Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2021, Vol. 29 ›› Issue (5): 796-805.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2021.00796
• Regular Articles • Previous Articles Next Articles
XIAO Chengli(), SUI Yuqing, XIAO Suheng, ZHOU Renlai()
Received:
2020-07-03
Online:
2021-05-15
Published:
2021-03-30
Contact:
XIAO Chengli,ZHOU Renlai
E-mail:xiaocl@nju.edu.cn;rlzhou@nju.edu.cn
CLC Number:
XIAO Chengli, SUI Yuqing, XIAO Suheng, ZHOU Renlai. A new perspective on spatial interaction research: The effects of multiple social factors[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2021, 29(5): 796-805.
社会因素 | 空间语言交流 | 视角采择 | |
---|---|---|---|
个体自身的 社会认知特质 | 认知风格(刘丽虹 等, | 共情能力(Mattan et al. | |
空间参考框架偏好(Vukovic & Williams, | 精神病态(Drayton et al. | ||
自闭症(Pearson et al. | |||
社交与沟通能力(Clements-Stephens et al. | |||
想象力(Muto et al. | |||
交互对象的 社会属性 | 人/非人属性 | 人vs. 没有人(Clinton et al. | 人vs. 木块(Samson et al. |
人 vs. 机器人(Carlson et al. | 人 vs. 箭头(Santiesteban et al. | ||
人 vs. 箭头 vs. 方块(Nielsen et al. | |||
人 vs. 台灯(Ward et al. | |||
内群体成员 vs. 外群体成员(Simpson & Todd, | |||
虚拟同伴 vs. 真实同伴(Elekes et al. | |||
交互对象的能力和状态 | 合作者的视角(Galati et al. | 交互对象的视线(Cole et al. | |
合作者的空间能力(Schober, | |||
合作者与目标物是否存在动作交互(Furlanetto et al. | |||
空间环境中的社会线索 | 空间朝向和合作者朝向的一致性(Galati et al. | 同时包含方向线索和社会线索的交互对象(Gunalp et al. | |
物体的可供性(Millett et al. | 任务无关他人的存在(Ward et al. |
社会因素 | 空间语言交流 | 视角采择 | |
---|---|---|---|
个体自身的 社会认知特质 | 认知风格(刘丽虹 等, | 共情能力(Mattan et al. | |
空间参考框架偏好(Vukovic & Williams, | 精神病态(Drayton et al. | ||
自闭症(Pearson et al. | |||
社交与沟通能力(Clements-Stephens et al. | |||
想象力(Muto et al. | |||
交互对象的 社会属性 | 人/非人属性 | 人vs. 没有人(Clinton et al. | 人vs. 木块(Samson et al. |
人 vs. 机器人(Carlson et al. | 人 vs. 箭头(Santiesteban et al. | ||
人 vs. 箭头 vs. 方块(Nielsen et al. | |||
人 vs. 台灯(Ward et al. | |||
内群体成员 vs. 外群体成员(Simpson & Todd, | |||
虚拟同伴 vs. 真实同伴(Elekes et al. | |||
交互对象的能力和状态 | 合作者的视角(Galati et al. | 交互对象的视线(Cole et al. | |
合作者的空间能力(Schober, | |||
合作者与目标物是否存在动作交互(Furlanetto et al. | |||
空间环境中的社会线索 | 空间朝向和合作者朝向的一致性(Galati et al. | 同时包含方向线索和社会线索的交互对象(Gunalp et al. | |
物体的可供性(Millett et al. | 任务无关他人的存在(Ward et al. |
[1] | 刘丽虹, 张积家, 王惠萍. (2005). 习惯的空间术语对空间认知的影响. 心理学报, 37(4),469-475. |
[2] | 肖承丽, 范丫, 徐刘飞, 周仁来. (2019). 以人为中心的人-机器人自然空间语言交互. 应用心理学, 25(4),319-331. |
[3] | 张越, 葛贤亮, 田志强, 葛列众. (2018). 基于空间的一级和二级视角转换的行为研究及理论综述. 心理科学, 41(2),504-509. |
[4] | 赵婧, 王璐, 苏彦捷. (2010). 视觉观点采择的发生发展及其影响因素. 心理发展与教育, 26(1),107-111. |
[5] | 赵显, 刘力, 张笑笑, 向振东, 付洪岭. (2012). 观点采择: 概念、操纵及其对群际关系的影响. 心理科学进展, 20(12),2079-2088. |
[6] |
Apperly, I. A., & Butterfill, S. A. (2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological Review, 116(4),953-970.
doi: 10.1037/a0016923 URL pmid: 19839692 |
[7] | Burigo, M., & Schultheis, H. (2018). The effects of direction and orientation of located objects on spatial language comprehension. Language and Cognition, 10(2),298-328. |
[8] | Carlson, L. A. (1999). Selecting a reference frame. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 1,365-379. |
[9] |
Carlson, L. A., Skubic, M., Miller, J., Huo, Z., & Alexenko, T. (2014). Strategies for human-driven robot comprehension of spatial descriptions by older adults in a robot fetch task. Topics in Cognitive Science, 6(3),513-533.
URL pmid: 24948449 |
[10] | Clark, H. H., & Murphy, G. L. (1982). Audience design in meaning and reference. Advances in Psychology, 9,287-299. |
[11] | Clark, H. H., & Wilkes-Gibbs, D. (1986). Referring as a collaborative process. Cognition, 22(1),1-39. |
[12] |
Clements-Stephens, A. M., Vasiljevic, K., Murray, A. J., & Shelton, A. L. (2013). The role of potential agents in making spatial perspective taking social. Frontiers In Human Neuroscience, 7,497.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00497 URL pmid: 24046735 |
[13] | Clinton, J. A., Magliano, J. P., & Skowronski, J. J. (2018). Gaining perspective on spatial perspective taking. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 30(1),85-97. |
[14] |
Cole, G. G., Atkinson, M., Le, A. T. D., & Smith, D. T. (2016). Do humans spontaneously take the perspective of others? Acta Psychologica, 164,165-168.
URL pmid: 26826864 |
[15] |
Conway, J .R., Lee, D., Ojaghi, M., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2017). Submentalizing or mentalizing in a level 1 perspective-taking task: A cloak and goggles test. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(3),454-465.
URL pmid: 27893269 |
[16] |
Creem-Regehr, S. H., Gagnon, K. T., Geuss, M. N., & Stefanucci, J. K. (2013). Relating spatial perspective taking to the perception of other's affordances: Providing a foundation for predicting the future behavior of others. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7,596.
URL pmid: 24068992 |
[17] | Drayton, L. A., Santos, L. R., & Baskin-Sommers, A. (2018). Psychopaths fail to automatically take the perspective of others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(13),3302-3307. |
[18] |
Duran, N. D., Dale, R., & Kreuz, R. J. (2011). Listeners invest in an assumed other’s perspective despite cognitive cost. Cognition, 121(1),22-40.
URL pmid: 21752357 |
[19] |
Elekes, F., Varga, M., & Király, I. (2016). Evidence for spontaneous level-2 perspective taking in adults. Consciousness and cognition, 41,93-103.
URL pmid: 26897297 |
[20] | Elekes, F., Varga, M., & Király, I. (2017). Level-2 perspectives computed quickly and spontaneously: Evidence from eight-to 9.5-year-old children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 35(4),609-622. |
[21] | Erle, T. M., Ruessmann, J. K., & Topolinski, S. (2018). The effects of visuo-spatial perspective-taking on trust. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 79,34-41. |
[22] | Fischer, T., & Demiris, Y. (2016,May). Markerless perspective taking for humanoid robots in unconstrained environments. Paper presented at the meeting of IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation, Stockholm, Sweden. |
[23] | Flavell, J. H., Everett, B. A., Croft, K., & Flavell, E. R. (1981). Young children's knowledge about visual perception: Further evidence for the level 1-level 2 distinction. Developmental Psychology, 17(1),99-103. |
[24] |
Freundlieb, M., Kovács, Á. M., & Sebanz, N. (2018). Reading your mind while you are reading—Evidence for spontaneous visuospatial perspective taking during a semantic categorization task. Psychological Science, 29(4),614-622.
URL pmid: 29447070 |
[25] |
Furlanetto, T., Becchio, C., Samson, D., & Apperly, I. A. (2016). Altercentric interference in level 1 visual perspective taking reflects the ascription of mental states, not submentalizing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(2),158-163.
URL pmid: 26389611 |
[26] | Furlanetto, T., Cavallo, A., Manera, V., Tversky, B., & Becchio, C. (2013). Through your eyes: Incongruence of gaze and action increases spontaneous perspective taking. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7,455. |
[27] |
Galati, A., & Avraamides, M. N. (2013). Flexible spatial perspective-taking: Conversational partners weigh multiple cues in collaborative tasks. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7,618.
URL pmid: 24133432 |
[28] |
Galati, A., & Avraamides, M. N. (2015). Social and representational cues jointly influence spatial perspective-taking. Cognitive Science, 39(4),739-765.
URL pmid: 25243841 |
[29] | Galati, A., Dale, R., & Duran, N. D. (2019). Social and configural effects on the cognitive dynamics of perspective-taking. Journal of Memory and Language, 104,1-24. |
[30] | Galati, A., Diavastou, A., & Avraamides, M. N. (2018). Signatures of cognitive difficulty in perspective-taking: Is the egocentric perspective always the easiest to adopt? Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 33(4),467-493. |
[31] | Galati, A., Michael, C., Mello, C., Greenauer, N. M., & Avraamides, M. N. (2013). The conversational partner’s perspective affects spatial memory and descriptions. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(2),140-159. |
[32] | Galati, A., Panagiotou, E., Tenbrink, T., & Avraamides, M. N. (2018). Dynamic strategy selection in collaborative spatial tasks. Discourse Processes, 55(8),643-665. |
[33] |
Gunalp, P., Moossaian, T., & Hegarty, M. (2019). Spatial perspective taking: Effects of social, directional, and interactive cues. Memory & Cognition, 47,1031-1043.
URL pmid: 30783909 |
[34] |
Heyes, C. (2014). Submentalizing: I am not really reading your mind. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(2),131-143.
URL pmid: 26173251 |
[35] | Knowles, M. L. (2014). Social rejection increases perspective taking. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 55,126-132. |
[36] | Kuhn, G., Vacaityte, I., D'Souza, A. D. C., Millett, A. C., & Cole, G. G. (2018). Mental states modulate gaze following, but not automatically. Cognition, 180,1-9. |
[37] | Levelt, W. J. M (1984). Some perceptual limitations on talking about space. In A. J. van Doorn, W. A. van der Grindand, & J. J. Koenderink (Eds.), Limits in perception (pp.323-358). VNU Science Press. |
[38] | Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. MIT Press. |
[39] | Levinson, S. C. (2003). Space in language and cognition: Explorations in cognitive diversity. Cambridge University Press. |
[40] |
Li, J., & Zhang, K. (2009). Regional differences in spatial frame of reference systems for people in different areas of China. Perceptual And Motor Skills, 108(2),587-596.
URL pmid: 19544964 |
[41] | Li, S., Scalise, R., Admoni, H., Rosenthal, S., & Srinivasa, S. S. (2016,August). Spatial references and perspective in natural language instructions for collaborative manipulation. Paper presented at the meeting of the 25th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication, New York, NY. |
[42] | Li, X., Carlson, L. A., Mou, W., Williams, M. R., & Miller, J.E. (2011). Describing spatial locations from perception and memory: The influence of intrinsic axes on reference object selection. Journal of Memory and Language, 65(2),222-236. |
[43] | Mattan, B. D., Rotshtein, P., & Quinn, K. A. (2016). Empathy and visual perspective-taking performance. Cognitive Neuroscience, 7(1-4),170-181. |
[44] |
Mazzarella, E., Hamilton, A., Trojano, L., Mastromauro, B., & Conson, M. (2012). Observation of another's action but not eye gaze triggers allocentric visual perspective. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(12),2447-2460.
URL pmid: 22901326 |
[45] |
Millett, A. C., D’Souza, A. D. C., & Cole, G. G. (2019). Attribution of vision and knowledge in “spontaneous perspective taking”. Psychological Research, 84,1758-1765.
URL pmid: 31025107 |
[46] |
Muto, H., Matsushita, S., & Morikawa, K. (2019). Object’s symmetry alters spatial perspective-taking processes. Cognition, 191,103987.
URL pmid: 31234116 |
[47] |
Nielsen, M. K., Slade, L., Levy, J. P., & Holmes, A. (2015). Inclined to see it your way: Do altercentric intrusion effects in visual perspective taking reflect an intrinsically social process? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68(10),1931-1951.
doi: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1023206 URL pmid: 25849956 |
[48] | Pearson, A., Ropar, D., & de C Hamilton, A. F. (2013). A review of visual perspective taking in autism spectrum disorder. Frontiers In Human Neuroscience, 7,652. |
[49] | Pellicano, E., & Rhodes, G. (2003). The role of eye-gaze in understanding other minds. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21(1),33-43. |
[50] | Pick, H. L., & Lockman, J. J. (1981). From frames of reference to spatial representations. In L. S. Liben, A. H. Patterson, & N. Newcombe (Eds.), Spatial representation and behaviour across the life span: Theory and application (pp.39-60). Academic Press. |
[51] | Pouliquen-Lardy, L., Milleville-Pennel, I., Guillaume, F., & Mars, F. (2016). Remote collaboration in virtual reality: Asymmetrical effects of task distribution on spatial processing and mental workload. Virtual Reality, 20,213-220. |
[52] | Quesque, F., Chabanat, E., & Rossetti, Y. (2018). Taking the point of view of the blind: Spontaneous level-2 perspective-taking in irrelevant conditions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 79,356-364. |
[53] |
Richard, L., & Waller, D. (2013). Toward a definition of intrinsic axes: The effect of orthogonality and symmetry on the preferred direction of spatial memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(6),1914-1920.
URL pmid: 23751008 |
[54] | Ryskin, R. A., Brown-Schmidt, S., Canseco-Gonzalez, E., Yiu, L. K., & Nguyen, E. T. (2014). Visuospatial perspective-taking in conversation and the role of bilingual experience. Journal of Memory and Language, 74,46-79. |
[55] |
Ryskin, R. A., Wang, R. F., & Brown-Schmidt, S. (2016). Listeners use speaker identity to access representations of spatial perspective during online language comprehension. Cognition, 147,75-84.
URL pmid: 26638050 |
[56] | Samson, D., Apperly, I. A., Braithwaite, J. J., Andrews, B.J., & Bodley Scott, S.E. (2010). Seeing it their way: Evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(5),1255-1266. |
[57] |
Santiesteban, I., Catmur, C., Hopkins, S. C., Bird, G., & Heyes, C. (2014). Avatars and arrows: Implicit mentalizing or domain-general processing? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(3),929-937.
URL pmid: 24377486 |
[58] | Schober, M. F. (2009). Spatial dialogue between partners with mismatched abilities. In K.R. Coventry, T. Tenbrink, & J. A. Bateman (Eds.), Spatial language and dialogue(pp.23-39). Oxford University Press. |
[59] | Shelton, A. L., Clements-Stephens, A. M., Lam, W. Y., Pak, D. M., & Murray, A. J. (2012). Should social savvy equal good spatial skills? The interaction of social skills with spatial perspective taking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(2),199-205. |
[60] |
Shelton, A. L., & McNamara, T. P. (2001). Visual memories from nonvisual experiences. Psychological Science, 12(4),343-347.
URL pmid: 11476104 |
[61] |
Shelton, A. L., & McNamara, T. P. (2004). Spatial memory and perspective taking. Memory & Cognition, 32,416-426.
URL pmid: 15285125 |
[62] |
Simpson, A. J., & Todd, A. R. (2017). Intergroup visual perspective-taking: Shared group membership impairs self-perspective inhibition but may facilitate perspective calculation. Cognition, 166,371-381.
URL pmid: 28605699 |
[63] |
Surtees, A., Apperly, I. A., & Samson, D. (2013). Similarities and differences in visual and spatial perspective-taking processes. Cognition, 129(2),426-438.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.06.008 URL pmid: 23999408 |
[64] |
Surtees, A., Apperly, I. A., & Samson, D. (2016). I’ve got your number: Spontaneous perspective-taking in an interactive task. Cognition, 150,43-52.
URL pmid: 26848735 |
[65] | Surtees, A. D. R., Butterfill, S. A., & Apperly, I. A. (2012). Direct and indirect measures of Level-2 perspective-taking in children and adults. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(1),75-86. |
[66] |
Surtees, A., Samson, D., & Apperly, I. A. (2016). Unintentional perspective-taking calculates whether something is seen, but not how it is seen. Cognition, 148,97-105.
URL pmid: 26752604 |
[67] |
Todd, A. R., Cameron, C. D., & Simpson, A. J. (2017). Dissociating processes underlying level-1 visual perspective taking in adults. Cognition, 159,97-101.
URL pmid: 27915132 |
[68] |
Tosi, A., Pickering, M. J., & Branigan, H. P. (2020). Speakers' use of agency and visual context in spatial descriptions. Cognition, 194,104070.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104070 URL pmid: 31669857 |
[69] | Trafton, J. G., Cassimatis, N. L., Bugajska, M. D., Brock, D. P., Mintz, F. E., & Schultz, A. C. (2005). Enabling effective human-robot interaction using perspective-taking in robots. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics - Part A: Systems and Humans, 35(4),460-470. |
[70] |
Tversky, B., & Hard, B. M. (2009). Embodied and disembodied cognition: Spatial perspective-taking. Cognition, 110(1),124-129.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.10.008 URL pmid: 19056081 |
[71] | Tversky, B., Lee, P., & Mainwaring, S. (1999). Why do speakers mix perspectives? Spatial Cognition and Computation, 1,399-412. |
[72] |
Vukovic, N., & Williams, J. N. (2015). Individual differences in spatial cognition influence mental simulation of language. Cognition, 142,110-122.
URL pmid: 26036923 |
[73] |
Ward, E., Ganis, G., & Bach, P. (2019). Spontaneous vicarious perception of the content of another’s visual perspective. Current Biology, 29(5),874-880.
doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.01.046 URL pmid: 30799242 |
[74] |
Ward, E., Ganis, G., McDonough, K. L., & Bach, P. (2020). Perspective taking as virtual navigation? Perceptual simulation of what others see reflects their location in space but not their gaze. Cognition, 199,104241.
URL pmid: 32105910 |
[75] | Zhao, X., Cusimano, C., & Malle, B. F. (2015,July). In search of triggering conditions for spontaneous visual perspective taking. Paper presented at the meeting of the 37th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Pasadena, CA. |
[76] | Zhao, X., Cusimano, C., & Malle, B. F. (2016,March). Do people spontaneously take a robot's visual perspective? Paper presented at the meeting of the 11th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction, Christchurch, New Zealand. |
[77] |
Zwickel, J. (2009). Agency attribution and visuospatial perspective taking. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16,1089-1093.
URL pmid: 19966260 |
[1] | CHENG Jieting, SHI Mengwei. Perceived warmth and competence: The role of physiological cues in social cognition [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2023, 31(3): 443-454. |
[2] | YIN Rong. Comparative studies of mind reading: Similarities and differences in theory of mind between non-human primates and humans and corresponding explanations [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2022, 30(11): 2540-2557. |
[3] | WEN Fangfang, KE Wenlin, ZUO Bin, DAI Yuee, NIE Siyuan, YAO Yi, HAN Shi. Implicit relational assessment procedure (IRAP): Measuring principle and applications [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2021, 29(11): 1936-1947. |
[4] | HUANG Xuzhe, CHU Xiaowei, LIU Qingqi, ZHOU Zongkui, FAN Cuiying. Bystander behavior in cyberbullying [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(7): 1248-1257. |
[5] | WANG Hongyu, CUI Zhisong, ZOU Chunlong, YU Jiali, ZHAO Di. Loyal or rebel? Employee bootleg innovation in Chinese context [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(6): 975-989. |
[6] | WANG Xieshun, SU Yanjie. From motor imitation to social cognition: The role of self-other control [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(4): 636-645. |
[7] | WANG Yilu, XIE Xiaofei. Prediction errors in helping and help-seeking: Causes and coping [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(1): 117-127. |
[8] | LI Wenqi, ZHANG Mengyuan, KOU Yu. Political orientation and its changing mechanism [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(3): 549-559. |
[9] | YANG Shenlong, GUO Yongyu, YU Feng, RAO Tingting, ZHAO Liang, XU Liying. Three explanatory perspectives on the root of system justification [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(12): 2238-2248. |
[10] | JIN Jian; LI Ye; CHEN Dongming; GUO Kaijiao. Effects and mechanisms of power and status on self-interested behavior [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2017, 25(5): 878-886. |
[11] | ZHENG Yi; SHANG Junchen; LI Boran; LIANG Yaowen; HE Jiamei; YOU Yilin; JIANG Zhongqing. The factors affecting attractiveness of human voice [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2017, 25(2): 237-246. |
[12] | YUE Tong, HUANG Xiting, LIU Guangyuan. Sex-related differences in the effects of Oxytocin on human social cognition [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2017, 25(12): 2157-2167. |
[13] | Fan Wei; Zhong Yiping; Fu Xiaolan. The influence of self-control on deception [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2016, 24(7): 997-1008. |
[14] | PAN Wei; CHEN Wei; WANG Yin; SHAN Chun-lei. The myth of broken mirror theory of autism: Origins, problems and prospects [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2016, 24(6): 958-973. |
[15] | PU Jiajia; Todd JACKSON. Biological, psychological, and social factors associated with anorexia nervosa [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2016, 24(12): 1873-1881. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||