Psychology, 24(3), 271-279. |
[42] |
Wang, Q. Q., Zhang, Q., Shi, W. D., Wang, Z. W., & Zhang, P. C. (2022). Online construction of spatial representation of numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect in number processing in interferential situations. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 54(7), 761-771.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.00761 |
[43] | Wang, Y. H., Bergh, D., Aust, F., Ly, A., Wagenmakers, E.-J., & Hu, C. P. (2023). The Implementation of Bayesian ANOVA in JASP: A Practical Primer. Psychology: Techniques and Applications, 11(9), 528-541. |
[44] | Wu, F., Gu, Q., Shi, Z. H., Gao, Z. F., & Shen, M. W. (2018). Striding over the “Classical Statistical Inference Trap”—Application of Bayes Factors in Psychological Studies. Chinese Journal of Applied Psychology, 24(3), 195-202. |
[45] | Xu, X. D., & Liu, C. (2006). The spatial character of number. Advances in Psychological Science, 14(6), 851-858. |
[46] | Xiang, X. R., Yan, L. Z., Fu, S. M., & Nan, W. Z. (2022). Processing stage flexibility of the SNARC effect: Task relevance or magnitude relevance? Frontiers in psychology, 13, 1022999. |
[47] | Yan, L. Z., Yang, G. C., Nan, W. Z., Liu, X., & Fu, S. M. (2021). The SNARC effect occurs in the response-selection stage. Acta Psychologica, 215, 103292. |
[48] |
Yan, L. Z., Chen, Y. X., Liu, X., Fu, S. M., & Nan, W. Z. (2022). The flexibility of spatial-numerical associations and its internal mechanism. Advances in Psychological Science, 30(1), 51-64.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.00051 |
[1] | JIANG Jiali, QI Yue, LEI Xiuya, LU Lifei, YU Xiao. The development of symbolic and non-symbolic SNARC effects: The roles of phonological abilities, visuospatial abilities and working memory [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2024, 56(6): 714-730. |
[2] | WANG Qiangqiang, ZHANG Qi, SHI Wendian, WANG Zhiwei, ZHANG Pengcheng. Online construction of spatial representation of numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect in number processing in interferential situations [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2022, 54(7): 761-771. |
[3] | HAN Meng, MAO Xinrui, CAI Mengtong, JIA Xi, GUO Chunyan. The effect of positive and negative signs on the SNARC effect in the magnitude judgment task [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(8): 995-1008. |
[4] | SUN Yueliang, ZHENG Weiyi, HE Xianyou. The processing of pure decimal numbers: Selective access or parallel access? [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(5): 611-621. |
[5] | SI Jiwei;ZHOU Chao;ZHANG Chuanhua;ZHONG Leilei. SNARC Effect of Non-Symbolic Number Information on Different Processing Levels: Eyetracking Evidence [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2013, 45(1): 11-22. |
[6] | ZHANG Li;CHEN Xue-Mei;WANG Qi;LI Hong. The Influence of Body Form and Social Environment on the SNARC Effect: Based on the Embodied Cognition [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2012, 44(10): 1309-1317. |
[7] | GAO Zai-Feng,SHUI Ren-De,CHEN Jing,CHEN Wen,TIAN Ying,,SHEN Mo-Wei. The Mechanism of Negative Numbers’ Spatial Representation [J]. , 2009, 41(02): 95-102. |
[8] | Zhu Xiangru,Liu Chang. Conflict Adaptation under the SNARC Effect: An ERP Study [J]. , 2008, 40(03): 283-290. |
[9] | Liu Chao,Mai Xiaoqin, Fu Xiaolan. THE SPATIAL NUMERICAL ASSOCIATION OF RESPONSE CODES EFFECT OF NUMBER PROCESSING IN DIFFERENT ATTENTION CONDITIONS [J]. , 2004, 36(06): 671-680. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||