[1] |
Barr D. J., Levy R., Scheepers C., & Tily H. J . (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255-278.
|
[2] |
Bates D., Mächler M., Bolker B., & Walker S . (2014). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. ArXiv preprint arXiv:1406.5823.
|
[3] |
Bernolet S., Collina S., & Hartsuiker R. J . (2016). The persistence of syntactic priming revisited. Journal of Memory and Language, 91, 99-116.
|
[4] |
Bernolet S., & Hartsuiker R. J . (2010). Does verb bias modulate syntactic priming? Cognition, 114(3), 455-461.
|
[5] |
Bock K . (1986). Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18(3), 355-387.
|
[6] |
Bock K . (1989). Closed-class immanence in sentence production. Cognition, 31(2), 163-186.
|
[7] |
Bock K., & Levelt W. J. M . (1994). Language production: Grammatical encoding. In: Gernsbacher, M. (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics. San Diego: Academic Press, 945-984.
|
[8] |
Bock K., & Loebell H . (1990). Framing sentences. Cognition, 35(1), 1-39.
|
[9] |
Branigan H. P., Pickering M. J., & Cleland A. A . (1999). Syntactic priming in written production: Evidence for rapid decay. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6(4), 635-640.
|
[10] |
Cai Z. G., Pickering M. J., & Branigan H. P . (2012). Mapping concepts to syntax: Evidence from structural priming in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Memory and Language, 66(4), 833-849.
|
[11] |
Cai Z. G., Pickering M. J., Yan H., & Branigan H. P . (2011). Lexical and syntactic representations in closely related languages: Evidence from Cantonese-Mandarin bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 65(4), 431-445.
|
[12] |
Chang F., Dell G. S., & Bock K . (2006). Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review, 113(2), 234-272.
|
[13] |
Chang F., Dell G. S., Bock K., & Griffin Z. M . (2000). Structural priming as implicit learning: A comparison of models of sentence production. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(2), 217-230.
|
[14] |
Ferreira V. S., & Bock K . (2006). The functions of structural priming. Language and Cognition Neuroscience, 21(7-8), 1011-1029.
|
[15] |
Hardy S. M., Messenger K., & Maylor E. A . (2017). Aging and syntactic representations: Evidence of preserved syntactic priming and lexical boost. Psychology and Aging, 32(6), 588-596.
|
[16] |
Hartsuiker R. J., Bernolet S., Schoonbaert S., Speybroeck S., & Vanderelst D . (2008). Syntactic priming persists while the lexical boost decays: Evidence from written and spoken dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(2), 214-238.
|
[17] |
Huang J., Pickering M. J., Yang J., Wang S., & Branigan H. P . (2016). The independence of syntactic processing in Mandarin: Evidence from structural priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 91, 81-98.
|
[18] |
Jaeger T. F . (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 434-446.
|
[19] |
Kutas M., & Federmeier K. D . (2011). Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621-647.
|
[20] |
Levelt W. J. M., & Kelter S . (1982). Surface form and memory in question answering. Cognitive Psychology, 14(1), 78-106.
|
[21] |
Levelt W. J. M., Roelofs A., & Meyer A. S . (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(1), 1-75.
|
[22] |
Mahowald K., James A., Futrell R., & Gibson E . (2016). A meta-analysis of syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 91, 5-27.
|
[23] |
Melinger A., & Dobel C . (2005). Lexically-driven syntactic priming. Cognition, 98(1), B11-B20.
|
[24] |
Pickering M. J., & Branigan H. P . (1998). The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 39(4), 633-651.
|
[25] |
Segaert K., Menenti L., Weber K., & Hagoort P . (2011). A paradox of syntactic priming: Why response tendencies show priming for passives, and response latencies show priming for actives. PloS One, 6(10), e24209.
|
[26] |
Segaert K., Weber K., Cladder-Micus M., & Hagoort P . (2014). The influence of verb-bound syntactic preferences on the processing of syntactic structures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(5), 1448-1460.
|
[27] |
Segaert K., Wheeldon L., & Hagoort P . (2016). Unifying structural priming effects on syntactic choices and timing of sentence generation. Journal of Memory and Language, 91, 59-80.
|
[28] |
Shao J . (2004). Semantic Grammar: A brief introduction. Journal of Jinan University (Humanities and Social Sciences), 26(1), 100-106.
|
|
[ 邵敬敏 . (2004). “语义语法”说略. 暨南学报(哲学社会科学版), 26(1), 100-106.]
|
[29] |
Xiao R., Mcenery T., & Qian Y . (2006). Passive constructions in English and Chinese: A corpus-based contrastive study. Languages in Contrast, 6(1), 109-149.
|
[30] |
Yang C. L., Perfetti C. A., & Liu Y . (2010). Sentence integration processes: An ERP study of Chinese sentence comprehension with relative clauses. Brain and Language, 112(2), 85-100.
|
[31] |
Yang J., & Zhang Y . (2007). Syntactic priming in sentence production. Advances in Psychological Science, 15(2), 288-294.
|
|
[ 杨洁, 张亚旭 . (2007). 句子产生中的句法启动. 心理科学进展, 15(2), 288-294.]
|
[32] |
Yang Q., & Zhang Q . (2015). Aging of speech production: Behavioral and neural mechanisms. Advances in Psychological Science, 23(12), 2072-2084.
|
|
[ 杨群, 张清芳 . (2015). 口语产生中的认知年老化及其神经机制. 心理科学进展, 23(12), 2072-2084.]
|