[1] |
Barber H., Vergara M., & Carreiras M . ( 2004). Syllable-frequency effects in visual word recognition: Evidence from ERPs. Neuroreport, 15(3), 545-548.
|
[2] |
Carrasco-Ortiz H., Midgley K. J., Grainger J., & Holcomb P. J . ( 2017). Interactions in the neighborhood: Effects of orthographic and phonological neighbors on N400 amplitude. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 41, 1-10.
|
[3] |
Chen B. G., Liu Y., Wang L. X., Peng D. L., & Perfetti C. A . ( 2007). The timing of graphic, phonological and semantic activation of high and low frequency Chinese character: An ERP study. Progress in Nature Science, 17(13), 62-70.
|
[4] |
Chen X.K, & Zhang J.J, . ( 2012). Role of familiarity of semantic radicals in the recognition of lowly familiar Chinese characters. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 44(7), 882-895.
|
|
[ 陈新葵, 张积家 . ( 2012). 义符熟悉性对低频形声字词汇通达的影响. 心理学报, 44(7), 882-895.]
|
[5] |
Ganis G., Kutas M., & Sereno M . ( 1996). The search for “common sense”: An electrophysiological study of the comprehension of words and pictures in reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8(2), 89-106.
|
[6] |
Hsiao J. H. W., Shillcock R., & Lavidor M . ( 2006). A TMS examination of semantic radical combinability effects in Chinese character recognition. Brain Research, 1078(1), 159-167.
|
[7] |
Hsiao J. H. W., Shillcock R., & Lavidor M . ( 2007). An examination of semantic radical combinability effects with lateralized cues in Chinese character recognition. Perception and Psychophysics, 69(3), 338-344.
|
[8] |
Hsu C. H., Tsai J. L., Lee C. Y, & Tzeng, O. J. L . ( 2009). Orthographic combinability and phonological consistency effects in reading Chinese phonograms: An event-related potential study. Brain and Language, 108(1), 56-66.
|
[9] |
Hung Y. H., Hung D. L., Tzeng O. J. L., & Wu D. H . ( 2014). Tracking the temporal dynamics of the processing of phonetic and semantic radicals in Chinese character recognition by MEG. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 29, 42-65.
|
[10] |
Kounios J., & Holcomb P.J . ( 1994). Concreteness effects in semantic processing: ERP evidence supporting dual-coding theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(4), 804-823.
|
[11] |
Lee C. Y., Tsai J. L., Chan W. H., Hsu C. H., Hung D. L., & Tzeng O. J . ( 2007). Temporal dynamics of the consistency effect in reading Chinese: An event-related potentials study. Neuroreport, 18(2), 147-151.
|
[12] |
Li Y. & Kang J. S. , ( 1993) . The phonetic radical study of modern Chinese phongram. In Y. Chen, Anaysis of modern Chinese character information (pp. 96). Shanghai: Shanghai Education Press.
|
|
[ 李燕, 康加深 . ( 1993). 现代汉语形声字声符研究. 见: 陈原. 现代汉语用字信息分析 (p. 96). 上海: 上海教育出版社.]
|
[13] |
Liu Y., Perfetti C. A., & Hart L . ( 2003). ERP evidence for the time course of graphic, phonological, and semantic information in Chinese meaning and pronunciation decision . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(6), 1231-1247.
|
[14] |
Taler V., &Phillips N.A . ( 2007). Event-related brain potential evidence for early effects of neighborhood density in word recognition. Neuroreport, 18(18), 1957-1961.
|
[15] |
Voss J.L., Federmeier K.D . ( 2010). FN400 potentials are functionally identical to N400 potentials and reflect semantic processing during recognition testing. Psychophysiology, 48(4), 532-546.
|
[16] |
Wang J., & Zhang J.J . ( 2016). The effects of category consistency and neighborhood size of the semantic radical on the semantic processing of Chinese character. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 48(11), 1390-1400.
|
|
[ 王娟, 张积家 . ( 2016). 义符的类别一致性和家族大小影响形声字的语义加工. 心理学报, 48(11), 1390-1400.]
|
[17] |
Wang X. S., Wu Y., Zhao S. M., Ni C., & Zhang M . ( 2016). The effects of semantic radicals and phonetic radicals in Chinese phonogram recognition. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 48(2), 130-140.
|
|
[ 王协顺, 吴岩, 赵思敏, 倪超, 张明 . ( 2016). 形旁和声旁在形声字识别中的作用. 心理学报, 48(2), 130-140.]
|
[18] |
Williams C. , ( 2012). Emerging development of semantic and phonological routes to character decoding in Chinese as a foreign language learners. Reading and Writing, 26(2), 293-315.
|
[19] |
Williams C., & Bever T. , ( 2010). Chinese character decoding: A semantic bias? Reading and Writing, 23(5), 589-605.
|
[20] |
Zhang J. J., Wang J., & Yin C . ( 2014). The role of phonetic radicals and semantic radicals in phonetics and semantics extraction of phonogram characters: An eye movement study on components perception. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 46(7), 885-900.
|
|
[ 张积家, 王娟, 印丛 . ( 2014). 义符和声符在形声字语音、语义提取中的作用—来自部件知觉眼动研究的证据. 心理学报, 46(7), 885-900.]
|
[21] |
Zhang J.J., & Zhang Y.Z, . ( 2016). The time course of semantic radical’s semantic and syntax activation under radical prime paradigm. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 48(9), 1070-1081.
|
|
[ 张积家, 章玉祉 . ( 2016). 义符启动范式下义符的语义和语法激活的时间进程. 心理学报, 48(9), 1070-1081.]
|
[22] |
Zhang Q., Zhang J. X., & Kong L. Y . ( 2009). An ERP study on the time course of phonological and semantic activation in Chinese word recognition. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 73(3), 235-245.
|
[23] |
Zhang Y.Z., & Zhang J.J, . ( 2017). The effects of neighborhood size and category consistency of the semantic radical on semantic radical’s semantic activation under radical priming paradigm. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 49(8), 1041-1052.
|
|
[ 章玉祉, 张积家 . ( 2017). 义符启动范式下家族大小和类别一致性对义符语义激活的影响. 心理学报, 49(8), 1041-1052.]
|
[24] |
Zhou X.L., & Marslen-Wilson W. , ( 1999). The nature of sublexical processing in reading Chinese characters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(4), 819-837.
|