Acta Psychologica Sinica ›› 2021, Vol. 53 ›› Issue (2): 139-154.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00139
• Reports of Empirical Studies • Previous Articles Next Articles
WANG Congxing1, YANG Yuqing1, XIONG Meng2,3, YE Yiduo1
Received:
2020-05-21
Published:
2021-02-25
Online:
2020-12-29
Contact:
YE Yiduo
WANG Congxing, YANG Yuqing, XIONG Meng, YE Yiduo. (2021). The influence of feature integration and processing depth on metaphorical association between moral concepts and container space. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 53(2), 139-154.
Add to citation manager EndNote|Ris|BibTeX
URL: https://journal.psych.ac.cn/acps/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00139
Containers Space | Lexical type | |
---|---|---|
Moral | Immoral | |
Inside | 629.42 ± 74.04 | 656.88 ± 80.22 |
Exterior | 632.65 ± 68.71 | 656.59 ± 83.46 |
Table 1 The reaction time and standard deviation of moral part-of-speech judgment in the analysis
Containers Space | Lexical type | |
---|---|---|
Moral | Immoral | |
Inside | 629.42 ± 74.04 | 656.88 ± 80.22 |
Exterior | 632.65 ± 68.71 | 656.59 ± 83.46 |
Containers Space | Lexical type | |
---|---|---|
Moral | Immoral | |
Inside | 0.963 ± 0.040 | 0.941 ± 0.036 |
Exterior | 0.959 ± 0.035 | 0.950 ± 0.035 |
Table 2 The accuracy and standard deviation of moral part-of-speech judgment in the analysis of the participants
Containers Space | Lexical type | |
---|---|---|
Moral | Immoral | |
Inside | 0.963 ± 0.040 | 0.941 ± 0.036 |
Exterior | 0.959 ± 0.035 | 0.950 ± 0.035 |
Containers Space | Deeper processing | Shallow work | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Moral | Immoral | Moral word | Immoral word | |
Inside | 656.87 ± 116.66 | 708.85 ± 103.84 | 711.77 ± 134.39 | 729.15 ± 140.91 |
Exterior | 692.37 ± 89.07 | 719.07 ± 106.62 | 707.01 ± 126.32 | 732.66 ± 138.11 |
Table 3 The reaction time and standard deviation of moral part-of-speech judgment in the analysis
Containers Space | Deeper processing | Shallow work | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Moral | Immoral | Moral word | Immoral word | |
Inside | 656.87 ± 116.66 | 708.85 ± 103.84 | 711.77 ± 134.39 | 729.15 ± 140.91 |
Exterior | 692.37 ± 89.07 | 719.07 ± 106.62 | 707.01 ± 126.32 | 732.66 ± 138.11 |
Containers Space | Deep | Shallow | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Moral | Immoral | Moral | Immoral | |
Inside | 0.973 ± 0.031 | 0.962 ± 0.035 | 0.961 ± 0.054 | 0.960 ± 0.047 |
Exterior | 0.972 ± 0.037 | 0.959 ± 0.048 | 0.970 ± 0.039 | 0.952 ± 0.050 |
Table 4 The accuracy and standard deviation of moral part-of-speech judgment in the analysis of the participants
Containers Space | Deep | Shallow | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Moral | Immoral | Moral | Immoral | |
Inside | 0.973 ± 0.031 | 0.962 ± 0.035 | 0.961 ± 0.054 | 0.960 ± 0.047 |
Exterior | 0.972 ± 0.037 | 0.959 ± 0.048 | 0.970 ± 0.039 | 0.952 ± 0.050 |
Containers Space | Deep | Shallow | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Moral | Immoral | Moral | Immoral | |
Inside | 609.61 ± 146.76 | 648.89 ± 148.33 | 610.06 ± 143.87 | 610.00 ± 157.97 |
Exterior | 625.65 ± 138.77 | 630.38 ± 117.37 | 649.27 ± 157.14 | 648.95 ± 152.28 |
Table 5 Reaction Time and standard deviation of letter judgment in participants analysis
Containers Space | Deep | Shallow | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Moral | Immoral | Moral | Immoral | |
Inside | 609.61 ± 146.76 | 648.89 ± 148.33 | 610.06 ± 143.87 | 610.00 ± 157.97 |
Exterior | 625.65 ± 138.77 | 630.38 ± 117.37 | 649.27 ± 157.14 | 648.95 ± 152.28 |
Containers Space | Deeper | Shallow | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Moral | Immoral | Moral | Immoral | |
Inside | 0.953 ± 0.054 | 0.943 ± 0.057 | 0.974 ± 0.032 | 0.973 ± 0.023 |
Exterior | 0.968 ± 0.041 | 0.925 ± 0.077 | 0.969 ± 0.035 | 0.956 ± 0.040 |
Table 6 Accuracy and standard deviation of letter judgment in participants analysis
Containers Space | Deeper | Shallow | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Moral | Immoral | Moral | Immoral | |
Inside | 0.953 ± 0.054 | 0.943 ± 0.057 | 0.974 ± 0.032 | 0.973 ± 0.023 |
Exterior | 0.968 ± 0.041 | 0.925 ± 0.077 | 0.969 ± 0.035 | 0.956 ± 0.040 |
Containers Space | Lexical type | |
---|---|---|
Moral | Immoral | |
Inside | 902.36 ± 173.14 | 1009.14 ± 204.53 |
Exterior | 1007.98 ± 226.86 | 984.90 ± 207.61 |
Table 7 The reaction time and standard deviation of part-of-speech judgment in participants analysis
Containers Space | Lexical type | |
---|---|---|
Moral | Immoral | |
Inside | 902.36 ± 173.14 | 1009.14 ± 204.53 |
Exterior | 1007.98 ± 226.86 | 984.90 ± 207.61 |
Containers Space | Lexical type | |
---|---|---|
Moral | Immoral | |
Inside | 0.948 ± 0.042 | 0.881 ± 0.070 |
Exterior | 0.879 ± 0.096 | 0.931 ± 0.045 |
Table 8 The accuracy and standard deviation of part-of-speech judgment in the subject analysis
Containers Space | Lexical type | |
---|---|---|
Moral | Immoral | |
Inside | 0.948 ± 0.042 | 0.881 ± 0.070 |
Exterior | 0.879 ± 0.096 | 0.931 ± 0.045 |
Containers Space | Lexical type | |
---|---|---|
Moral | Immoral | |
Inside | 981.94 ± 151.04 | 1142.86 ± 176.78 |
Exterior | 1142.55 ± 179.44 | 1080.46 ± 175.93 |
Table 9 Reaction Time and standard deviation of container space judgment in subject analysis
Containers Space | Lexical type | |
---|---|---|
Moral | Immoral | |
Inside | 981.94 ± 151.04 | 1142.86 ± 176.78 |
Exterior | 1142.55 ± 179.44 | 1080.46 ± 175.93 |
Containers Space | Lexical type | |
---|---|---|
Moral | Immoral | |
Inside | 0.965 ± 0.032 | 0.888 ± 0.055 |
Exterior | 0.907 ± 0.057 | 0.929 ± 0.056 |
Table 10 Reaction time and standard deviation of container space judgment in subject analysis
Containers Space | Lexical type | |
---|---|---|
Moral | Immoral | |
Inside | 0.965 ± 0.032 | 0.888 ± 0.055 |
Exterior | 0.907 ± 0.057 | 0.929 ± 0.056 |
[1] |
Amer, T., Gozli, D. G., & Pratt, J. (2017). Biasing spatial attention with semantic information: An event coding approach. Psychological Research, 82(3), 1-19.
doi: 10.1007/s00426-018-0970-2 URL |
[2] |
Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(4), 577-660.
doi: 10.1017/S0140525X99002149 URL |
[3] |
Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617-645.
doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639 URL pmid: 17705682 |
[4] |
Bobro, M. E. (1999). Leibniz on embodiment and the moral order. The Southern journal of philosophy, 37(3), 377-396.
doi: 10.1111/sjp.1999.37.issue-3 URL |
[5] |
Boot, I., & Pecher, D. (2011). Representation of categories: Metaphorical use of the container schema. Experimental Psychology, 58(2), 162-170.
URL pmid: 20705546 |
[6] |
Borghi, A. M., Binkofski, F., Castelfranchi, C., Cimatti, F., Scorolli, C., & Tummolini, L. (2017). The challenge of abstract concepts. Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 263-292.
doi: 10.1037/bul0000089 URL pmid: 28095000 |
[7] | Buss, M. D. (2015). Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind (Y. Zhang, & K. Jiang, Trans.). New York and London: Taylor & Francis Group. (Original work published 1999) |
[8] | Casasanto, D. (2017). The hierarchical structure of mental metaphors. In B. Hampe (Ed.), Metaphor: Embodied cognition and discourse (pp. 46-61). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. |
[9] |
Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106(2), 579-593.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.004 URL pmid: 17509553 |
[10] |
Casasanto, D., & Bottini, R. (2014). Mirror reading can reverse the flow of time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(2), 473-479.
doi: 10.1037/a0033297 URL |
[11] | Chen, X., Jiang, Q., Hou, M., & Zhu, M. Y. (2014). Embodied morality: A new approach in moral psychology. Psychological Development and Education, 30(6), 664-672. |
[12] |
Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11(6), 671-684.
doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(72)80001-X URL |
[13] | Fischer, E. (2017). Two analogy strategies: The cases of mind metaphors and introspection. Association Science. 30(2), 211-243 |
[14] | Gibbs, R. W., Jr. (2006) Embodiment and cognitive science. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. |
[15] |
He, X. L., & Chen, J. (2020). Cognitive development of multiple metaphors of power concepts in 3~5 year-old children. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 52(2), 149-161.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.00149 URL |
[16] |
Holyoak, K. J., & Stamenković, D. (2018). Metaphor comprehension: A critical review of theories and evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 144(6), 641-671.
URL pmid: 29517263 |
[17] |
Hommel, B. (1998). Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual- task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 24(5), 1368-1384.
doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.24.5.1368 URL |
[18] |
Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(11), 494-500.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.08.007 URL pmid: 15491903 |
[19] |
Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 849-878.
doi: 10.1017/S0140525X01000103 URL |
[20] |
Huang, Y., Tse, C.-S., & Xie, J. (2018). The bidirectional congruency effect of brightness-valence metaphoric association in the Stroop-like and priming paradigms. Acta Psychologica, 189(10), 76-92.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.10.006 URL |
[21] | Jia, L., & Jiang, G. F. (2016). The vertical spatial metaphor of moral concepts: Psychological reality and bidirectional mapping. Psychological Development and Education, 32(2), 158-165. |
[22] |
Kahneman, D., Treisman, A., & Gibbs, B. (1992). The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information. Cognitive Psychology, 24(2), 175-219.
doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(92)90007-o URL pmid: 1582172 |
[23] | Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (Eds).(1980) Metaphors We Live By. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. |
[24] | Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (Eds). (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books. |
[25] |
Lee, S. W. S., & Schwarz, N. (2012). Bidirectionality, mediation, and moderation of metaphorical effects: The embodiment of social suspicion and fishy smells. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(5), 737-749.
doi: 10.1037/a0029708 URL |
[26] |
Li, Y., Zhang, C., & Wang, Y. (2019). The effect of moral emotions on the metaphorical mapping of morality and its neural mechanism. Advances in Psychological Science, 27(7), 1224-1231.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2019.01224 URL |
[27] |
Liu, W. J., Shen, M. Q., Li, Y., & Wang, R. M. (2016). The interaction between emotional concept processing and emotional face perception. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 48(2), 163-173.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2016.00163 URL |
[28] |
Louwerse, M. M. (2008). Embodied relations are encoded in language. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 838-844.
doi: 10.3758/pbr.15.4.838 URL pmid: 18792513 |
[29] |
Louwerse, M. M., & Jeuniaux, P. (2010). The linguistic and embodied nature of conceptual processing. Cognition, 114(1), 96-104.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.09.002 URL pmid: 19818435 |
[30] | Lu, Z. Y., Guo, S. P., & Jiang, Z. L. (2017). The size metaphor of moral concepts: Psychological reality and mapping relationship. Journal of South China Normal University (Social Science Edition), 49(2), 70-78. |
[31] |
Lu, Z. Y., Jia, L. L., & Zhai, D. X. (2017). The mapping for vertical spatial metaphor of the moral concepts: Bidirectional and unbalanced. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 49(2), 186-196.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2017.00186 URL |
[32] |
Meier, B. P., Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2004). Why good guys wear white: Automatic inferences about stimulus valence based on brightness. Psychological Science, 15(2), 82-87.
doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01502002.x URL pmid: 14738513 |
[33] |
Meier, B. P., Sellbom, M., & Wygant, D. B. (2007). Failing to take the moral high ground: Psychopathy and the vertical representation of morality. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(4), 757-767.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.02.001 URL |
[34] |
Pitt, B., & Casasanto, D. (2020). The correlations in experience principle: How culture shapes concepts of time and number. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(6), 1048-1070.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000696 URL |
[35] |
Riello, M., & Rusconi, E. (2011). Unimanual SNARC effect: Hand matters. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 372.
URL pmid: 22207856 |
[36] | Treisman, A. M. (1998). Feature binding, attention and object perception. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences, 353(1373), 1295-1306. |
[37] |
Wang, B., Fu, Y., & Zhang, J. J. (2019). Influence of language and culture on retrieval-induced forgetting under the self-referential condition: Evidence from the Han and the Mosuo. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 51(4), 450-461.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.00450 URL |
[38] |
Wang, C. X., Ma, J. P., Deng, J., Yang, Z. W., & Ye, Y. D. (2020). The depth of conceptual processing affects the metaphorical association between moral concepts and horizontal orientation. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 52(4), 426-439.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.00426 URL |
[39] |
Wang, X. X., Jiang, S., & Zhang, J. J. (2018). Effect of the spatial linguistic symbol on the container metaphor of seniority rules. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 50(9), 953-964.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.00953 URL |
[40] |
Wang, Z., & Lu, Z. Y. (2013). The vertical spatial metaphor of moral concepts and its influence on cognition. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 45(5), 538-545.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2013.00538 URL |
[41] | Wang, Z. H., & Yao, Z. (2012). Concreteness effects of emotional noun words: Evidences from ERP. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 44(2), 154-165. |
[42] |
Williams, L. E., & Bargh, J. A. (2008). Experiencing physical warmth promotes interpersonal warmth. Science, 322(5901), 606-607.
URL pmid: 18948544 |
[43] |
Williams, L. E., Huang, J. Y., & Bargh, J. A. (2009). The scaffolded mind: Higher mental processes are grounded in early experience of the physical world. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39(7), 1257-1267.
doi: 10.1002/ejsp.665 URL pmid: 20046813 |
[44] |
Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 625-636.
doi: 10.3758/bf03196322 URL pmid: 12613670 |
[45] | Yan, S. C. (2011). Physical cleanliness and morality. Advances in Psychological Science, 19(8), 1242-1248. |
[46] |
Yang, J. P., Guo, X. M., & Wang, X. C. (2017). Metaphorical representation of moral concepts: Evidence from red/white color, left/right position and upright/skew font. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 49(7), 875-885.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2017.00875 URL |
[47] | Ye, H. S. (Ed).(2017) Principles and applications of embodied cognition. Beijing, China: The Commercial Press. |
[48] |
Yin, R., & Ye, H. S. (2014). The black and white metaphor representation of moral concepts and its influence on moral cognition. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 46 (9), 1331-1346.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2014.01331 URL |
[49] |
Yin, R., Su, D. Q., & Ye, H. S. (2013). Conceptual Metaphor Theory: Basing on Theories of Embodied Cognition. Advances in Psychological Science, 21(2), 220-234.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.00220 URL |
[50] |
Yu, N., Wang, T. F., & He, Y. L. (2016). Spatial subsystem of moral metaphors: A cognitive semantic Study. Metaphor and Symbol, 31(4), 195-211.
doi: 10.1080/10926488.2016.1223470 URL |
[51] | Zhao, Y., & Wu, L. (2019). The representation of moral metaphor in the vision of embodied cognition. Psychological Exploration, 39(4), 308-313. |
[52] |
Zhong, C. B., & Leonardelli, G. J. (2008). Cold and lonely: Does social exclusion literally feel cold? Psychological Science, 19(9), 838-842.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02165.x URL pmid: 18947346 |
[53] | Zhu, Y., & Zhang, L. (2001). Experimental research on the self-reference effect. Science in China, 31(6), 537-543. |
[1] | ZHAN Youlong, XIAO Xiao, TAN Qianbao, LI Jin, ZHONG Yiping. Influence of reputational concern and social distance on moral decision-making under the harmful dilemma: Evidence from behavioral and ERPs study [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2022, 54(6): 613-627. |
[2] | LI Rui, XIA Ling-Xiang. The mediating effect of aggression motivation on the relationship between trait anger and reactive aggression: A longitudinal study [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2021, 53(7): 788-797. |
[3] | DUAN Shen, MENG Lu. The polarization effect of project presentation in ranking list on project evaluation [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2021, 53(11): 1271-1285. |
[4] | CUI Fang, YANG Jiamiao, GU Ruolei, LIU Jie. Functional connectivities of the right temporoparietal junction and moral network predict social framing effect: Evidence from resting-state fMRI [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2021, 53(1): 55-66. |
[5] | WANG Congxing, MA Jianping, DENG Jue, YANG Zhongwang, YE Yiduo. The depth of conceptual processing affects the metaphorical connection between moral concepts and horizontal orientation [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2020, 52(4): 426-439. |
[6] | ZHANG Jijia,FU Ya,WANG Bin. Gender culture influence on spatial and weight metaphors of kinship words: Evidence from Bai, Yi, and Mosuo nationalities [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2020, 52(4): 440-455. |
[7] | WANG Hanlin, JIANG Zeliang, FENG Xiaohui, LU Zhongyi. Spatial iconicity of moral concepts: Co-dependence of linguistic and embodied symbols [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2020, 52(2): 128-138. |
[8] | HE Xiaoling,CHEN Jun. Cognitive development of multiple metaphors of power concepts in 3~5 year-old children [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2020, 52(2): 149-161. |
[9] | FAN Yafeng, JIANG Jing, CUI Wenquan. The backfire effect of default amounts on donation behavior in online donation platform [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(4): 415-427. |
[10] | GENG Xiaowei,FANG Jinru,HAN Yanfang,LI Zhongquan,ZHAO Mi,YANG Ye. The influence of moral relativism and disgust on moral intuitive judgment [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(4): 517-526. |
[11] | MA Lijun,MA Yunxiao,HE Xiaoqing,LIU Haitao,ZHANG Jingyu. Processing of Chinese homophonic two-part allegoric sayings: Effects of familiarity and homophone [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(12): 1306-1317. |
[12] | CHEN Siyun, WEI Haiying, MENG Lu. The impact of congruency between moral appeal and social perception on charitable donation [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(12): 1351-1362. |
[13] | LYU Xiaokang, FU Chunye, WANG Xinjian. Effect and underlying mechanism of refutation texts on the trust and moral judgment of patients [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(10): 1171-1186. |
[14] | LI Xiao-dan,DING Dao-qun,YE Hao-sheng. The influence of embodied implicit power on fair decision making [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(1): 106-116. |
[15] | Xinxiao WANG, Shan JIANG, Jijia ZHANG. Effect of the spatial linguistic symbol on the container metaphor of seniority rules [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(9): 953-964. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||