[1] |
Blicher, D. L., Diehl, R. L., & Cohen, L. B . (1990). Effects of syllable duration on the perception of the Mandarin tone 2/tone 3 distinction: Evidence of auditory enhancement. Journal of Phonetics, 18(1), 37-49.
|
[2] |
Chandrasekaran, B., Krishnan, A., & Gandour, J. T . (2007). Mismatch negativity to pitch contours is influenced by language experience. Brain Research, 1128, 148-156.
|
[3] |
Cheng, Y. Y., & Lee, C. Y . (2018). The development of mismatch responses to Mandarin lexical tone in 12-to 24-month-old infants. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 448-459.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00448
URL
|
[4] |
Cheng, Y. Y., Wu, H. C., Tzeng, Y. L., Yang, M. T., Zhao, L. L., & Lee, C. Y . (2013). The development of mismatch responses to Mandarin lexical tones in early infancy. Developmental Neuropsychology, 38(5), 281-300.
doi: 10.1080/87565641.2013.799672
URL
|
[5] |
He, C., Hotson, L., & Trainor, L. J . (2007). Mismatch responses to pitch changes in early infancy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(5), 878-892.
|
[6] |
He, C., Hotson, L., & Trainor, L. J . (2009a). Development of infant mismatch responses to auditory pattern changes between 2 and 4 months old. European Journal of Neuroscience, 29(4), 861-867.
|
[7] |
He, C., Hotson, L., & Trainor, L. J . (2009b). Maturation of cortical mismatch responses to occasional pitch change in early infancy: Effects of presentation rate and magnitude of change. Neuropsychologia, 47(1), 218-229.
|
[8] |
Hua, Z . (2002). Phonological development in specific contexts: studies of Chinese-speaking children. Child language and development. Australian Review of Applied Linguistics, 29(1), 12.1-12.5.
|
[9] |
Kaan, E., Wayland, R., Bao, M., & Barkley, C. M . (2007). Effects of native language and training on lexical tone perception: An event-related potential study. Brain Research, 1148, 113-122.
|
[10] |
Kuo, Y. C., Lee, C. Y., Chen, M. C., Liu, T. L., & Cheng, S. K . (2014). The impact of spectral resolution on the mismatch response to mandarin Chinese tones: An ERP study of cochlear implant simulations. Clinical Neurophysiology, 125(8), 1568-1575.
|
[11] |
Kushnerenko, E., Ceponiene, R., Balan, P., Fellman, V., & Näätänen, R . (2002). Maturation of the auditory change detection response in infants: A longitudinal ERP study. Neuroreport, 13(15), 1843-1848.
|
[12] |
Lee, C. Y., Yen, H. L., Yeh, P. W., Lin, W. H., Cheng, Y. Y., Tzeng, Y. L., & Wu, H. C . (2012). Mismatch responses to lexical tone, initial consonant, and vowel in Mandarin- speaking preschoolers. Neuropsychologia, 50(14), 3228-3239.
|
[13] |
Ma, W., Zhou, P., Singh, L., & Gao, L . (2017). Spoken word recognition in young tone language learners: Age- dependent effects of segmental and suprasegmental variation. Cognition, 159, 139-155.
|
[14] |
Morr, M. L., Shafer, V. L., Kreuzer, J. A., & Kurtzberg, D . (2002). Maturation of mismatch negativity in typically developing infants and preschool children. Ear and Hearing, 23(2), 118-136.
|
[15] |
Näätänen, R., Paavilainen, P., Rinne, T., & Alho, K . (2007). The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: A review. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118(12), 2544-2590.
|
[16] |
Sun, Q . (2016). ERP study on pre-attentive processing of Chinese tones in adults and young children (Unpublished Master's dissertation). Liaoning Normal University.
|
|
[ 孙琪 . (2016). 汉语声调加工的ERP研究 (硕士学位论文). 辽宁师范大学. ]
|
[17] |
Trainor, L. J., Samuel, S. S., Desjardins, R. N., & Sonnadara, R. R . (2001). Measuring temporal resolution in infants using mismatch negativity. Neuroreport, 12(11), 2443-2448.
|
[18] |
Tsao, F. M . (2017). Perceptual improvement of lexical tones in infants: Effects of tone language experience. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 558.
|
[19] |
Wang, X. D., Wang, M., & Chen, L . (2013). Hemispheric lateralization for early auditory processing of lexical tones: Dependence on pitch level and pitch contour. Neuropsychologia, 51(11), 2238-2244.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.07.015
URL
|
[20] |
Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C . (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7(1), 49-63.
|
[21] |
Winkler, I . (2007). Interpreting the mismatch negativity. Journal of Psychophysiology, 21(3-4), 147-163.
|
[22] |
Wong, P., Schwartz, R. G., & Jenkins, J. J . (2005). Perception and production of lexical tones by 3-year-old, Mandarin-speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(5), 1065-1079.
|
[23] |
Xi, J., Zhang, L., Shu, H., Zhang, Y., & Li, P . (2010). Categorical perception of lexical tones in Chinese revealed by mismatch negativity. Neuroscience, 170(1), 223-231.
|
[24] |
Yang, Y. F. (Ed).(2016). Rating scales for children’s developmental behavior and mental health. Beijing, China: People's Medical Publishing House.
|
|
[ 杨玉凤 (编). (2016). 儿童发育行为心理评定量表. 北京: 人民卫生出版社.]
|
[25] |
Yu, K., Wang, R., Li, L., & Li, P . (2014). Processing of acoustic and phonological information of lexical tones in Mandarin Chinese revealed by mismatch negativity. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8(3), 729.
|
[26] |
Yu, K., Zhou, Y., Li, L., Su, J. A., Wang, R., & Li, P . (2017). The interaction between phonological information and pitch type at pre-attentive stage: An ERP study of lexical tones. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 32(9), 1164-1175.
doi: 10.1080/23273798.2017.1310909
URL
|