Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2023, Vol. 31 ›› Issue (8): 1429-1442.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2023.01429
• Conceptual Framework • Previous Articles Next Articles
RAN Yaxuan(), CAI Hui, ZHANG Yunhan, HAN Xinying
Received:
2023-01-14
Online:
2023-08-15
Published:
2023-05-12
CLC Number:
RAN Yaxuan, CAI Hui, ZHANG Yunhan, HAN Xinying. The maximizing bias and behavioral effects of joint consumption: A perspective of group mental accounting[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(8): 1429-1442.
效应 | 作者 | 消费类型 | 主要内容 |
---|---|---|---|
标签效应 | Thaler, | 单独消费 | 人们倾向于使用小额的意外之财来购买那些他们平常不会消费的商品。 |
Thaler, | 单独消费 | 人们更愿意使用礼品卡来进行享乐消费。 | |
Reinholtz et al., | 单独消费 | 当使用某品牌礼品卡时, 人们更倾向于购买该品牌旗下最具代表性的商品。 | |
Chuang et al., | 单独消费 | 经历服务结果失败的人更期待获得心理补救(如诚挚的道歉), 而经历服务过程失败的人更期待获得实物补救(如全额退款)。 | |
预算效应 | Soman & Gourville, | 单独消费 | 当某个心理账户内的累计消费额接近人们对这个账户预期的预算时, 他们在很大概率上会停止或者抑制在该领域上的消费。 |
Kivetz, | 单独消费 | 个人有时会利用心理账户来放纵自己的行为, 通过做出预先承诺来满足购买非必要享乐商品的欲望。 | |
Huebner et al., | 单独消费 | 当面临日常消费时, 人们会灵活地将某项消费划分到还有预算余额的账户; 当面临非日常消费时, 人们会无意识放宽对这个账户的财务预算。 | |
Cheema & Soman, | 单独消费 | 灵活心理账户存在两种作用机制:一是将模糊的支出灵活地划分到不同的心理账户中; 二是专门建立新的心理账户存放这些模糊支出。 | |
Loureiro & Haws, | 单独消费 | 消费者的心情和认知负荷情况会影响他们是否选择采取这个心理账户机制来合理化自己的过度消费行为。 |
效应 | 作者 | 消费类型 | 主要内容 |
---|---|---|---|
标签效应 | Thaler, | 单独消费 | 人们倾向于使用小额的意外之财来购买那些他们平常不会消费的商品。 |
Thaler, | 单独消费 | 人们更愿意使用礼品卡来进行享乐消费。 | |
Reinholtz et al., | 单独消费 | 当使用某品牌礼品卡时, 人们更倾向于购买该品牌旗下最具代表性的商品。 | |
Chuang et al., | 单独消费 | 经历服务结果失败的人更期待获得心理补救(如诚挚的道歉), 而经历服务过程失败的人更期待获得实物补救(如全额退款)。 | |
预算效应 | Soman & Gourville, | 单独消费 | 当某个心理账户内的累计消费额接近人们对这个账户预期的预算时, 他们在很大概率上会停止或者抑制在该领域上的消费。 |
Kivetz, | 单独消费 | 个人有时会利用心理账户来放纵自己的行为, 通过做出预先承诺来满足购买非必要享乐商品的欲望。 | |
Huebner et al., | 单独消费 | 当面临日常消费时, 人们会灵活地将某项消费划分到还有预算余额的账户; 当面临非日常消费时, 人们会无意识放宽对这个账户的财务预算。 | |
Cheema & Soman, | 单独消费 | 灵活心理账户存在两种作用机制:一是将模糊的支出灵活地划分到不同的心理账户中; 二是专门建立新的心理账户存放这些模糊支出。 | |
Loureiro & Haws, | 单独消费 | 消费者的心情和认知负荷情况会影响他们是否选择采取这个心理账户机制来合理化自己的过度消费行为。 |
[1] | 李爱梅, 凌文辁. (2007). 心理账户:理论与应用启示. 心理科学进展, 15(5), 727-734. |
[2] | 冉雅璇, 李志强, 牛熠欣, 陈斯允. (2022a). “共买+共用”:共有消费的研究评述与未来展望. 珞珈管理评论, 40(1), 137-152. |
[3] | 冉雅璇, 张谱月, 陈斯允, 项典典. (2022b). 有“新”同享:共有消费促进对不熟悉产品的选择. 心理学报, 54(8), 979-995. |
[4] |
Campbell, D. T. (1958). Common fate, similarity, and other indices of the status of aggregates of persons as social entities. Behavioral Science, 3(1), 14-25.
doi: 10.1002/bs.3830030103 URL |
[5] |
Cheema, A., & Soman, D. (2006). Malleable mental accounting: The effect of flexibility on the justification of attractive spending and consumption decisions. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 16(1), 33-44.
doi: 10.1207/s15327663jcp1601_6 URL |
[6] |
Chowdhury, T. G., Ratneshwar, S., & Mohanty, P. (2009). The time-harried shopper: Exploring the differences between maximizers and satisficers. Marketing Letters, 20, 155-167.
doi: 10.1007/s11002-008-9063-0 URL |
[7] |
Chuang, S. C., Cheng, Y. H., Chang, C. J., & Yang, S. W. (2012). The effect of service failure types and service recovery on customer satisfaction: A mental accounting perspective. The Service Industries Journal, 32(2), 257-271.
doi: 10.1080/02642069.2010.529435 URL |
[8] |
Dar-Nimrod, I., Rawn, C. D., Lehman, D. R., & Schwartz, B. (2009). The maximization paradox: The costs of seeking alternatives. Personality and Individual Differences, 46(5-6), 631-635.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.01.007 URL |
[9] |
Dzhogleva, H., & Lamberton, C. P. (2014). Should birds of a feather flock together? Understanding self-control decisions in dyads. Journal of Consumer Research, 41(6), 361-380.
doi: 10.1086/676599 URL |
[10] |
Eliaz, K., Ray, D., & Razin, R. (2006). Choice shifts in groups: A decision-theoretic basis. American Economic Review, 96(4), 1321-1332.
doi: 10.1257/aer.96.4.1321 URL |
[11] |
Etkin, J. (2016). Choosing variety for joint consumption. Journal of Marketing Research, 53(6), 1019-1033.
doi: 10.1509/jmr.14.0209 URL |
[12] |
Fei, X., You, Y., & Yang, X. (2020). “We” are different: Exploring the diverse effects of friend and family accessibility on consumers’ product preferences. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 30(3), 543-550.
doi: 10.1002/jcpy.v30.3 URL |
[13] |
Garbinsky, E. N., & Gladstone, J. J. (2018). The consumption consequences of couples pooling finances. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 29(3), 353-369.
doi: 10.1002/jcpy.2019.29.issue-3 URL |
[14] |
Gorlin, M., & Dhar, R. (2012). Bridging the gap between joint and individual decisions: Deconstructing preferences in relationships. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22(3), 320-323.
doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2012.05.002 URL |
[15] |
Heath, C., & Soll, B. J. (1996). Mental budgeting and consumer decisions. Journal of Consumer Research, 23(1), 40-52.
doi: 10.1086/jcr.1996.23.issue-1 URL |
[16] | Huebner, J., Fleisch, E., & Ilic, A. (2020). Assisting mental accounting using smartphones: Increasing the salience of credit card transactions helps consumer reduce their spending. Computers in Human Behavior, 113, 1-13. |
[17] |
Iyengar, S. S., Wells, R. E., & Schwartz, B. (2006). Doing better but feeling worse: Looking for the “best” jobs undermines satisfaction. Psychological Science, 17, 143-150.
pmid: 16466422 |
[18] |
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1984). Choices, values, and frames. American Psychologist, 39(4), 341-350.
doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.39.4.341 URL |
[19] |
Kim, K., & Miller, E. G. (2017). Vulnerable maximizers: The role of decision difficulty. Judgment and Decision Making, 12(5), 516-526.
doi: 10.1017/S1930297500006537 URL |
[20] |
Kivetz, R. (1999). Advances in research on mental accounting and reason-based choice. Marketing Letters, 10(3), 249-266.
doi: 10.1023/A:1008066718905 URL |
[21] |
Lee, Y. H., & Qiu, C. (2009). When uncertainty brings pleasure: The role of prospect imageability and mental imagery. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(4), 624-633.
doi: 10.1086/599766 URL |
[22] |
Liu, P. J., Dallas, S. K., & Fitzsimons, G. J. (2019). A framework for understanding consumer choices for others. Journal of Consumer Research, 46(3), 407-434.
doi: 10.1093/jcr/ucz009 |
[23] |
Liu, P. J., & Min, K. (2020). Where do you want to go for dinner? A preference expression asymmetry in joint consumption. Journal of Marketing Research, 57(6), 1037-1054.
doi: 10.1177/0022243720949497 URL |
[24] |
Loureiro, Y. K., & Haws, K. L. (2015). Positive affect and malleable mental accounting: An investigation of the role of positive affect in flexible expense categorization and spending. Psychology & Marketing, 32(6), 670-677.
doi: 10.1002/mar.20808 URL |
[25] |
Lowe, M., Nikolova, H., Miller, C. J., & Dommer, S. L. (2019). Ceding and succeeding: How the altruistic can benefit from the selfish in joint decisions. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 29(4), 652-661.
doi: 10.1002/jcpy.v29.4 URL |
[26] |
Luan, M., Fu, L., & Li, H. (2018). Do maximizers maximize for others? Self-other decision-making differences in maximizing and satisficing. Personality and Individual Differences, 121, 52-56.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.09.009 URL |
[27] | Luan, M., & Li, H. (2019). Do maximizers maximize in private? The influence of public versus private context on maximizing. Personality and Individual Differences, 150, 109-481. |
[28] |
Ma, J., & Roese, N. J. (2014). The maximizing mind-set. Journal of Consumer Research, 41(1), 71-92.
doi: 10.1086/674977 URL |
[29] |
Mann, R. P. (2020). Collective decision-making by rational agents with differing preferences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 117(19), 10388-10396.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.2000840117 URL |
[30] |
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224-253.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224 URL |
[31] |
Nenkov, G. Y., Morrin, M., Ward, A., Schwartz, B., & Hulland, J. (2008). A short form of the maximization scale: Factor structure, reliability and validity studies. Judgment and Decision Making, 3, 371-388.
doi: 10.1017/S1930297500000395 URL |
[32] |
Nikolova, H., Lamberton, C., & Coleman, N. V. (2018). Stranger danger: When and why consumer dyads behave less ethically than individuals. Journal of Consumer Research, 45(1), 90-108.
doi: 10.1093/jcr/ucx108 URL |
[33] |
Oishi, S., Tsutsui, Y., Eggleston, C., & Galinha, I. C. (2014). Are maximizers unhappier than satisficers? A comparison between Japan and the USA. Journal of Research in Personality, 49, 14-20.
doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2013.12.001 URL |
[34] |
Peetz, J., & Howard, A. (2021). Balancing prosocial effort across social categories: Mental accounting heuristics in helping decisions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 47(9), 1414-1429.
doi: 10.1177/0146167220976683 pmid: 33272101 |
[35] |
Purvis, A., Howell, R. T., & Iyer, R. (2011). Exploring the role of personality in the relationship between maximization and well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 370-375.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.10.023 URL |
[36] | Reinholtz, N., Bartels, D. M., & Parker, J. R. (2015). On the mental accounting of restricted-use funds: How gift cards change what people purchase. Journal of Consumer Research, 42(4), 596-614. |
[37] |
Rossignac-Milon, M., Bolger, N., Zee, K. S., Boothby, E. J., & Higgins, E. T. (2021). Merged minds: Generalized shared reality in dyadic relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 120(4), 1-30.
doi: 10.1037/pspa0000246 URL |
[38] | Schultze, T., Mojzisch, A., & Schulz-Hardt, S. (2019). Why dyads heed advice less than individuals do. Judgment and Decision Making, 14(3), 349-363. |
[39] |
Schwartz, B., Ward, A., Monterosso, J., Lyubomirsky, S., White, K., & Lehman, D. R. (2002). Maximizing versus satisficing: Happiness is a matter of choice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1178-1197.
pmid: 12416921 |
[40] |
Shefrin, H. M., & Thaler, R. H. (1988). The behavioral life-cycle hypothesis. Economic Inquiry, 26(4), 609-643.
doi: 10.1111/ecin.1988.26.issue-4 URL |
[41] |
Simpson, J. A., Griskevicius, V., & Rothman, A. J. (2012). Consumer decisions in relationships. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22(3), 304-314.
doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2011.09.007 URL |
[42] |
Soman, D., & Gourville, J. T. (2001). Transaction decoupling: How price bundling affects the decision to consume. Journal of Marketing Research, 38(1), 30-44.
doi: 10.1509/jmkr.38.1.30.18828 URL |
[43] |
Steffel, M., & Le Boeuf, R. A. (2014). Overindividuation in gift giving: Shopping for multiple recipients leads givers to choose unique but less preferred gifts. Journal of Consumer Research, 40(6), 1167-1180.
doi: 10.1086/674199 URL |
[44] |
Stillman, P. E., Fujita, K., Sheldon, O., & Trope, Y. (2018). From “me” to “we”: The role of construal level in promoting maximized joint outcomes. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 147, 16-25.
doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2018.05.004 URL |
[45] |
Thaler, R. H. (1980). Toward a positive theory of consumer choice. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 1(1), 39-60.
doi: 10.1016/0167-2681(80)90051-7 URL |
[46] |
Thaler, R. H. (1985). Mental accounting and consumer choice. Marketing Science, 4(3), 199-214.
doi: 10.1287/mksc.4.3.199 URL |
[47] |
Thaler, R. H. (1999). Mental accounting matters. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 12(3), 183-206.
doi: 10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0771 URL |
[48] |
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1991). Loss aversion in riskless choice: A reference-dependent model. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 106(4), 1039-1061.
doi: 10.2307/2937956 URL |
[49] |
Vanbergen, N., Irmak, C., & Sevilla, J. (2020). Product entitativity: How the presence of product replicates increases perceived and actual product efficacy. Journal of Consumer Research, 47(2), 192-214.
doi: 10.1093/jcr/ucaa006 URL |
[50] | Ward, M. K., & Broniarczyk, S. M. (2016). Ask and you shall (not) receive: Close friends prioritize relational signaling over recipient preferences in their gift choices. Journal of Marketing Research, 53(6), S270-S287. |
[51] |
Winet, Y. K., Tu, Y., Choshen-Hillel, S., & Fishbach, A. (2022). Social exploration: When people deviate from options explored by others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 122(3), 427-442.
doi: 10.1037/pspi0000350 URL |
[52] |
Wu, E. C., Moore, S. G., & Fitzsimons, G. J. (2019). Wine for the table: Self-construal, group size, and choice for self and others. Journal of Consumer Research, 46(3), 508-527.
doi: 10.1093/jcr/ucy082 URL |
[53] |
Yang, L. W., Chartrand, T. L., & Fitzsimons, G. J. (2015). The influence of gender and self-monitoring on the products consumers choose for joint consumption. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 32(4), 398-407.
doi: 10.1016/j.ijresmar.2015.05.008 URL |
[54] | Zein, M. E., Bahrami, B., & Hertwig, R. (2019). Shared responsibility in collective decisions. Nature, 3(6), 554-559. |
[55] |
Zhu, X., Dalal, D. K., & Hwang, T. (2017). Is maximizing a bad thing? Linking maximizing tendency to positive outcomes through future-oriented thinking. Journal of Individual Differences, 38(2), 94-101.
doi: 10.1027/1614-0001/a000226 URL |
[1] | WANG Chujun, WAN Xiaoang. The “double-edged sword” effect of eating together on food consumption and its mechanisms [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(7): 1318-1330. |
[2] | XIE Zhipeng, QIN Huanyu, WANG Ziye, WANG Jingyuan, HE Yi. Tainted or elegant? Sexy effect on marketing [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 0, (): 0-0. |
[3] | WANG Xingang, LI Zulan, ZHANG Ting. Double valence coping strategies for damaged brands in the social media environment: From the perspective of group polarization theory [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(5): 709-720. |
[4] | LIU Hongyan, ZHENG Yun, LIU Yaozhong, QIAO Fei. The double-edged sword effect of consumers’ photo-taking behavior on consumption experience [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(3): 480-491. |
[5] | YANG Zhichao, WANG Ting. Zero in consumer decision-making: The zero-price effect and the zero-comparison effect [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(3): 492-506. |
[6] | ZHANG Hao, XIAO Bangming, HUANG Minxue. The in-feed native advertising avoidance mechanism and re-targeting strategy based on user dynamic information processing mode [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(2): 223-239. |
[7] | HUANG Jianping, XU Jingxian, WAN Xiaoang. Influence of associative learning on consumer behavior: From the perspective of product search experience [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(11): 2414-2423. |
[8] | LI Danhui, DU Jiangang, LI Xiaonan. The effect of gaze cues on consumers [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(11): 2607-2618. |
[9] | LI Chunqing, ZHANG Jieli, LIU Wei, ZHANG Chenlu. The relationship between customer experience journey and input with digitalized interactive platforms offerings [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(11): 2424-2447. |
[10] | LIU Wumei, XIAO Haiyi, WANG Xuefeng. Residential mobility and consumption: Explanation based on regulatory focus theory [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(11): 2595-2606. |
[11] | ZHAO Na, QIN Xuezhe, LIU Yaqian, SUN Ling. You get what you pay for? The mechanisms and moderators of price-quality effect [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(10): 2372-2380. |
[12] | JIN Fei. How sharing on social media influences consumer choices [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(8): 1785-1793. |
[13] | ZHANG Sha, XU Mengchen, JIANG Peiqi, ZHAO Hong. Understanding local community customers: Perspectives from place attachment and customer satiation [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(7): 1482-1495. |
[14] | LI Xi, LI Tongmao, HU Jihao. The mechanism and influences of multilingual packaging strategy for tourism product: From the perspective of selective accessibility model [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(6): 1216-1229. |
[15] | ZENG Fue, CHEN Wenbin, HE Qiong. The characteristics of consumers’ new product adoption [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(6): 1350-1366. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||