心理学报 ›› 2024, Vol. 56 ›› Issue (2): 161-178.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2024.00161
收稿日期:
2022-07-21
发布日期:
2023-12-01
出版日期:
2024-02-25
通讯作者:
周欣悦, E-mail: xinyuezhou@zju.edu.cn
作者简介:
数据源自速途元宇宙研究院联合北京城市副中心应用场景产业联盟发布的《2022虚拟人产业研究报告》。
基金资助:
YAN Xiao, MO Tiantian, ZHOU Xinyue()
Received:
2022-07-21
Online:
2023-12-01
Published:
2024-02-25
摘要:
近年来, 社交网络上涌现出大量具有真人特征的虚拟人物, 他们以第一人称视角叙事以获得影响力, 然而其背后的道德伦理问题也逐渐浮现。当社交媒体虚拟人犯错时, 不同文化背景的人们对其道德判断会有何不同?本文基于心智感知理论, 通过5个主要实验探索中西方文化差异对虚拟人道德责任判断的影响和机制。研究表明, 在被告知虚拟人的不道德行为后, 比起西方文化, 中国文化影响下的人们认为虚拟人需要承担更大的道德责任; 但这种文化差异没有出现在对真人的道德责任判断上(实验1a~1c)。并且不论虚拟人是由真实人类还是人工智能驱动, 这种文化差异都会存在(实验1c)。这种文化差异通过感知心智能力来中介(实验2)。具体而言, 比起西方文化, 中国文化影响下的人们认为虚拟人的心智能力(特别是感知力)更高, 因此需要承担更大的道德责任。此外, 对虚拟人更大的道德责任判断会导致中国文化(相比西方文化)影响下的人们更倾向于对虚拟人施加道德惩罚(实验3)。本文通过实证研究将道德责任判断和心智感知的对象拓展到虚拟人上, 并揭示了中西方文化差异及其后续影响。
中图分类号:
闫霄, 莫田甜, 周欣悦. (2024). 中西方文化差异对虚拟人道德责任判断的影响. 心理学报, 56(2), 161-178.
YAN Xiao, MO Tiantian, ZHOU Xinyue. (2024). The influence of cultural differences between China and the West on moral responsibility judgments of virtual humans. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 56(2), 161-178.
[1] |
Adler, N. E., Epel, E. S., Castellazzo, G., & Ickovics, J. R. (2000). Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: Preliminary data in healthy white women. Health Psychology, 19(6), 586-592.
doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.19.6.586 pmid: 11129362 |
[2] |
Allem, J.-P., Escobedo, P., & Dharmapuri, L. (2020). Cannabis surveillance with Twitter data: Emerging topics and social bots. American Journal of Public Health, 110(3), 357-362.
doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2019.305461 URL |
[3] | Appel, J., von Der Pütten, A., Krämer, N. C., & Gratch, J. (2012). Does humanity matter? Analyzing the importance of social cues and perceived agency of a computer system for the emergence of social reactions during human- computer interaction. Advances in Human-Computer Interaction, 2012, 1-10. |
[4] | Aristotle. (1999). Nicomachean ethics (T. Irwin. Trans. & Ed.). Cambridge (2nd ed.). Hackett. |
[5] |
Arsenyan, J., & Mirowska, A. (2021). Almost human? A comparative case study on the social media presence of virtual influencers. International Journal of Human- Computer Studies, 155, 102694.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2021.102694 URL |
[6] |
Awad, E., Levine, S., Kleiman-Weiner, M., Dsouza, S., Tenenbaum, J. B., Shariff, A.,... Rahwan, I. (2020). Drivers are blamed more than their automated cars when both make mistakes. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(2), 134-143.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0762-8 pmid: 31659321 |
[7] |
Bigman, Y. E., & Gray, K. (2018). People are averse to machines making moral decisions. Cognition, 181, 21-34.
doi: S0010-0277(18)30208-7 pmid: 30107256 |
[8] |
Broadbent, E. (2017). Interactions with robots: The truths we reveal about ourselves. Annual Review of Psychology, 68(1), 627-652.
doi: 10.1146/psych.2017.68.issue-1 URL |
[9] |
Bryson, J. J., Diamantis, M. E., & Grant, T. D. (2017). Of, for, and by the people: The legal lacuna of synthetic persons. Artificial Intelligence and Law, 25(3), 273-291.
doi: 10.1007/s10506-017-9214-9 URL |
[10] |
Buchtel, E. E., Guan, Y., Peng, Q., Su, Y., Sang, B., Chen, S. X., & Bond, M. H. (2015). Immorality east and west: Are immoral behaviors especially harmful, or especially uncivilized? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(10), 1382-1394.
doi: 10.1177/0146167215595606 pmid: 26253486 |
[11] |
Cameron, C. D., Payne, B. K., & Knobe, J. (2010). Do theories of implicit race bias change moral judgments? Social Justice Research, 23(4), 272-289.
doi: 10.1007/s11211-010-0118-z URL |
[12] |
Carlson, B. D., Donavan, D. T., Deitz, G. D., Bauer, B. C., & Lala, V. (2020). A customer-focused approach to improve celebrity endorser effectiveness. Journal of Business Research, 109, 221-235.
doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2019.11.048 URL |
[13] | Champagne, M., & Tonkens, R. (2015). Bridging the responsibility gap in automated warfare. Philosophy & Technology, 28(1), 125-137. |
[14] |
Chen, H., Bolton, L. E., Ng, S., Lee, D., & Wang, D. (2018). Culture, relationship norms, and dual entitlement. Journal of Consumer Research, 45(1), 1-20.
doi: 10.1093/jcr/ucx118 URL |
[15] |
Choi, I., Koo, M., & Choi, J. A. (2007). Individual differences in analytic versus holistic thinking. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(5), 691-705.
pmid: 17440200 |
[16] | Chu, H. D., Li, Y. Y., Ye, J. H., Hu, F. P., He, Q., & Zhao, L. (2019). Moral judgment about human and robot agents in personal and impersonal dilemmas. Chinese Journal of Applied Psychology, 25(3), 262-271. |
[褚华东, 李园园, 叶君惠, 胡凤培, 何铨, 赵雷. (2019). 个人-非个人道德困境下人对智能机器道德判断研究. 应用心理学, 25(3), 262-271.] | |
[17] |
Clarke, R. (1992). Free will and the conditions of moral responsibility. Philosophical Studies: An International Journal for Philosophy in the Analytic Tradition, 66(1), 53-72.
doi: 10.1007/BF00668395 URL |
[18] |
Coeckelbergh, M. (2020). Artificial intelligence, responsibility attribution, and a relational justification of explainability. Science and Engineering Ethics, 26(4), 2051-2068.
doi: 10.1007/s11948-019-00146-8 pmid: 31650511 |
[19] | Constantinescu, M., Vică, C., Uszkai, R., & Voinea, C. (2022). Blame it on the AI? On the moral responsibility of Artificial Moral Advisors. Philosophy & Technology, 35(2), 1-26. |
[20] | Cowell, J. M., Lee, K., Malcolm‐Smith, S., Selcuk, B., Zhou, X., & Decety, J. (2017). The development of generosity and moral cognition across five cultures. Developmental Science, 20(4), e12403. |
[21] |
Dang, J., & Liu, L. (2022). Implicit theories of the human mind predict competitive and cooperative responses to AI robots. Computers in Human Behavior, 134, 107300.
doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2022.107300 URL |
[22] |
Dietze, P., & Knowles, E. D. (2016). Social class and the motivational relevance of other human beings: Evidence from visual attention. Psychological Science, 27(11), 1517-1527.
pmid: 27698091 |
[23] |
Dietze, P., & Knowles, E. D. (2021). Social class predicts emotion perception and perspective-taking performance in adults. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 47(1), 42-56.
doi: 10.1177/0146167220914116 URL |
[24] | Eyssel, F. A., & Pfundmair, M. (2015, August). Predictors of psychological anthropomorphization, mind perception, and the fulfillment of social needs: A case study with a zoomorphic robot. In 2015 24th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication (RO-MAN) (pp. 827-832). IEEE, Kobe, Japan. |
[25] |
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191.
doi: 10.3758/BF03193146 URL |
[26] | Feller, M. (2018). We can't Get Enough of This Drama Between Two ‘Virtual Instagram Influencer’. Retrieved July 10, 2023, from https://www.elle.com/culture/tech/a19849553/lil-miquela-bermuda-instagram-virtual-influencer-hacking/ |
[27] | Fuller, C. J. (2004). The camphor flame: Popular Hinduism and society in India. Princeton University Press. |
[28] |
Graham, J., Meindl, P., Beall, E., Johnson, K. M., & Zhang, L. (2016). Cultural differences in moral judgment and behavior, across and within societies. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 125-130.
doi: S2352-250X(15)00233-X pmid: 29506787 |
[29] |
Gray, H. M., Gray, K., & Wegner, D. M. (2007). Dimensions of mind perception. Science, 315(5812), 619-619.
pmid: 17272713 |
[30] |
Gray, K., Knobe, J., Sheskin, M., Bloom, P., & Barrett, L. F. (2011). More than a body: Mind perception and the nature of objectification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(6), 1207-1220.
doi: 10.1037/a0025883 pmid: 22059848 |
[31] |
Gray, K., & Wegner, D. M. (2009). Moral typecasting: Divergent perceptions of moral agents and moral patients. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(3), 505-520.
doi: 10.1037/a0013748 pmid: 19254100 |
[32] |
Gray, K., & Wegner, D. M. (2012). Feeling robots and human zombies: Mind perception and the uncanny valley. Cognition, 125(1), 125-130.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.007 pmid: 22784682 |
[33] |
Gray, K., Young, L., & Waytz, A. (2012). Mind perception is the essence of morality. Psychological Inquiry, 23(2), 101-124.
doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2012.651387 pmid: 22754268 |
[34] |
Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293(5537), 2105-2108.
doi: 10.1126/science.1062872 pmid: 11557895 |
[35] |
Haidt, J., Koller, S. H., & Dias, M. G. (1993). Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(4), 613-628.
pmid: 8229648 |
[36] |
Hakli, R., & Mäkelä, P. (2019). Moral responsibility of robots and hybrid agents. The Monist, 102(2), 259-275.
doi: 10.1093/monist/onz009 URL |
[37] |
Han, M. C., & Kim, Y. (2018). How culture and friends affect acceptance of social media commerce and purchase intentions: A comparative study of consumers in the U.S. and China. Journal of International Consumer Marketing, 30(5), 326-335.
doi: 10.1080/08961530.2018.1466226 URL |
[38] |
Hayes, A. F. (2015). An index and test of linear moderated mediation. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 50(1), 1-22.
doi: 10.1080/00273171.2014.962683 pmid: 26609740 |
[39] |
Himma, K. E. (2009). Artificial agency, consciousness, and the criteria for moral agency: What properties must an artificial agent have to be a moral agent? Ethics and Information Technology, 11(1), 19-29.
doi: 10.1007/s10676-008-9167-5 URL |
[40] | Hou, Y. B., & Zhu, Y. (2002). The effect of culture on thinking style of Chinese people. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 34(1), 106-111. |
[侯玉波, 朱滢. (2002). 文化对中国人思维方式的影响. 心理学报, 34(1), 106-111.] | |
[41] |
Huang, Z. H., Jing, Y. M., Yu, F., Gu, R. L., Zhou, X. Y., & Cai, H. J. (2018). Increasing individualism and decreasing collectivism? Cultural and psychological change around the globe. Advances in Psychological Science, 26(11), 2068-2080.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.02068 |
[黄梓航, 敬一鸣, 喻丰, 古若雷, 周欣悦, 张建新, 蔡华俭. (2018). 个人主义上升, 集体主义式微?——全球文化变迁与民众心理变化. 心理科学进展, 26(11), 2068-2080.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.02068 |
|
[42] |
Johnson, A. H., & Barrett, J. (2003). The role of control in attributing intentional agency to inanimate objects. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 3(3), 208-217.
doi: 10.1163/156853703322336634 URL |
[43] | Kim, D., & Jo, D. (2022). Effects on co-presence of a virtual human: A comparison of display and interaction types. Electronics, 11(3), 367. |
[44] | Kollanyi, B., Howard, P. N., & Woolley, S. C. (2016). Bots and automation over Twitter during the first U.S. presidential debate. Comprop Data Memo, 1, 1-4. |
[45] | Krumhuber, E. G., Swiderska, A., Tsankova, E., Kamble, S. V., & Kappas, A. (2015). Real or artificial? Intergroup biases in mind perception in a cross-cultural perspective. PLoS One, 10(9), e0137840. |
[46] |
Letheren, K., Kuhn, K. A. L., Lings, I., & Pope, N. K. L. (2016). Individual difference factors related to anthropomorphic tendency. European Journal of Marketing, 50(5/6), 973-1002.
doi: 10.1108/EJM-05-2014-0291 URL |
[47] | Liu, B. H., & Peng, K. P. (2012). Challenge and contribution of cultural psychology to empirical legal studies. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 44(3), 413-426. |
[刘邦惠, 彭凯平. (2012). 跨文化的实证法学研究:文化心理学的挑战与贡献. 心理学报, 44(3), 413-426.] | |
[48] | Lynn, R. (2007). Virtual rape is traumatic, but is it a crime? Retrieved September 19, 2022, from https://www.wired.com/2007/05/sexdrive-0504/ |
[49] | Malle, B. F., Scheutz, M., Arnold, T., Voiklis, J., & Cusimano, C. (2015, March). Sacrifice one for the good of many? People apply different moral norms to human and robot agents. In 2015 10th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI) (pp. 117-124). IEEE, Portland, USA: ACM. |
[50] | McGuire, B. F. (2018). Buddhist uploads. In M. Bass and D.W. Pasulka (Eds.), Posthumanism: The future of Homo Sapiens (pp. 143-153). New York: Macmillan. |
[51] |
Miller, J. G., & Bersoff, D. M. (1992). Culture and moral judgment: How are conflicts between justice and interpersonal responsibilities resolved? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(4), 541-554.
pmid: 1583583 |
[52] | Mo, T., Sui, J., Zhao, Y., & Zhou, X. (2022). Moral positive illusion: Self-other valuation difference in moral foundation theory. Ethics & Behavior, 1-18. |
[53] |
Morling, B. (2016). Cultural difference, inside and out. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10(12), 693-706.
doi: 10.1111/spc3.v10.12 URL |
[54] |
Na, J., Grossmann, I., Varnum, M. E., Kitayama, S., Gonzalez, R., & Nisbett, R. E. (2010). Cultural differences are not always reducible to individual differences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(14), 6192-6197.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1001911107 pmid: 20308553 |
[55] | Ohnuma, R. (2007). Head, eyes, flesh, and blood: Giving away the body in Indian Buddhist literature. Columbia University Press. |
[56] |
Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128(1), 3-72.
pmid: 11843547 |
[57] |
Parthemore, J., & Whitby, B. (2014). Moral agency, moral responsibility, and artifacts: What existing artifacts fail to achieve (and why), and why they, nevertheless, can (and do!) make moral claims upon us. International Journal of Machine Consciousness, 6(2), 141-161.
doi: 10.1142/S1793843014400162 URL |
[58] | Peng, K. P., Yu, F., & Bai, Y. (2011). Experimental ethics: New challenges and contributions to the understanding of human moral behaviors. Social Sciences in China, 182(6), 15-25. |
[彭凯平, 喻丰, 柏阳. (2011). 实验伦理学: 研究、贡献与挑战. 中国社会科学, 182(6), 15-25.] | |
[59] |
Rhim, J., Lee, G.-B., & Lee, J.-H. (2020). Human moral reasoning types in autonomous vehicle moral dilemma: A cross-cultural comparison of Korea and Canada. Computers in Human Behavior, 102, 39-56.
doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.08.010 URL |
[60] |
Samuel, G. (1989). The body in Buddhist and Hindu tantra: Some notes. Religion, 19(3), 197-210.
doi: 10.1016/0048-721X(89)90019-5 URL |
[61] |
Sands, S., Campbell, C., Plangger, K., & Ferraro, C. (2022). Unreal influence: Leveraging AI in influencer marketing. European Journal of Marketing, 56(6), 1721-1747.
doi: 10.1108/EJM-12-2019-0949 URL |
[62] |
Schein, C., & Gray, K. (2018). The theory of dyadic morality: Reinventing moral judgment by redefining harm. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 22(1), 32-70.
doi: 10.1177/1088868317698288 pmid: 28504021 |
[63] |
Shoemaker, D. (2011). Attributability, answerability, and accountability: Toward a wider theory of moral responsibility. Ethics, 121(3), 602-632.
doi: 10.1086/659003 URL |
[64] |
Sui, J., & Mo, T. (2022). Morality in the era of smart devices. International Journal of Emerging Markets, 17(4), 1107-1122.
doi: 10.1108/IJOEM-06-2021-0917 URL |
[65] |
Sullivan, Y. W., & Wamba, F. S. (2022). Moral judgments in the age of artificial intelligence. Journal of Business Ethics, 178(4), 917-943.
doi: 10.1007/s10551-022-05053-w |
[66] |
Sung, E. C., Han, D.-I. D., Bae, S., & Kwon, O. (2022). What drives technology-enhanced storytelling immersion? The role of digital humans. Computers in Human Behavior, 132, 107246.
doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2022.107246 URL |
[67] |
Tharp, M., Holtzman, N. S., & Eadeh, F. R. (2017). Mind perception and individual differences: A replication and extension. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 39(1), 68-73.
doi: 10.1080/01973533.2016.1256287 URL |
[68] |
Triandis, H. C., & Gelfand, M. J. (1998). Converging measurement of horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(1), 118-128.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.118 URL |
[69] | van Genderen, H. (2018). Do we need new legal personhood in the age of robots and AI? In M. Corrales, M. Fenwick, N. Forgó (Eds.), Robotics, AI and the Future of Law (pp. 15-55). Springer, Singapore. |
[70] |
Vauclair, C.-M., & Fischer, R. (2011). Do cultural values predict individuals' moral attitudes? A cross‐cultural multilevel approach. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41(5), 645-657.
doi: 10.1002/ejsp.v41.5 URL |
[71] |
Volante, M., Babu, S. V., Chaturvedi, H., Newsome, N., Ebrahimi, E., Roy, T.,... Fasolino, T. (2016). Effects of virtual human appearance fidelity on emotion contagion in affective inter-personal simulations. IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics, 22(4), 1326-1335.
doi: 10.1109/TVCG.2945 URL |
[72] | Wang, E. J., & Yue, G. A. (2006). Moral diversity between eastern and western cultures — A analysis in the perspective of cultural psychology. Morality and Civilization, (2), 52-56. |
[王恩界, 乐国安. (2006). 东西方文化背景下的道德观差异——来自于文化心理学视角的分析. 道德与文明, (2), 52-56.] | |
[73] |
Ward, A. F., Olsen, A. S., & Wegner, D. M. (2013). The harm-made mind: Observing victimization augments attribution of minds to vegetative patients, robots, and the dead. Psychological Science, 24(8), 1437-1445.
doi: 10.1177/0956797612472343 pmid: 23749051 |
[74] |
Waytz, A., Gray, K., Epley, N., & Wegner, D. M. (2010). Causes and consequences of mind perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(8), 383-388.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.006 pmid: 20579932 |
[75] | Willard, A. K., & McNamara, R. A. (2019). The minds of god (s) and humans: Differences in mind perception in Fiji and North America. Cognitive Science, 43(1), e12703. |
[76] |
Wu, S., & Keysar, B. (2007). The effect of culture on perspective taking. Psychological Science, 18(7), 600-606.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01946.x pmid: 17614868 |
[77] |
Young, A. D., & Monroe, A. E. (2019). Autonomous morals: Inferences of mind predict acceptance of AI behavior in sacrificial moral dilemmas. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 85, 103870.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2019.103870 URL |
[78] |
Zimmerman, M. J. (1997). Moral responsibility and ignorance. Ethics, 107(3), 410-426.
doi: 10.1086/233742 URL |
[1] | 魏新东, 汪凤炎. 自我-朋友冲突情境下智慧推理的文化差异及其机制[J]. 心理学报, 2021, 53(11): 1244-1259. |
[2] | 任志洪, 张雅文, 江光荣. 正念冥想对焦虑症状的干预: 效果及其影响因素元分析[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(3): 283-305. |
[3] | 柯江林,孙健敏,李永瑞. 心理资本: 本土量表的开发及中西比较[J]. 心理学报, 2009, 41(09): 875-888. |
[4] | 崔红,王登峰. 中西方“外向性”人格维度的内涵分析——中西方人格量表在中国人群中的测量[J]. 心理学报, 2006, 38(03): 414-421. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||