心理科学进展 ›› 2019, Vol. 27 ›› Issue (8): 1331-1343.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2019.01331
• 研究构想 • 下一篇
收稿日期:
2019-02-26
出版日期:
2019-07-26
发布日期:
2019-06-25
通讯作者:
苏彦捷
E-mail:yjsu@pku.edu.cn
基金资助:
SU Yanjie(), XIE Dongjie, WANG Xiaonan
Received:
2019-02-26
Online:
2019-07-26
Published:
2019-06-25
Contact:
SU Yanjie
E-mail:yjsu@pku.edu.cn
摘要:
第三方惩罚是一种特殊的利他行为, 在维护社会规范和人类合作中发挥着重要作用。现有研究对遵守社会规范行为及其认知机制进行了大量探讨, 而第三方惩罚这一维护社会规范行为的认知加工过程, 尤其是认知控制在其中发挥的具体作用, 是目前研究的热点之一。通过(1)借助不同技术方法来探究认知控制在第三方惩罚中的具体作用; (2)从发展的视角, 在认知控制发展的关键阶段来考察其作用随年龄的发展变化, 用以解释第三方惩罚这一利他行为的发展轨迹, 完善对第三方惩罚的认知基础的理解, 并构建第三方惩罚决策的心理模型。
中图分类号:
苏彦捷, 谢东杰, 王笑楠. (2019). 认知控制在第三方惩罚中的作用. 心理科学进展 , 27(8), 1331-1343.
SU Yanjie, XIE Dongjie, WANG Xiaonan. (2019). The role of cognitive control in third-party punishment. Advances in Psychological Science, 27(8), 1331-1343.
[1] | 陈思静 . (2011). 社会规范激活:第三方惩罚的心理机制及其对合作行为的影响 (博士学位论文). 浙江大学, 杭州. |
[2] | 陈思静, 马剑虹 . (2011). 第三方惩罚与社会规范激活——社会责任感与情绪的作用. 心理科学, 34( 3), 670-675. |
[3] | 丁毅, 纪婷婷, 陈旭 . (2012). 利他惩罚的发生机制及其神经基础. 心理发展与教育, 28( 6), 658-664. |
[4] | 黄翯青, 苏彦捷 . (2012). 共情的毕生发展:一个双过程的视角. 心理发展与教育, 28( 4), 434-441. |
[5] | 霍夫曼 , K.(2011). 行动中的心理学 (第八版; 苏彦捷等译). 北京: 中国人民大学出版社. |
[6] | 李政汉, 杨国春, 南威治, 李琦, 刘勋 . (2018). 冲突解决过程中认知控制的注意调节机制. 心理科学进展, 26( 6), 966-974. |
[7] | 人民日报评论部. (2018-11-14). 培育深入人心的规则意识(人民观点)——如何提升我们的社会文明 人民日报, pp. 5. |
[8] | 谢东杰, 苏彦捷 . (2019). 第三方惩罚的演化与认知机制. 心理科学, 42( 1), 216-222. |
[9] | 颜志强, 苏金龙, 苏彦捷 . (2018). 共情与同情:词源、概念和测量. 心理与行为研究, 16( 4), 433-440. |
[10] | 殷西乐, 李建标, 陈思宇, 刘晓丽, 郝洁 . (2019). 第三方惩罚的神经机制:来自经颅直流电刺激的证据. 心理学报, 51( 5), 571-583. |
[11] | Artavia-Mora L., Bedi A. S., & Rieger M . (2017). Intuitive help and punishment in the field. European Economic Review, 92, 133-145. |
[12] |
Baumard N., Mascaro O., & Chevallier C . (2012). Preschoolers are able to take merit into account when distributing goods. Developmental Psychology, 48( 2), 492-498.
doi: 10.1037/a0026598 |
[13] | Bellucci G., Chernyak S., Hoffman M., Deshpande G., Dal Monte O., Knutson K. M., .. Krueger F . (2017). Effective connectivity of brain regions underlying third-party punishment: Functional MRI and granger causality evidence. Social Neuroscience, 12( 2), 124-134. |
[14] | Bendor J., & Swistak P (2001). The evolution of norms. American Journal of Sociology, 106( 6), 1493-1545. |
[15] | Best J.R., &Miller P.H . (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81( 6), 1641-1660. |
[16] |
Blake P.., & McAuliffe K , (2011). “I had so much it didn't seem fair”: Eight-year-olds reject two forms of inequity. Cognition, 120( 2), 215-224.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.04.006 |
[17] | Blake P. R., Piovesan M., Montinari N., Warneken F., & Gino F . (2015). Prosocial norms in the classroom: The role of self-regulation in following norms of giving. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 115, 18-29. |
[18] | Bicchieri C., .(2006).The Grammar of society:he nature and dynamics of social norms. New York: TCambridge University Press. |
[19] | Buckholtz J. W., Martin J. W., Treadway M. T., Jan K., Zald D. H., Jones O., & Marois R . (2015). From blame to punishment: Disrupting prefrontal cortex activity reveals norm enforcement mechanisms. Neuron, 87( 6), 1369-1380. |
[20] | Camerer C. F. (2003).Behavioral game theory:Experiments in strategic interaction. New York:Princeton University Press. |
[21] | Carlsmith K. M., Darley J. M., & Robinson P. H . (2002). Why do we punish? Deterrence and just deserts as motives for punishment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83( 2), 284-299. |
[22] |
Casey B. J., Getz S., & Galvan A . (2008). The adolescent brain. Developmental Review, 28( 1), 62-77.
doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.003 |
[23] | Chaiken S., &Trope Y. ,(1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology. New York: Guilford Press. |
[24] | Ciaramidaro A., Toppi J., Casper C., Freitag C. M., Siniatchkin M., & Astolfi L . (2018). Multiple-brain connectivity during third party punishment: An EEG hyperscanning study. Scientific Reports, 8, 6822. |
[25] | Corsi P. . (1972). Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). McGill University, Montreal. |
[26] | Crone E.A., &Steinbeis N , (2017). Neural perspectives on cognitive control development during childhood and adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21( 3), 205-215. |
[27] | Darley J.M., & Pittman T.S . (2003). The psychology of compensatory and retributive justice. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7( 4), 324-336. |
[28] | Davis M.H . (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85. |
[29] | de Waal F , (2008). Putting the altruism back into altruism: The evolution of empathy. Annual Review of Psychology, 59( 1), 279-300. |
[30] | Decety J., Bartal I. B., Uzefovsky F., & Knafo-Noam A . (2016). Empathy as a driver of prosocial behaviour: Highly conserved neurobehavioural mechanisms across species. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 371( 1686), 20150077. |
[31] | Decety J., & Yoder K.J . (2016). Empathy and motivation for justice: Cognitive empathy and concern, but not emotional empathy, predict sensitivity to injustice for others. Social Neuroscience, 11( 1), 1-14. |
[32] | Decety J. &Yoder K.J . (2017). The emerging social neuroscience of justice motivation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21( 1), 6-14. |
[33] | DeScioli P., & Kurzban R , (2009). Mysteries of morality. Cognition, 112( 2), 281-299. |
[34] | Diamond A , (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64( 1), 135-168. |
[35] | Egas M, Riedl A , (2008). The economics of altruistic punishment and the maintenance of cooperation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275( 1637), 871-878. |
[36] | Eisenberg N, & Fabes, R. A . (1990). Empathy: Conceptualization, measurement, and relation to prosocial behavior. Motivation and Emotion, 14( 2), 131-149. |
[37] | Eisenberg N., Spinrad T. L.& Morris A., , (2013). Empathy- related responding in children. In M. Killen, & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of Moral Development (2nd ed.). New York: Psychology Press. |
[38] | Enge S., Mothes H., Fleischhauer M., Reif A., & Strobel A . (2017). Genetic variation of dopamine and serotonin function modulates the feedback-related negativity during altruistic punishment. Scientific Reports, 7, 2996. |
[39] | Fehr E., Bernhard H., & Rockenbach B . (2008). Egalitarianism in young children. Nature, 454( 7208), 1079-1083. |
[40] |
Fehr E., Fischbacher U , (2004a). Social norms and human cooperation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8( 4), 185-190.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.007 |
[41] |
Fehr E., Fischbacher U , (2004b). Third-party punishment and social norms. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25( 2), 63-87.
doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(04)00005-4 |
[42] | Fehr E., & Gächter S ,(2002). Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature, 415( 6868), 137-140. |
[43] | Fehr E. Fehr E., & Rockenbach B , (2003). Detrimental effects of sanctions on human altruism. Nature, 422( 6928), 137-140. |
[44] | Fehr E., & Schurtenberger I , (2018). Normative foundations of human cooperation. Nature Human Behaviour, 2( 7), 458-468. |
[45] | Feng C., Deshpande G., Liu C., Gu R., Luo Y. J., & Krueger F . (2016). Diffusion of responsibility attenuates altruistic punishment: A functional magnetic resonance imaging effective connectivity study. Human Brain Mapping, 37( 2), 663-677. |
[46] | Fernández-Martín A.,& Calvo M.G . (2016). Selective orienting to pleasant versus unpleasant visual scenes. Cognition, 155, 108-112. |
[47] | Friehe T.,& Schildberg-Hörisch H , (2018). Predicting norm enforcement: The individual and joint predictive power of economic preferences, personality, and self-control. European Journal of Law and Economics, 45( 1), 127-146. |
[48] | Fukushima H, & Hiraki K , (2009). Whose loss is it? Human electrophysiological correlates of non-self reward processing. Social Neuroscience, 4( 3), 261-275. |
[49] | Geeraerts S. B., Hessels R. S., van der Stigchel S., Huijding J., Endendijk J. J., van den Boomen C., .. Deković M . (2019). Individual differences in visual attention and self-regulation: A multimethod longitudinal study from infancy to toddlerhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 180, 104-112. |
[50] | Glass L., Moody L., Grafman J., & Krueger F . (2016). Neural signatures of third-party punishment: Evidence from penetrating traumatic brain injury. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11( 2), 253-262. |
[51] |
Grecucci A., Giorgetta C., van’t Wout M., Bonini N., & Sanfey A. G . (2013). Reappraising the ultimatum: An fMRI study of emotion regulation and decision making. Cerebral Cortex, 23( 2), 399-410.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs028 |
[52] | Grocke P., Rossano F., & Tomasello M . (2015). Procedural justice in children: Preschoolers accept unequal resource distributions if the procedure provides equal opportunities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 140, 197-210. |
[53] | Gromet D.M., &Darley J.M . (2009). Punishment and beyond: Achieving justice through the satisfaction of multiple goals. Law & Society Review, 43( 1), 1-38. |
[54] |
Gummerum M.,& Chu M.T . (2014). Outcomes and intentions in children's, adolescents', and adults' second- and third-party punishment behavior. Cognition, 133( 1), 97-103.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.06.001 |
[55] | Gummerum M., van Dillen L. F., van Dijk E., & López-Pérez B . (2016). Costly third-party interventions: The role of incidental anger and attention focus in punishment of the perpetrator and compensation of the victim. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 65, 94-104. |
[56] | Henrich J., Ensminger J., McElreath R., Barr A., Barrett C., Bolyanatz A., .. Ziker J . (2010). Markets, religion, community size, and the evolution of fairness and punishment. Science, 327( 5972), 1480-1484. |
[57] | Henrich J., McElreath R., Barr A., Ensminger J., Barrett C., Bolyanatz A., .. Ziker J . (2006). Costly punishment across human societies. Science, 312( 5781), 1767-1770. |
[58] | Hu Y., Strang S., & Weber B . (2015). Helping or punishing strangers: Neural correlates of altruistic decisions as third-party and of its relation to empathic concern. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 24. |
[59] | Jordan J. J., Hoffman M., Bloom P., & Rand D. G . (2016). Third-party punishment as a costly signal of trustworthiness. Nature, 530( 7591), 473-476. |
[60] | Kahneman D (2011). Thinking Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. |
[61] | Kinner V. L., Kuchinke L., Dierolf A. M., Merz C. J., Otto T., & Wolf O. T . (2017). What our eyes tell us about feelings: Tracking pupillary responses during emotion regulation processes. Psychophysiology, 54( 4), 508-518. |
[62] | Krueger F., & Hoffman M , (2016). The emerging neuroscience of third-party punishment. Trends in Neurosciences, 39( 8), 499-501. |
[63] |
Kurzban R.,& DeScioli P ,(2013). Adaptationist punishment in humans. Journal of Bioeconomics, 15( 3), 269-279.
doi: 10.1007/s10818-013-9153-9 |
[64] | Lahey J.N., & Oxley D , (2016). The power of eye tracking in economics experiments. American Economic Review, 106( 5), 309-313. |
[65] |
Leliveld M. C., van Dijk E., & van beest I . (2012). Punishing and compensating others at your own expense: The role of empathic concern on reactions to distributive injustice. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42( 2), 135-140.
doi: 10.1002/ejsp.872 |
[66] | Li Z. Q., Wu X. Y., Zhang L. S., & Zhang Z. Y . (2017). Habitual cognitive reappraisal was negatively related to perceived immorality in the harm and fairness domains. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1805. |
[67] | Liu Y. J., Li L., Zheng L., & Guo X. Y . (2017). Punish the perpetrator or compensate the victim? Gain vs. loss context modulate third-party altruistic behaviors. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2066. |
[68] | Logan G. D., Cowan W. B., & Davis K. A . (1984). On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: A model and a method. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10( 2), 276-291. |
[69] | Lotz S., Baumert A., Schlösser T., Gresser F., & Fetchenhauer D . (2011). Individual differences in third-party interventions: How justice sensitivity shapes altruistic punishment. Negotiation and Conflict Management Research, 4( 4), 297-313. |
[70] |
Lotz S., Okimoto T. G., Schlösser T., & Fetchenhauer D . (2011). Punitive versus compensatory reactions to injustice: Emotional antecedents to third-party interventions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47( 2), 477-480.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.10.004 |
[71] | Lu T.Z., & McKeown S , (2018). The effects of empathy, perceived injustice and group identity on altruistic preferences: Towards compensation or punishment. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 48( 12), 683-691. |
[72] | McAuliffe K., Jordan J. J., & Warneken F . (2015). Costly third-party punishment in young children. Cognition, 134, 1-10. |
[73] | McAuliffe K., Raihani N. J., & Dunham Y . (2017). Children are sensitive to norms of giving. Cognition, 167, 151-159. |
[74] | McRae K., Gross J. J., Weber J., Robertson E. R., Sokol-Hessner P., Ray R. D., .. Ochsner K. N . (2012). The development of emotion regulation: An fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal in children, adolescents and young adults. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7( 1), 11-22. |
[75] | Mikula G., Scherer K. R., & Athenstaedt U . (1998). The role of injustice in the elicitation of differential emotional reactions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24( 7), 769-783. |
[76] | Miller E.., &Cohen J.D . (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24( 1), 167-202. |
[77] | Mothes H., Enge S., & Strobel A . (2016). The interplay between feedback-related negativity and individual differences in altruistic punishment: An EEG study. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16( 2), 276-288. |
[78] | Müller-Leinß J-M., Enzi B., Flasbeck V., & Brüne M . (2018). Retaliation or selfishness? An rTMS investigation of the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in prosocial motives. Social Neuroscience, 13( 6), 701-709. |
[79] | Nelissen R. M.A., &Zeelenberg M ,(2009). Moral emotions as determinants of third-party punishment: Anger and guilt and the functions of altruistic sanctions. Judgment and Decision Making, 4( 7), 543-553. |
[80] | Overbye K., Huster R. J., Walhovd K. B., Fjell A. M., & Tamnes C. K . (2018). Development of the P300 from childhood to adulthood: A multimodal EEG and MRI study. Brain Structure and Function, 223( 9), 4337-4349. |
[81] | Patil I., Zanon M., Novembre G., Zangrando N., Chittaro L., & Silani G . (2018). Neuroanatomical basis of concern- based altruism in virtual environment. Neuropsychologia, 116, 34-43. |
[82] | Raihani N.J., &Bshary R , (2015). The reputation of punishers. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 30( 2), 98-103. |
[83] |
Rand D. G., Greene J. D., & Nowak M. A . (2012). Spontaneous giving and calculated greed. Nature, 489( 7416), 427-430.
doi: 10.1038/nature11467 |
[84] | Riedl K., Jensen K., Call J., & Tomasello M . (2012). No third-party punishment in chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109( 37), 14824-14829. |
[85] | Riggs K. J., Jolley R. P., & Simpson A . (2013). The role of inhibitory control in the development of human figure drawing in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114( 4), 537-542. |
[86] | Rogoff B., (2003).The cultural nature of human development. New York: Oxford University Press. |
[87] | Rosen M. L., Stern C. E., Michalka S. W., Devaney K. J., & Somers D. C . (2015). Cognitive control network contributions to memory-guided visual attention. Cerebral Cortex, 26( 5), 2059-2073. |
[88] | Saadeh W., Rizzo C. P., & Roberts D. G . (2002). Spanking. Clinical Pediatrics, 41( 2), 87-91. |
[89] | Salali G. D., Juda M., & Henrich J . (2015). Transmission and development of costly punishment in children. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36( 2), 86-94. |
[90] | Schmitt M., Gollwitzer M., Maes J., & Arbach D . (2005). Justice sensitivity: Assessment and location in the personality space. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 21, 202-211. |
[91] | Schmitt M. J., Neumann R., & Montada L . (1995). Dispositional sensitivity to befallen injustice. Social Justice Research, 8( 4), 385-407. |
[92] | Sheppes G.., . (2014). Emotion regulation choice:heory and findings. In Gross, J. J. (Ed.). Handbook of Emotion Regulation (pp. 126-139). New York: Guilford Press. |
[93] | Sheskin M., Nadal A., Croom A., Mayer T., Nissel J., & Bloom P . (2016). Some equalities are more equal than others: Quality equality emerges later than numerical equality. Child Development, 87( 5), 1520-1528. |
[94] | Shulman E. P., Smith A. R., Silva K., Icenogle G., Duell N., Chein J., & Steinberg L . (2016). The dual systems model: Review, reappraisal, and reaffirmation. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 103-117. |
[95] | Sober E., &Wilson D. S. .(1998) .Unto others:The evolution and psychology of unselfish behavior Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press The evolution and psychology of unselfish behavior . Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press. |
[96] | Spence M.., (1974). Market Signaling:Informational Transfer in Hiring and Related Screening Processes (Harvard Economic Studies) . Cambridge, Mass:Harvard University Press. |
[97] |
Steinberg L.., (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review, 28( 1), 78-106.
doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002 |
[98] | Sun L., Tan P. S., Cheng Y., Chen J. W., & Qu C . (2015). The effect of altruistic tendency on fairness in third-party punishment. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 820. |
[99] | Tan F.F., & Xiao E. , (2018). Third-party punishment: Retribution or deterrence? Journal of Economic Psychology, 67, 34-46. |
[100] | Tomasello M., (2009). Why We Cooperate. Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press. |
[101] | Vaish A..,& Tomasello M. , (2013). Origins of human cooperation and morality. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 231-255. |
[102] | vanProoijen J.W . (2010). Retributive versus compensatory justice: Observers’ preference for punishing in response to criminal offenses. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40( 1), 72-85. |
[103] | Vanderhasselt M., de Raedt R., & Baeken C . (2009). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and stroop performance: Tackling the lateralization. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16( 3), 609-612. |
[104] | Wang J. T., Spezio M., & Camerer C. F . (2010). Pinocchio's pupil: Using eye tracking and pupil dilation to understand truth telling and deception in sender-receiver games. The American Economic Review, 100( 3), 984-1007. |
[105] | Warneken F., &Tomasello M . (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311( 5765), 1301-1303. |
[106] |
Warneken F,. &Tomasello M . (2013). Parental presence and encouragement do not influence helping in young children. Infancy, 18( 3), 345-368.
doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00120.x |
[107] | Wechsler D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children-WISCIV. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation. |
[108] | Wilson D.S., &Sober E. , (1994). Reintroducing group selection to the human behavioral sciences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17( 4), 585-608. |
[109] | Xie D., Pei M., & Su Y . (2019). “Favoring my playmate seems fair”: Inhibitory control and theory of mind in preschoolers’ self-disadvantaging behaviors. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 184, 158-173. |
[110] | Yan Z. Q., Pei M., & Su Y. J . (2018). Physical cue influences children's empathy for pain: The role of attention allocation. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2378. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02378 |
[111] | Yang F., Choi Y. J., Misch A., Yang X., & Dunham Y . (2018). In defense of the commons: Young children negatively evaluate and sanction free riders. Psychological Science, 29( 10), 1598-1611. |
[112] |
Zaki J., &Mitchell J.P . (2013). Intuitive prosociality. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22( 6), 466-470.
doi: 10.1177/0963721413492764 |
[1] | 叶舒琪, 尹俊婷, 李招贤, 罗俊龙. 情绪对直觉与分析加工的影响机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(5): 736-746. |
[2] | 郭理, 加锁锁, 李圭泉, 李蔓林. 高处不胜寒?领导工作场所孤独感的多层次双刃剑效应[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(4): 582-596. |
[3] | 杨庆, 李亚琴. 不确定是坏的么?不确定状态中的错误加工特点及其解释机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(3): 338-349. |
[4] | 杨智超, 王艇. 消费决策中的零:零价格效应和零比较效应[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(3): 492-506. |
[5] | 马原啸, 陈旭. 焦虑易感群体焦虑识别与消退中催产素的作用[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(1): 10-19. |
[6] | 肖婷炜, 董洁, 梁飞, 王福顺, 李洋. 厌恶情绪与自杀行为的关系[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(1): 87-98. |
[7] | 寇东晓, 王晓玉. 权力对人际敏感性的影响[J]. 心理科学进展, 2023, 31(1): 108-115. |
[8] | 邹迪, 李红, 王福顺. 唤醒定义探析及其认知神经生理基础[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(9): 2020-2033. |
[9] | 刘薇, 沈晓玲. 团队行动中反思与团队创新关系研究的动态视角——认知与情绪的双元路径[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(8): 1759-1769. |
[10] | 梁飞, 江瑶, 肖婷炜, 董洁, 王福顺. 基本情绪的神经基础:来自fMRI与机器视觉技术研究的证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(8): 1832-1843. |
[11] | 杨倩. 负性情绪在冲突适应中的作用机制:分离与整合视角[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(8): 1844-1855. |
[12] | 刘春晓, 刘立志, 王丹, 陈文锋. 集体仪式促进群体情绪感染的机制[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(8): 1870-1882. |
[13] | 王燕青, 龚少英, 姜甜甜, 吴亚男. 情感代理能否提高多媒体学习的效果?[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(7): 1524-1535. |
[14] | 武晓菲, 肖风, 罗劲. 创造性认知重评在情绪调节中的迁移效应及其神经基础[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(3): 477-485. |
[15] | 刘俊材, 冉光明, 张琪. 不同情绪载体的神经活动及其异同——脑成像研究的ALE元分析[J]. 心理科学进展, 2022, 30(3): 536-555. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||