心理科学进展 ›› 2024, Vol. 32 ›› Issue (6): 965-980.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2024.00965
收稿日期:
2023-09-26
出版日期:
2024-06-15
发布日期:
2024-04-07
通讯作者:
陈维扬, E-mail: chenwy@swufe.edu.cn
基金资助:
YANG Lei1, CHEN Weiyang2(), ZHU Qiujin1, ZHONG Nian1
Received:
2023-09-26
Online:
2024-06-15
Published:
2024-04-07
摘要:
累积文化演化是文化演化的一个重要子集, 侧重关注文化的逐渐累积, 指的是文化在传递和修改的过程中, 在复杂性、效率性等方面发生了适应性的变化。虽然累积文化演化是一个跨学科概念, 但却有着浓厚的心理学底色。心理因素尤其是认知因素在其概念框架和解释流派的发展过程中占据着重要地位, 对认知的重视推动了累积文化演化的理论发展和解释流派的整合。未来研究可以从理论发展角度进一步明晰累积文化演化的衡量标准, 从解释流派整合角度探明累积文化演化的过程与细节, 以及从心理学角度更深入考察心理因素对累积文化演化的影响。
中图分类号:
杨蕾, 陈维扬, 朱秋锦, 钟年. (2024). 心理学视野下的累积文化演化:理论发展与解释流派整合. 心理科学进展 , 32(6), 965-980.
YANG Lei, CHEN Weiyang, ZHU Qiujin, ZHONG Nian. (2024). Cumulative cultural evolution theory from a psychological perspective: Theoretical development and integration of schools. Advances in Psychological Science, 32(6), 965-980.
“棘轮效应” (Ratcheting effect) | “登山效应” (Mountaineering effect) | |
---|---|---|
视角 | 描述累积文化演化为单向、连续性的改进过程。 | 描述累积文化演化为多样、整体性的改进过程。 |
理论基础 | 基于自然选择理论, 着重于文化信息的保真度的重要性, 强调文化信息的传递和社会学习机制的作用。 | 在“棘轮效应”基础上融入了认知科学观点, 除社会学习外, 也重视个体认知加工对累积文化演化的影响。 |
衡量标准 | 核心标准(Mesoudi & Thornton, (1)现有行为、文化产品的改变或新的行为、文化产品的出现; (2)通过社会学习将上述行为或文化产品进行传递; (3)传递过程中, 文化行为和文化产品得到改善, 体现为文化适应性的提升; (4)随着时间的推移, 前三个步骤不断重复进而产生文化的持续改进。 | 核心标准的拓展(Haidle & Schlaudt, (1)个体(文化)行为的发展与变化; (2)自然或社会的学习环境发生变化, 在改变后的环境中传递新(文化)行为; (3)习得的行为会导致表现的改变, 这种改变为未来的发展提供了可能性和限制性; (4)以上步骤随着时间推移不断重复以产生累积性的发展。 |
理论意义 | 开始关注文化的累积性演化趋势; 奠定了累积文化演化概念的核心框架; 有助于理解社会学习对信息的高保真度传递的作用。 | 提供了更广阔的文化累积性演化趋势视角; 考虑到了文化累积性发展中的复杂性和多样性; 有助于理解个体认知因素对信息加工和传递的影响。 |
表1 “棘轮效应”与“登山效应”概念框架对比
“棘轮效应” (Ratcheting effect) | “登山效应” (Mountaineering effect) | |
---|---|---|
视角 | 描述累积文化演化为单向、连续性的改进过程。 | 描述累积文化演化为多样、整体性的改进过程。 |
理论基础 | 基于自然选择理论, 着重于文化信息的保真度的重要性, 强调文化信息的传递和社会学习机制的作用。 | 在“棘轮效应”基础上融入了认知科学观点, 除社会学习外, 也重视个体认知加工对累积文化演化的影响。 |
衡量标准 | 核心标准(Mesoudi & Thornton, (1)现有行为、文化产品的改变或新的行为、文化产品的出现; (2)通过社会学习将上述行为或文化产品进行传递; (3)传递过程中, 文化行为和文化产品得到改善, 体现为文化适应性的提升; (4)随着时间的推移, 前三个步骤不断重复进而产生文化的持续改进。 | 核心标准的拓展(Haidle & Schlaudt, (1)个体(文化)行为的发展与变化; (2)自然或社会的学习环境发生变化, 在改变后的环境中传递新(文化)行为; (3)习得的行为会导致表现的改变, 这种改变为未来的发展提供了可能性和限制性; (4)以上步骤随着时间推移不断重复以产生累积性的发展。 |
理论意义 | 开始关注文化的累积性演化趋势; 奠定了累积文化演化概念的核心框架; 有助于理解社会学习对信息的高保真度传递的作用。 | 提供了更广阔的文化累积性演化趋势视角; 考虑到了文化累积性发展中的复杂性和多样性; 有助于理解个体认知因素对信息加工和传递的影响。 |
图1 “加州派”和“巴黎派”解释路径图及对应案例 注:图左侧为解释路径, 右侧为案例。案例1对应加州派观点。如图所示, 在文化被创造的初期, 一共有5种文化特征。随着时间发展, 初代带密集点的圆形文化特征被更多人选择和复制, 在所有文化信息中频率增加, 最终该特征呈现出一种稳定的文化发展趋势。案例2对应巴黎派观点。如图所示, 在文化被创造的初期, 也具有5种文化特征, 与案例1不同的是, 带密集点的圆形特征在初期并不存在。这些文化特征随时间在代际间传递, 虽然最初这些文化特征各不相同, 但由于文化吸引因子的引导, 文化特征被逐渐修改(每一列代表一种文化特征的修改过程), 这种修改即有偏转变, 使得文化逐渐朝着带密集点的圆形这一特征收敛, 最终呈现出与案例1一样的稳定的文化发展趋势。资料来源:作者整理
[1] | 陈维扬, 谢天. (2020). 文化演化的认知视角——从个体社会学习出发探究文化动态性. 心理科学进展, 28(12), 2137−2149. |
[2] | 陈维扬, 谢天. (2023). 朝向集体主义和保守主义:病原体对文化演化的影响. 应用心理学, 29(5), 396−407. |
[3] | 解光云. (2016). 古典传承与文艺复兴. 吉林大学社会科学学报, 56(2), 121−128. |
[4] | 刘颖, 肖天久. (2014). 《红楼梦》计量风格学研究. 红楼梦学刊, (4), 260−281. |
[5] | 路甬祥. (2005). 学科交叉与交叉科学的意义. 中国科学院院刊, 20(1), 58−60. |
[6] | 索传军, 肖玥. (2023). 交叉学科的学科特征分析与评价. 中国人民大学学报, 37(6), 132−143. |
[7] | 吴铮. (2020). 生态转换与人机共生: 人类与人工智能存在的关系研究. 人民论坛·学术前沿, (11), 108−111. |
[8] | 辛自强, 刘国芳. (2012). 文化进化的实验与非实验研究方法. 北京师范大学学报:社会科学版, (3), 5−13. |
[9] | 杨蕾, 陈维扬, 白宝玉, 钟年. (2023). 累积文化演化的内在机制:实验证据. 心理科学进展, 31(5), 866−886. |
[10] | 张昊天, 喻丰. (2023). 自然性偏好的概念、表现及成因. 心理科学进展, 31(12), 2393−2405. |
[11] | Acerbi A., Kendal J., & Tehrani J. J. (2017). Cultural complexity and demography: The case of folktales. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(4), 474−480. |
[12] | Acerbi A., & Mesoudi A. (2015). If we are all cultural Darwinians what’s the fuss about? Clarifying recent disagreements in the field of cultural evolution. Biology & Philosophy, 30(4), 481−503. |
[13] | Alem S., Perry C. J., Zhu X., Loukola O. J., Ingraham T., Søvik E., & Chittka L. (2016). Correction: Associative mechanisms allow for social learning and cultural transmission of string pulling in an insect. Plos Biology, 14(12), e1002589. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002564 |
[14] | Baumard N., & Dubourg E. (2022). Why imaginary worlds? The psychological foundations and cultural evolution of fictions with imaginary worlds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 45, e276. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X21000923 |
[15] | Baumard N., Huillery E., Hyafil A., & Safra L. (2022). The cultural evolution of love in literary history. Nature Human Behaviour, 6(4), 506−522. |
[16] | Baumard N., Safra L., Martins M., & Chevallier C. (2024). Cognitive fossils: Using cultural artifacts to reconstruct psychological changes throughout history. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 28(2), 172−186. |
[17] | Bebbington K., MacLeod C., Ellison T. M., & Fay N. (2017). The sky is falling: Evidence of a negativity bias in the social transmission of information. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(1), 92−101. |
[18] | Bedford O., & Yeh K. (2021). Evolution of the conceptualization of filial piety in the global context: From skin to skeleton. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 570547. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.570547 |
[19] |
Berl R. E. W., Samarasinghe A. N., Roberts S. G., Jordan F. M., & Gavin M. C. (2021). Prestige and content biases together shape the cultural transmission of narratives. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 3, e42. https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2021.37
doi: 10.1017/ehs.2021.37 URL pmid: 37588523 |
[20] | Blaine T., & Boyer P. (2018). Origins of sinister rumors: A preference for threat-related material in the supply and demand of information. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(1), 67−75. |
[21] | Bluet A., Osiurak F., Claidière N., & Reynaud E. (2022). Impact of technical reasoning and theory of mind on cumulative technological culture: insights from a model of micro-societies. Humanities and Social Sciences Communications, 9(1), 231. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-022-01251-z |
[22] | Boesch C., & Tomasello M. (1998). Chimpanzee and human cultures. Current Anthropology, 39(5), 591−614. |
[23] | Bothara J., Ingham J., & Dizhur D. (2022). Qualifying the earthquake resilience of vernacular masonry buildings along the Himalayan arc. Journal of Building Engineering, 52, 104339. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jobe.2022.104339 |
[24] | Boyd R., & Richerson P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago press. |
[25] | Boyd R., & Richerson P. J. (1996). Why culture is common, but cultural evolution is rare. Proceedings of the British Academy, 88, 77−93. |
[26] |
Brand C. O., Acerbi A., & Mesoudi A. (2019). Cultural evolution of emotional expression in 50 years of song lyrics. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 1, e11. https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2019.11
doi: 10.1017/ehs.2019.11 URL pmid: 37588398 |
[27] |
Brand C. O., Heap S., Morgan T. J. H., & Mesoudi A. (2020). The emergence and adaptive use of prestige in an online social learning task. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 12095. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-68982-4
doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-68982-4 URL pmid: 32694697 |
[28] | Bredeche N., & Fontbonne N. (2021). Social learning in swarm robotics. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 377(1843), 20200309. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0309 |
[29] | Brinkmann L., Gezerli D., Kleist K. V., Müller T. F., Rahwan I., & Pescetelli N. (2022). Hybrid social learning in human-algorithm cultural transmission. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences, 380(2227), 20200426. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsta.2020.0426 |
[30] | Buskell A. (2017). What are cultural attractors? Biology & Philosophy, 32(3), 377−394. |
[31] | Buskell A. (2022). Cumulative culture and complex cultural traditions. Mind & Language, 37(3), 284−303. |
[32] | Caldwell C. A. (2020). Using experimental research designs to explore the scope of cumulative culture in humans and other animals. Topics in Cognitive Science, 12(2), 673−689. |
[33] | Caldwell C. A., & Eve R. M. (2014). Persistence of contrasting traditions in cultural evolution: Unpredictable payoffs generate slower rates of cultural change. Plos One, 9(6), e99708. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0099708 |
[34] | Caldwell C. A., & Millen A. E. (2008). Studying cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1509), 3529−3539. |
[35] | Caldwell C. A., & Millen A. E. (2010). Conservatism in laboratory microsocieties: Unpredictable payoffs accentuate group-specific traditions. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(2), 123−130. |
[36] | Castro L., & Toro M. A. (2004). The evolution of culture: From primate social learning to human culture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101(27), 10235− 10240. |
[37] | d’Astous, A., & Labrecque, J. (2021). The impact of responsible food packaging perceptions on naturalness and healthiness inferences, and consumer buying intentions. Foods, 10(10), 2366. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods10102366 |
[38] |
Davis S., Rawlings B., Clegg J. M., Ikejimba D., Watson- Jones R. E., Whiten A., & Legare C. H. (2022). Cognitive flexibility supports the development of cumulative cultural learning in children. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 14073. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-18231-7
doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-18231-7 URL pmid: 35982124 |
[39] | Dean L. G., Kendal R. L., Schapiro S. J., Thierry B., & Laland K. N. (2012). Identification of the social and cognitive processes underlying human cumulative culture. Science, 335(6072), 1114−1118. |
[40] | Derex M., Beugin M., Godelle B., & Raymond M. (2013). Experimental evidence for the influence of group size on cultural complexity. Nature, 503(7476), 389−391. |
[41] |
Dubourg E., Thouzeau V., de Dampierre C., Mogoutov A., & Baumard N. (2023). Exploratory preferences explain the human fascination for imaginary worlds in fictional stories. Scientific Reports, 13(1), 8657. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-35151-2
doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-35151-2 URL pmid: 37246187 |
[42] | Eriksson K., & Coultas J. C. (2014). Corpses, maggots, poodles and rats: Emotional selection operating in three phases of cultural transmission of urban legends. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 14(1-2), 1−26. |
[43] | Fay N., De Kleine N., Walker B., & Caldwell C. A. (2019). Increasing population size can inhibit cumulative cultural evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(14), 6726−6731. |
[44] | Fay N., Ellison T. M., Tylén K., Fusaroli R., Walker B., & Garrod S. (2018). Applying the cultural ratchet to a social artefact: The cumulative cultural evolution of a language game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(3), 300−309. |
[45] | Fay N., Walker B., Kashima Y., & Perfors A. (2021). Socially situated transmission: The bias to transmit negative information is moderated by the social context. Cognitive Science, 45(9), e13033. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.13033 |
[46] | Fracchia J., & Lewontin R. C. (1999). Does culture evolve? History and Theory, 38(4), 52−78. |
[47] | Garland E. C., Garrigue C., & Noad M. J. (2022). When does cultural evolution become cumulative culture? A case study of humpback whale song. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 377(1843), 20200313. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0313 |
[48] | Gruber T., Chimento M., Aplin L. M., & Biro D. (2021). Efficiency fosters cumulative culture across species. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 377(1843), 20200308. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0308 |
[49] | Guillou L., Safra L., & Baumard N. (2023). Using portraits to quantify the changes of generalized social trust in European history: A replication study. Plos One, 18(9), e289741. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289741 |
[50] | Haidle M. N. (2019). The origin of cumulative culture—not a single-trait event, but multifactorial processes. In K. A. Overmann & F. L. Coolidge (Eds.), Squeezing minds from stones (pp. 128−148). Oxford University Press. |
[51] | Haidle M. N., & Schlaudt O. (2020). Where does cumulative culture begin? A plea for a sociologically informed perspective. Biological Theory, 15(3), 161−174. |
[52] | Haidle M. N., & Schlaudt O. (2021). Taking the historical- social dimension seriously: A reply to Bandini et al. Biological Theory, 16(2), 83−89. |
[53] | Harris J. A., Boyd R., & Wood B. M. (2021). The role of causal knowledge in the evolution of traditional technology. Current Biology, 31(8), 1798−1803. |
[54] | Henrich J. (2015). The Secret of Our Success: How Culture Is Driving Human Evolution, Domesticating Our Species, and Making Us Smarter. Princeton University Press. |
[55] | Henrich J., & McElreath R. (2007). Dual-inheritance theory:The evolution of human cultural capacities and cultural evolution. In L. Barrett & R. Dunbar (Eds.), Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 555−570). Oxford University Press. |
[56] | Heyes C. M. (1994). Social learning in animals: Categories and mechanisms. Biological Reviews, 69(2), 207−231. |
[57] | Hong Z., & Henrich J. (2021). The cultural evolution of epistemic practices. Human Nature, 32(3), 622−651. |
[58] | Horner C. G., Galletta D., Crawford J., & Shirsat A. (2021). Emotions: The unexplored fuel of fake news on social media. Journal of Management Information Systems, 38(4), 1039−1066. |
[59] | Jagiello R., Heyes C., & Whitehouse H. (2022a). Tradition and invention: The bifocal stance theory of cultural evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 45, e249. https://doi.org/ 10.1017/S0140525X22000383 |
[60] | Jagiello R., Heyes C., & Whitehouse H. (2022b). Bifocal stance theory: An effort to broaden, extend, and clarify. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 45, e275. https://doi.org/ 10.1017/S0140525X2200173X |
[61] | Jones B. K., & Kerr R. (2023). “If Winter Comes, Can Spring Be Far Behind?”: Climate and preference for redemptive narratives. Ecopsychology, 15(4), 382−390. |
[62] | Kidd C., & Birhane A. (2023). How AI can distort human beliefs. Science, 380(6651), 1222−1223. |
[63] | Kolodny O., Creanza N., & Feldman M. W. (2015). Evolution in leaps: The punctuated accumulation and loss of cultural innovations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(49), E6762−E6769. |
[64] | Kuckling J. (2023). Recent trends in robot learning and evolution for swarm robotics. Frontiers in Robotics and AI, 10, 1134841. https://doi.org/10.3389/frobt.2023.1134841 |
[65] | Lee S. H., & Yamamoto S. (2023). The evolution of prestige: Perspectives and hypotheses from comparative studies. New Ideas in Psychology, 68, 100987. https://doi.org/10. 1016/j.newideapsych.2022.100987 |
[66] | Li H., & Cao Y. (2020). For the love of nature: People who prefer natural versus synthetic drugs are higher in nature connectedness. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 71, 101496. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101496 |
[67] | Lombard M. (2016). Mountaineering or ratcheting? Stone Age hunting weapons as proxy for the evolution of human technological, behavioral and cognitive flexibility. In M. Haidle, N. Conard, & M. Bolus (Eds.), The nature of culture (pp.135−146). Springer. |
[68] | Lucas A. J., Kings M., Whittle D., Davey E., Happé F., Caldwell C. A., & Thornton A. (2020). The value of teaching increases with tool complexity in cumulative cultural evolution. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 287(1939), 20201885. https://doi.org/ 10.1098/rspb.2020.1885 |
[69] | Ma S., Tsay C., & Chen E. E. (2023). Preference for talented naturals over hard workers emerges in childhood and shapes behavior. Child Development, 94(3), 674−690. |
[70] | Marshall-Pescini S., & Whiten A. (2008). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and the question of cumulative culture: An experimental approach. Animal Cognition, 11(3), 449− 456. |
[71] | Mesoudi A. (2017). Pursuing Darwin’s curious parallel: Prospects for a science of cultural evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(30), 7853−7860. |
[72] | Mesoudi A. (2021). Cultural selection and biased transformation: Two dynamics of cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 376(1828), 20200053. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0053 |
[73] | Mesoudi A., & Thornton A. (2018). What is cumulative cultural evolution? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 285(1880), 20180712. https://doi.org/ 10.1098/rspb.2018.0712 |
[74] | Mesoudi A., & Whiten A. (2008). The multiple roles of cultural transmission experiments in understanding human cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1509), 3489− 3501. |
[75] | Miton H., & Charbonneau M. (2018). Cumulative culture in the laboratory: Methodological and theoretical challenges. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 285(1879), 20180677. https://doi.org/doi:10.1098/rspb.2018. 0677 |
[76] | Miton H., Claidière N., & Mercier H. (2015). Universal cognitive mechanisms explain the cultural success of bloodletting. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(4), 303− 312. |
[77] | Morgan T. J. H., Uomini N. T., Rendell L. E., Chouinard- Thuly L., Street S. E., Lewis H. M., ... Laland K. N. (2015). Experimental evidence for the co-evolution of hominin tool-making teaching and language. Nature Communications, 6(1), 6029. https://doi.org/10.1038/nco mms7029 |
[78] | Morin O. (2013). How portraits turned their eyes upon us: Visual preferences and demographic change in cultural evolution. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(3), 222−229. |
[79] | Osiurak F., Claidière N., Bluet A., Brogniart J., Lasserre S., Bonhoure T., ... Reynaud E. (2022). Technical reasoning bolsters cumulative technological culture through convergent transformations. Science Advances, 8(9), l7446. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.abl7446 |
[80] | Osiurak F., Claidière N., & Federico G. (2023). Bringing cumulative technological culture beyond copying versus reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 27(1), 30−42. |
[81] | Osiurak F., Cretel C., Duhau-Marmon N., Fournier I., Marignier L., De Oliveira E., ... Reynaud E. (2020). The pedagogue, the engineer, and the friend. Human Nature, 31(4), 462−482. |
[82] | Osiurak F., De Oliveira E., Navarro J., & Reynaud E. (2020). The castaway island: Distinct roles of theory of mind and technical reasoning in cumulative technological culture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(1), 58−66. |
[83] | Osiurak F., Lasserre S., Arbanti J., Brogniart J., Bluet A., Navarro J., & Reynaud E. (2021). Technical reasoning is important for cumulative technological culture. Nature Human Behaviour, 5(12), 1643−1651. |
[84] | Osiurak F., & Reynaud E. (2020). The elephant in the room: What matters cognitively in cumulative technological culture. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 43, e156. https:// doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X19003236 |
[85] | Planck M. (1975). Vorträge und Erinnerungen. Wissenschaftliche Buchgesellschaft. |
[86] | Rawlings B. S., Legare C. H., Brosnan S. F., & Vale G. L. (2021). Leveling the playing field in studying cumulative cultural evolution: Conceptual and methodological advances in nonhuman animal research. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition, 47(3), 252−273. |
[87] |
Reindl E., Apperly I. A., Beck S. R., & Tennie C. (2017). Young children copy cumulative technological design in the absence of action information. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1788. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-01715-2
doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-01715-2 URL pmid: 28496154 |
[88] | Reindl E., Gwilliams A. L., Dean L. G., Kendal R. L., & Tennie C. (2020). Skills and motivations underlying children’s cumulative cultural learning: Case not closed. Palgrave Communications, 6(1), 1−9. |
[89] | Rendell L., Fogarty L., Hoppitt W. J. E., Morgan T. J. H., Webster M. M., & Laland K. N. (2011). Cognitive culture: Theoretical and empirical insights into social learning strategies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(2), 68−76. |
[90] | Ross G. J. (2020). Tracking the evolution of literary style via dirichlet-multinomial change point regression. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series A: Statistics in Society, 183(1), 149−167. |
[91] | Sasaki T., & Biro D. (2017). Cumulative culture can emerge from collective intelligence in animal groups. Nature Communications, 8(1), 15049. https://doi.org/10.1038/ ncomms15049 |
[92] | Scott-Phillips T., Blancke S., & Heintz C. (2018). Four misunderstandings about cultural attraction. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 27(4), 162−173. |
[93] | Shin M., Kim J., van Opheusden B., & Griffiths T. L. (2023). Superhuman artificial intelligence can improve human decision-making by increasing novelty. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 120(12), e2080127176. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2214840120 |
[94] | Singh M. (2022). Subjective selection and the evolution of complex culture. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 31(6), 266−280. |
[95] | Sperber D. (1996). Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Blackwell. |
[96] | Sterelny K. (2017). Cultural evolution in California and Paris. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, 62, 42−50. |
[97] | Tan R., & Fay N. (2011). Cultural transmission in the laboratory: Agent interaction improves the intergenerational transfer of information. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(6), 399−406. |
[98] |
Tinits P., & Sobchuk O. (2020). Open-ended cumulative cultural evolution of Hollywood film crews. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 2, e26. https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2020.21
doi: 10.1017/ehs.2020.21 URL pmid: 37588342 |
[99] | Tomasello M. (1990). Cultural transmission in the tool use and communicatory signaling of chimpanzees? In S. T. Parker & K. R. Gibson (Eds.), Language and intelligence in monkeys and apes: Comparative developmental perspectives (pp.274−311). Cambridge University Press. |
[100] | Tomasello M. (2000). Two hypotheses about primate cognition. In C. Heyes & L. Huber (Eds.), The evolution of cognition (pp.165−183). The MIT Press. |
[101] | Tomasello M., Kruger A. C., & Ratner H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(3), 495−511. |
[102] | Turchin P., & Gavrilets S. (2021). Tempo and mode in cultural macroevolution. Evolutionary Psychology, 19(4), 172623640. https://doi.org/10.1177/14747049211066600 |
[103] | Vale G. L., Coughlin C., & Brosnan S. F. (2022). The importance of thinking about the future in culture and cumulative cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 377(1866), 20210349. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2021.0349 |
[104] | Van de Vliert E., & Murray D. R. (2018). Climate and creativity: Cold and heat trigger invention and innovation in richer populations. Creativity Research Journal, 30(1), 17−28. |
[105] | van Leeuwen E. J. C., & Hoppitt W. (2023). Biased cultural transmission of a social custom in chimpanzees. Science Advances, 9(7), e5675. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.ade5675 |
[106] | Varnum M. E. W., Krems J. A., Morris C., Wormley A., & Grossmann I. (2021). Why are song lyrics becoming simpler? A time series analysis of lyrical complexity in six decades of American popular music. Plos One, 16(1), e244576. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0244576 |
[107] | Verpooten J., & Dewitte S. (2017). The conundrum of modern art: Prestige-driven coevolutionary aesthetics trumps evolutionary aesthetics among art experts. Human Nature, 28(1), 16−38. |
[108] | Vidiella B., Carrignon S., Bentley R. A., O’Brien M. J., & Valverde S. (2022). A cultural evolutionary theory that explains both gradual and punctuated change. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 19(196), 20220570. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2022.0570 |
[109] | Wasielewski H. (2014). Imitation is necessary for cumulative cultural evolution in an unfamiliar, opaque task. Human Nature, 25(1), 161−179. |
[110] | Whiten A., Biro D., Bredeche N., Garland E. C., & Kirby S. (2022). The emergence of collective knowledge and cumulative culture in animals, humans and machines. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 377(1843), 20200306. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0306 |
[111] | Wilks C. E. H., Rafetseder E., Renner E., Atkinson M., & Caldwell C. A. (2021). Cognitive prerequisites for cumulative culture are context-dependent: Children’s potential for ratcheting depends on cue longevity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 204, 105031. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.105031 |
[112] | Williams H., & Lachlan R. F. (2021). Evidence for cumulative cultural evolution in bird song. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 377(1843), 20200322. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0322 |
[113] | Winfield A. F. T., & Blackmore S. (2021). Experiments in artificial culture: From noisy imitation to storytelling robots. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 377(1843), 20200323. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0323 |
[114] | Youngblood M. (2019). Conformity bias in the cultural transmission of music sampling traditions. Royal Society Open Science, 6(9), 191149. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.191149 |
[1] | 杨蕾, 陈维扬, 白宝玉, 钟年. 累积文化演化的内在机制:实验证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(5): 866-886. |
[2] | 陈维扬, 谢天. 文化演化的认知视角——从个体社会学习出发探究文化动态性[J]. 心理科学进展, 2020, 28(12): 2137-2149. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||