Advances in Psychological Science ›› 2019, Vol. 27 ›› Issue (12): 1988-1995.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2019.01988
• Research Method • Previous Articles Next Articles
YIN Jixing1, HU Chuanpeng2,3()
Received:
2019-01-31
Online:
2019-12-15
Published:
2019-10-21
Contact:
HU Chuanpeng
E-mail:hcp4715@hotmail.com
CLC Number:
YIN Jixing, HU Chuanpeng. Neuroscience bias: Reproducibility and exploration of psychological mechanisms[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(12): 1988-1995.
原创性研究 | 支持与否 | 重复研究 | 支持与否 |
---|---|---|---|
McCabe & Castel, 2008, Cognition | √ | Rhodes et al., 2014, J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn | √ |
Gurley & Marcus, 2008, Behav Sci Law | √ | Fernandez-Duque et al., 2015, J Cogn Neurosci | √ |
Weisberg et al., 2008, J Cogn Neurosci | √ | Weisberg et al., 2015, Judgm Decis Mak | √ |
McCabe et al., 2011, Behav Sci Law | √ | Schweitzer & Saks, 2011, Behav Sci Law | × |
Keehner et al., 2011, Psychon Bull Rev | √ | Gruber & Dickerson, 2012, Public Underst Sci | × |
Greene & Chill, 2012, Behav Sci Law | √ | Michael et al., 2013, Psychon Bull Rev | × |
Ikeda et al, 2013, Psychon Bull Rev | √ | Schweitzer et al., 2013, Cognition | × |
Munro & Munro, 2014, Basic Appl Soc Psych | √ | West et al., 2014, Basic Appl Soc Psych | × |
Scurich & Shniderman, 2014, PloS one | √ | Appelbaum et al., 2015, Psychol Public Policy Law | × |
Plunkett et al., 2014, Cogsci | √ | Marshall et al, 2017, J Forens Psychiatry Psychol | × |
Shariff & Greene et al, 2014, Psychol Sci | √ | Im, Cho et al, 2018, PloS one | × |
Sapolsky, 2015, Unpublished doctorial dissertation | √ | ||
Diekmann et al., 2015, Int J Sel Assess | √ | ||
Hopkins et al., 2016, Cognition | √ | ||
Minahan & Siedlecki, 2016, Pers Individ Dif | √ | ||
Im et al., 2017, Br J Educ Psychol | √ | ||
Macdonald et al., 2017, Front Psychol | √ | ||
Blakey, 2017, Front Psychol | √ | ||
Schweitzer et al, 2011, Psychol Public Policy Law | × | ||
Baker et al, 2013, PLoS One | × | ||
Hook & Farah, 2013, J Cogn Neurosci | × | ||
Saks et al, 2014, J Empir Leg Stud | × |
原创性研究 | 支持与否 | 重复研究 | 支持与否 |
---|---|---|---|
McCabe & Castel, 2008, Cognition | √ | Rhodes et al., 2014, J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn | √ |
Gurley & Marcus, 2008, Behav Sci Law | √ | Fernandez-Duque et al., 2015, J Cogn Neurosci | √ |
Weisberg et al., 2008, J Cogn Neurosci | √ | Weisberg et al., 2015, Judgm Decis Mak | √ |
McCabe et al., 2011, Behav Sci Law | √ | Schweitzer & Saks, 2011, Behav Sci Law | × |
Keehner et al., 2011, Psychon Bull Rev | √ | Gruber & Dickerson, 2012, Public Underst Sci | × |
Greene & Chill, 2012, Behav Sci Law | √ | Michael et al., 2013, Psychon Bull Rev | × |
Ikeda et al, 2013, Psychon Bull Rev | √ | Schweitzer et al., 2013, Cognition | × |
Munro & Munro, 2014, Basic Appl Soc Psych | √ | West et al., 2014, Basic Appl Soc Psych | × |
Scurich & Shniderman, 2014, PloS one | √ | Appelbaum et al., 2015, Psychol Public Policy Law | × |
Plunkett et al., 2014, Cogsci | √ | Marshall et al, 2017, J Forens Psychiatry Psychol | × |
Shariff & Greene et al, 2014, Psychol Sci | √ | Im, Cho et al, 2018, PloS one | × |
Sapolsky, 2015, Unpublished doctorial dissertation | √ | ||
Diekmann et al., 2015, Int J Sel Assess | √ | ||
Hopkins et al., 2016, Cognition | √ | ||
Minahan & Siedlecki, 2016, Pers Individ Dif | √ | ||
Im et al., 2017, Br J Educ Psychol | √ | ||
Macdonald et al., 2017, Front Psychol | √ | ||
Blakey, 2017, Front Psychol | √ | ||
Schweitzer et al, 2011, Psychol Public Policy Law | × | ||
Baker et al, 2013, PLoS One | × | ||
Hook & Farah, 2013, J Cogn Neurosci | × | ||
Saks et al, 2014, J Empir Leg Stud | × |
[1] | 胡传鹏, 邓晓红, 周治金, 邓小刚 . ( 2011). 神经法学: 年轻的认知神经科学与古老的法学联姻. 科学通报, 56( 36), 3041-3053. |
[2] | 胡传鹏, 王非, 过继成思, 宋梦迪, 隋洁, 彭凯平 . ( 2016). 心理学研究中的可重复性问题:从危机到契机. 心理科学进展, 24( 9), 1504-1518 |
[3] | 刘媛媛, 丁一, 彭凯平, 胡传鹏 . ( 2019). 多项式加工树模型在社会心理学中的应用. 心理科学, 42( 2), 422-429. |
[4] | Alimardani, A., & Chin, J. M . ( 2019). Neurolaw in Australia: The Use of Neuroscience in Australian Criminal Proceedings. Retrieved January 31, 2019, from . |
[5] | Anderson, P.W . ( 1972). More is different. Science, 177( 4047), 393-396. |
[6] | Appelbaum, P. S., Scurich, N., & Raad, R . ( 2015). Effects of behavioral genetic evidence on perceptions of criminal responsibility and appropriate punishment. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 21( 2), 134-144. |
[7] | Aronson, J. D . ( 2010). The law's use of brain evidence. Annual Review of Law and Social Science, 6, 93-108. |
[8] | Baker, D. A., Schweitzer, N., Risko, E. F., & Ware, J. M . ( 2013). Visual attention and the neuroimage bias. PLOS one, 8( 9), e74449. |
[9] | Blakey, R. ( 2017). Does watching a play about the teenage brain affect attitudes toward young offenders? Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 964. |
[10] | Chin, J. M . ( 2014). Psychological science's replicability crisis and what it means for science in the courtroom. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 20( 3), 225-238. |
[11] | Chin, J., Growns, B., & Mellor, D. T. ( 2019. Improving expert evidence: The role of open science and transparency. Retrieved January 31, 2019, from |
[12] | Dar-Nimrod, I., & Heine, S. J . ( 2011). Genetic essentialism: On the deceptive determinism of DNA. Psychological Bulletin, 137( 5), 800-818. |
[13] | Diekmann, J., König, C. J., & Alles, J . ( 2015). The role of neuroscience information in choosing a personality test: Not as seductive as expected. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 23( 2), 99-108. |
[14] | Fernandez-Duque, D., Evans, J., Christian, C., & Hodges, S. D . ( 2015). Superfluous neuroscience information makes explanations of psychological phenomena more appealing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27( 5), 926-944. |
[15] | Funk, C., & Rainie, L. ( 2015). Public and scientists’ views on science and society. Retrieved January 31, 2019, from . |
[16] | Greene, E., & Cahill, B.S . ( 2012). Effects of neuroimaging evidence on mock juror decision making. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 30( 3), 280-296. |
[17] | Gruber, D., & Dickerson, J. A . ( 2012). Persuasive images in popular science: Testing judgments of scientific reasoning and credibility. Public Understanding of Science, 21( 8), 938-948. |
[18] | Gurley, J. R., & Marcus, D. K . ( 2008). The effects of neuroimaging and brain injury on insanity defenses. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 26( 1), 85-97. |
[19] | Hardiman, M., Rinne, L., Gregory, E., & Yarmolinskaya, J . ( 2012). Neuroethics, neuroeducation, and classroom teaching: Where the brain sciences meet pedagogy. Neuroethics, 5( 2), 135-143. |
[20] | Hook, C. J., & Farah, M. J . ( 2013). Look again: Effects of brain images and mind-brain dualism on lay evaluations of research. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25( 9), 1397-1405. |
[21] | Hopkins, E. J., Weisberg, D. S., & Taylor, J. C . ( 2016). The seductive allure is a reductive allure: People prefer scientific explanations that contain logically irrelevant reductive information. Cognition, 155, 67-76. |
[22] | Horikawa, T., Tamaki, M., Miyawaki, Y., & Kamitani, Y . ( 2013). Neural Decoding of Visual Imagery During Sleep. Science, 340( 6132), 639-642. |
[23] | Hu, C.-P., Jiang, X., Jeffrey, R., & Zuo, X.-N . ( 2018). Open science as a better gatekeeper for science and society: A perspective from Neurolaw. Science Bulletin, 63( 23), 1529-1531. |
[24] | Hütter, M., & Klauer, K. C . ( 2016). Applying processing trees in social psychology. European Review of Social Psychology, 27( 1), 116-159. |
[25] | Ikeda, K., Kitagami, S., Takahashi, T., Hattori, Y., & Ito, Y . ( 2013). Neuroscientific information bias in metacomprehension: The effect of brain images on metacomprehension judgment of neuroscience research. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20( 6), 1357-1363. |
[26] | Im, S.-h., Cho, J.-Y., Dubinsky, J. M., & Varma, S . ( 2018). Taking an educational psychology course improves neuroscience literacy but does not reduce belief in neuromyths. PLOS one, 13( 2), e0192163. |
[27] | Im, S. h., Varma, K., & Varma, S . ( 2017). Extending the seductive allure of neuroscience explanations effect to popular articles about educational topics. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 87( 4), 518-534. |
[28] | Jacoby, L. L . ( 1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30( 5), 513-541 |
[29] | Janda, L. H., England, K., Lovejoy, D., & Drury, K . ( 1998). Attitudes toward psychology relative to other disciplines. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 29( 2), 140-143. |
[30] | Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow . New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. |
[31] | Keehner, M., Mayberry, L., & Fischer, M. H . ( 2011). Different clues from different views: The role of image format in public perceptions of neuroimaging results. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18( 2), 422-428. |
[32] | Keller, J. (2005). In genes we trust: The biological component of psychological essentialism and its relationship to mechanisms of motivated social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88( 4), 686-702. |
[33] | Kim, N. S., & Keil, F. C . ( 2003). From symptoms to causes: Diversity effects in diagnostic reasoning. Memory & Cognition, 31( 1), 155-165. |
[34] | Krakauer, J. W., Ghazanfar, A. A., Gomez-Marin, A., MacIver, M. A., & Poeppel, D ., ( 2017). Neuroscience needs behavior: Correcting a reductionist bias. Neuron, 93( 3), 480-490. |
[35] | Lilienfeld, S. O . ( 2012). Public skepticism of psychology: Why many people perceive the study of human behavior as unscientific. American Psychologist, 67( 2), 111-129. |
[36] | Lowenberg, K. (2010. fMRI lie detection fails its first hearing on reliability. Retrieved January 31, 2019, from |
[37] | Marshall, J., Lilienfeld, S. O., Mayberg, H., & Clark, S. E . ( 2017). The role of neurological and psychological explanations in legal judgments of psychopathic wrongdoers. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 28( 3), 412-436. |
[38] | McCabe, D. P., & Castel, A. D . ( 2008). Seeing is believing: The effect of brain images on judgments of scientific reasoning. Cognition, 107( 1), 343-352. |
[39] | McCabe, D. P., Castel, A. D., & Rhodes, M. G . ( 2011). The influence of fMRI lie detection evidence on juror decision- making. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 29( 4), 566-577. |
[40] | Medin, D.L . ( 1989). Concepts and conceptual structure. American Psychologist, 44( 12), 1469-1481. |
[41] | Michael, R. B., Newman, E. J., Vuorre, M., Cumming, G., & Garry, M . ( 2013). On the (non) persuasive power of a brain image. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20( 4), 720-725. |
[42] | Minahan, J., & Siedlecki, K.L . ( 2016). Individual differences in Need for Cognition influence the evaluation of circular scientific explanations. Personality and Individual Differences, 99, 113-117. |
[43] | Miton, H., & Mercier, H., (2016). Cognitive obstacles to pro-vaccination beliefs. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19( 11), 633-636. |
[44] | Monterosso, J., Royzman, E. B., & Schwartz, B . ( 2005). Explaining away responsibility: Effects of scientific explanation on perceived culpability. Ethics & Behavior, 15( 2), 139-158. |
[45] | Munro, G.D., & Munro, C. A . ( 2014). “Soft” Versus “Hard” Psychological Science: Biased evaluations of scientific evidence that threatens or supports a strongly held political identity. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 36( 6), 533-543. |
[46] | Perrachione, T. K., & Perrachione, J. R . ( 2008). Brains and brands: Developing mutually informative research in neuroscience and marketing. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 7( 4-5), 303-318. |
[47] | Plunkett, D., Lombrozo, T., & Buchak, L . ( 2014). Because the brain agrees: The impact of neuroscientific explanations for belief. Cognitive Science, 36(36), 1180-1185. |
[48] | Redmond, E. C., & Griffith, C. J . ( 2004). Consumer perceptions of food safety risk, control and responsibility. Appetite, 43( 3), 309-313. |
[49] | Rhodes, R. E., Rodriguez, F., & Shah, P . ( 2014). Explaining the alluring influence of neuroscience information on scientific reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40( 5), 1432-1440. |
[50] | Roets, A., & van H., A . ( 2011). The role of need for closure in essentialist entitativity beliefs and prejudice: An epistemic needs approach to racial categorization. British Journal of Social Psychology, 50( 1), 52-73. |
[51] | Saks, M. J., Schweitzer, N., Aharoni, E., & Kiehl, K. A . ( 2014). The impact of neuroimages in the sentencing phase of capital trials. Journal of Empirical Legal Studies, 11( 1), 105-131. |
[52] | Sapolsky, Z. G . ( 2015). Neuroscience information's effect on causal explanations of psychological disorders and treatment recommendations (Unpublished doctorial dissertation). Long Island University, New York, U.S. |
[53] | Schauer, F. (2010). Neuroscience, lie-detection, and the law: Contrary to the prevailing view, the suitability of brain- based lie-detection for courtroom or forensic use should be determined according to legal and not scientific standards. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14( 3), 101-103. |
[54] | Schweitzer, N., Baker, D. A., & Risko, E. F . ( 2013). Fooled by the brain: Re-examining the influence of neuroimages. Cognition, 129( 3), 501-511. |
[55] | Schweitzer, N. J., & Saks, M. J . ( 2011). Neuroimage evidence and the insanity defense. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 29( 4), 592-607. |
[56] | Schweitzer, N. J., Saks, M. J., Murphy, E. R., Roskies, A. L., Sinnott-Armstrong, W., & Gaudet, L. M . ( 2011). Neuroimages as evidence in a mens rea defense: No impact. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 17( 3), 357-393. |
[57] | Scurich, N., & Shniderman, A. (2014). The selective allure of neuroscientific explanations. PLOS one, 9( 9), e107529. |
[58] | Shariff, A. F., Greene, J. D., Karremans, J. C., Luguri, J. B., Clark, C. J., Schooler, J. W., .. Vohs, K. D . ( 2014). Free will and punishment: A mechanistic view of human nature reduces retribution. Psychological Science, 25( 8), 1563-1570. |
[59] | Spranger, T. M . (Ed.) ( 2011). International Neurolaw: A Comparative Analysis. Berlin, Germany: Springer Berlin Heidelberg. |
[60] | Szucs, D., & Ioannidis, J. P . ( 2017). Empirical assessment of published effect sizes and power in the recent cognitive neuroscience and psychology literature. Plos Biology, 15( 3), e2000797. |
[61] | Turnwald, B. P., Goyer, J. P., Boles, D. Z., Silder, A., Delp, S. L., & Crum, A. J . ( 2018). Learning one’s genetic risk changes physiology independent of actual genetic risk. Nature Human Behaviour. 3, 48-56. |
[62] | Webster, M. (1973). Psychological reductionism, methodological individualism, and large-scale problems. American Sociological Review, 38( 2), 258-273. |
[63] | Weisberg, D. S., Keil, F. C., Goodstein, J., Rawson, E., & Gray, J. R . ( 2008). The seductive allure of neuroscience explanations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20( 3), 470-477. |
[64] | Weisberg, D. S., Taylor, J. C., & Hopkins, E. J . ( 2015). Deconstructing the seductive allure of neuroscience explanations. Judgment and Decision Making, 10 (5), 429-441. |
[65] | West, M. L., Lawson, V. Z., & Grose-Fifer, J. (2014). The effect of electrophysiological neuroscientific deception detection evidence on juror judgments in a criminal trial. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 36 (2), 133-144. |
[66] | Yzerbyt, V., Rocher, S. & Schadron, G.. ( 1997). Stereotypes as explanations: A subjective essentialistic view of group perception. In R. Spears, P. J. Oakes, N. Ellemers, & S. A. Haslam (Eds.), The social psychology of stereotyping and group life (pp. 20-50). Malden, Massachusetts,, USA: Blackwell Publishing. |
[1] | ZHANG Qi; YIN Tianzi; RAN Guangming. Psychological and Neural Mechanisms for the Superiority Effect of Dynamic Facial Expressions [J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2015, 23(9): 1514-1522. |
[2] | WANG Fen;HUANG Yu-Xia. Psychological and Neural Mechanisms of Mindfulness [J]. , 2011, 19(11): 1635-1644. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||