Acta Psychologica Sinica ›› 2023, Vol. 55 ›› Issue (9): 1424-1440.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01424
• Reports of Empirical Studies • Previous Articles Next Articles
WANG Yang1,2, WEN Fangfang1,2, ZUO Bin3,1()
Published:
2023-09-25
Online:
2023-06-09
Contact:
ZUO Bin
E-mail:zuobin@mail.sysu.edu.cn
Supported by:
WANG Yang, WEN Fangfang, ZUO Bin. (2023). The preference and development for societal-type cues in 3- to 8-year-olds' perception of groups. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 55(9), 1424-1440.
Figure 1. Illustrations of experimental materials1 (1All rights reserved for the figures, if needed, please send email to xiayanyu99@163.com or wangyang_9501@mails.ccnu.edu.cn)
Variables | Triad classification task | Exclusion task | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
B | SE | z | p | B | SE | z | p | |
Fixed effects | ||||||||
Intercept | 0.79 | 0.63 | 1.25 | 0.211 | -1.20 | 0.80 | -1.50 | 0.133 |
Age | 0.08 | 0.10 | 0.81 | 0.416 | 0.38 | 0.13 | 2.87 | 0.004 |
Physical-type cues (gender - color) | 0.38 | 0.61 | 0.62 | 0.538 | 1.24 | 0.63 | 1.95 | 0.050 |
Physical-type cues (SES - color) | -0.60 | 0.60 | -0.99 | 0.321 | 1.91 | 0.64 | 2.99 | 0.003 |
Societal-type cues (group belonging - norm) | -1.14 | 0.56 | -2.04 | 0.042 | -0.65 | 0.61 | -1.06 | 0.288 |
Societal-type cues (common interest - norm) | 1.58 | 0.63 | 2.50 | 0.013 | 1.86 | 0.67 | 2.78 | 0.005 |
Age × physical cues (gender - color) | -0.14 | 0.10 | -1.40 | 0.162 | -0.16 | 0.10 | -1.49 | 0.137 |
Age × physical cues (SES - color) | 0.00 | 0.10 | 0.01 | 0.996 | -0.27 | 0.10 | -2.60 | 0.009 |
Age × social cues (group belonging - norm) | 0.27 | 0.09 | 2.94 | 0.003 | 0.07 | 0.10 | 0.66 | 0.511 |
Age × social cues (common interest - norm) | -0.05 | 0.10 | -0.51 | 0.610 | -0.15 | 0.11 | -1.38 | 0.167 |
Random effects | Variance | SD | Variance | SD | ||||
Child participants | 1.32 | 1.15 | 3.74 | 1.94 |
Table 1 Results of the generalized linear mixed model of age, physical-type, and societal-type cues in predicting children's cue-type selection
Variables | Triad classification task | Exclusion task | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
B | SE | z | p | B | SE | z | p | |
Fixed effects | ||||||||
Intercept | 0.79 | 0.63 | 1.25 | 0.211 | -1.20 | 0.80 | -1.50 | 0.133 |
Age | 0.08 | 0.10 | 0.81 | 0.416 | 0.38 | 0.13 | 2.87 | 0.004 |
Physical-type cues (gender - color) | 0.38 | 0.61 | 0.62 | 0.538 | 1.24 | 0.63 | 1.95 | 0.050 |
Physical-type cues (SES - color) | -0.60 | 0.60 | -0.99 | 0.321 | 1.91 | 0.64 | 2.99 | 0.003 |
Societal-type cues (group belonging - norm) | -1.14 | 0.56 | -2.04 | 0.042 | -0.65 | 0.61 | -1.06 | 0.288 |
Societal-type cues (common interest - norm) | 1.58 | 0.63 | 2.50 | 0.013 | 1.86 | 0.67 | 2.78 | 0.005 |
Age × physical cues (gender - color) | -0.14 | 0.10 | -1.40 | 0.162 | -0.16 | 0.10 | -1.49 | 0.137 |
Age × physical cues (SES - color) | 0.00 | 0.10 | 0.01 | 0.996 | -0.27 | 0.10 | -2.60 | 0.009 |
Age × social cues (group belonging - norm) | 0.27 | 0.09 | 2.94 | 0.003 | 0.07 | 0.10 | 0.66 | 0.511 |
Age × social cues (common interest - norm) | -0.05 | 0.10 | -0.51 | 0.610 | -0.15 | 0.11 | -1.38 | 0.167 |
Random effects | Variance | SD | Variance | SD | ||||
Child participants | 1.32 | 1.15 | 3.74 | 1.94 |
Experimental conditions | Triad classification task | Exclusion task | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
3-4 years | 5-6 years | 7-8 years | 3-4 years | 5-6 years | 7-8 years | |
Color × norm | 76.12 | 78.95 | 70.42 | 51.47 | 68.00 | 77.14 |
Color × group belonging | 67.16 | 86.84 | 87.32 | 56.92 | 61.64 | 70.42 |
Color × common interest | 80.60 | 89.47 | 83.10 | 67.16 | 82.69 | 76.06 |
Gender × norm | 65.67 | 68.42 | 57.75 | 66.67 | 66.18 | 76.06 |
Gender × group belonging | 70.15 | 74.68 | 80.28 | 58.21 | 68.49 | 71.83 |
Gender × common interest | 86.57 | 86.84 | 80.28 | 82.09 | 78.38 | 81.43 |
SES × norm | 58.21 | 67.11 | 64.79 | 74.63 | 61.11 | 73.24 |
SES × group belonging | 61.19 | 59.21 | 76.06 | 59.70 | 65.21 | 64.79 |
SES × common interests | 86.57 | 88.16 | 81.69 | 89.55 | 80.82 | 81.43 |
Table 2 Children's societal-type cue preferences in triad classification and exclusion tasks
Experimental conditions | Triad classification task | Exclusion task | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
3-4 years | 5-6 years | 7-8 years | 3-4 years | 5-6 years | 7-8 years | |
Color × norm | 76.12 | 78.95 | 70.42 | 51.47 | 68.00 | 77.14 |
Color × group belonging | 67.16 | 86.84 | 87.32 | 56.92 | 61.64 | 70.42 |
Color × common interest | 80.60 | 89.47 | 83.10 | 67.16 | 82.69 | 76.06 |
Gender × norm | 65.67 | 68.42 | 57.75 | 66.67 | 66.18 | 76.06 |
Gender × group belonging | 70.15 | 74.68 | 80.28 | 58.21 | 68.49 | 71.83 |
Gender × common interest | 86.57 | 86.84 | 80.28 | 82.09 | 78.38 | 81.43 |
SES × norm | 58.21 | 67.11 | 64.79 | 74.63 | 61.11 | 73.24 |
SES × group belonging | 61.19 | 59.21 | 76.06 | 59.70 | 65.21 | 64.79 |
SES × common interests | 86.57 | 88.16 | 81.69 | 89.55 | 80.82 | 81.43 |
Figure 5. The proportion of children's cues selection in the first effective categorization under color × gender × norm condition (bolded outline corresponds to societal-type cues)
Figure 6. The proportion of children's cues selection in the first effective categorization under socioeconomic status × belonging × interest (bolded outline corresponds to societal-type cues; SES indicates socioeconomic status cue)
Figure 7. An example of children categorizing by position feature. Note. ② is the result of a 4-year-old boy's categorization of ①. The child told the researcher that the upper and lower rows were different groups. ③ and ④ were for a 4-year-old girl categorization in two successive trials. This child was categorizing by socioeconomic status, and the positions of upper and lower rows were switched before and after a categorization.
Figure 8. Examples of categorization behaviors that were coded as fairness-dependent. Note. ① is a categorization of a 5-year-old boy, ② is a categorization of a 4-year-old girl.
[1] | Bates, D., Machler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2014). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1-48. |
[2] | Bender, R., & Lange, S. (2001). Adjusting for multiple testing -- when and how?. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 54(4), 343-349. |
[3] |
Bian, L., Leslie, S. J., & Cimpian, A. (2017). Gender stereotypes about intellectual ability emerge early and influence children's interests. Science, 355(6323), 389-391.
doi: 10.1126/science.aah6524 pmid: 28126816 |
[4] |
Bian, L., Sloane, S., & Baillargeon, R. (2018). Infants expect ingroup support to override fairness when resources are limited. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(11), 2705-2710.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1719445115 pmid: 29483252 |
[5] |
Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2007). Developmental intergroup theory: Explaining and reducing children's social stereotyping and prejudice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, Explaining and reducing Children's Social stereotyping and Prejudice. Current directions in Psychological Science, 16(3), 162-166.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00496.x URL |
[6] | Brewer, M. B., Hong, Y. Y., & Li, Q. (2004). Dynamic entitativity:Perceiving groups as actors. In V. Yzerbyt, C. Judd, & O. Corneille (Eds.), The psychology of group perception: contributions to the study of homogeneity, entitativity, and essentialism (pp. 25-38). New York: Psychology Press. |
[7] |
Burkholder, A. R., Elenbaas, L., & Killen, M. (2021). Giving priority to race or wealth in peer group contexts involving social inclusion. Developmental Psychology, 57(5), 651-661.
doi: 10.1037/dev0001178 pmid: 34166012 |
[8] | Chalik, L., & Rhodes, M. (2014). Preschoolers use social allegiances to predict behavior. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15(1), 136-160. |
[9] |
Chalik, L., Rivera, C., & Rhodes, M. (2014). Children's use of categories and mental states to predict social behavior. Developmental Psychology, 50(10), 2360-2367.
doi: 10.1037/a0037729 pmid: 25151249 |
[10] | Chen, X. M. (2000). Qualitative research in social sciences (pp. 227-229). Beijing: Education and Science Publishing House. |
[11] |
Coley, J. D. (2012). Where the wild things are: Informal experience and ecological reasoning. Child Development, 83(3), 992-1006.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01751.x pmid: 22548352 |
[12] |
de Martino, B., Kumaran, D., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Frames, biases, and rational decision-making in the human brain. Science, 313(5787), 684-687.
doi: 10.1126/science.1128356 pmid: 16888142 |
[13] | Du, D., & Su, Y. J. (2005). The development of children's gender stereotype and the influence of stereotyped information. Psychological Exploration, 25(4), 56-61. |
[14] |
Dunham, Y., Stepanova, E. V., Dotsch, R., & Todorov, A. (2015). The development of race-based perceptual categorization: Skin color dominates early category judgments. Developmental Science, 18(3), 469-483.
doi: 10.1111/desc.12228 pmid: 25154647 |
[15] |
Durante, F., & Fiske, S. T. (2017). How social-class stereotypes maintain inequality. Current Opinion in Psychology, 18, 43-48.
doi: S2352-250X(17)30047-7 pmid: 29221511 |
[16] | Fiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A continuum of impression formation, from category-based to individuating processes: Influences of information and motivation on attention and interpretation. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 23, 1-74. |
[17] | Foster-Hanson, E., & Rhodes, M. (2019). Normative social role concepts in early childhood. Cognitive Science, 43(8), e12782. |
[18] |
Hebart, M. N., Zheng, C. Y., Pereira, F., & Baker, C. I. (2020). Revealing the multidimensional mental representations of natural objects underlying human similarity judgements. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(11), 1173-1185.
doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-00951-3 pmid: 33046861 |
[19] | Jordan, A., & Dunham, Y. (2020). Are category labels primary? Children use similarities to reason about social groups. Developmental Science, 24(2), e13013. |
[20] |
Kelly, D. J., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., Lee, K., Ge, L., & Pascalis, O. (2007). The other-race effect develops during infancy: Evidence of perceptual narrowing. Psychological Science, 18(12), 1084-1089.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02029.x pmid: 18031416 |
[21] |
Kinzler, K. D., Shutts, K., & Correll, J. (2010). Priorities in social categories. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40(4), 581-592.
doi: 10.1002/ejsp.v40:4 URL |
[22] | Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1-26. |
[23] |
Lapidow, E., Tandon, T., Goddu, M., & Walker, C. M. (2021). A tale of three platforms: Investigating preschoolers' second-order inferences using in-person, Zoom, and Lookit methodologies. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 731404.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.731404 URL |
[24] |
Lei, R. F., Leshin, R. A., & Rhodes, M. (2020). The development of intersectional social prototypes. Psychological Science, 31(8), 911-926.
doi: 10.1177/0956797620920360 pmid: 32501742 |
[25] |
Li, X. M., & Tan, P. (2018). Applied research on framing effect and related techniques. Advances in Psychological Science, 26(12), 2230-2237.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.02230 URL |
[26] | Lin, C. D., Yang, Z. L., & Huang, X. T. (Eds.). (2003). The comprehensive dictionary of psychology. Shanghai: Shanghai Educational Publishing House. |
[27] |
Martin, C. L., Ruble, D. N., & Szkrybalo, J. (2002). Cognitive theories of early gender development. Psychological Bulletin, 128(6), 903-933.
doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.6.903 pmid: 12405137 |
[28] |
Monroe, B. M., Koenig, B. L., Wan, K. S., Laine, T., Gupta, S., & Ortony, A. (2018). Re-examining dominance of categories in impression formation: A test of dual-process models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 115(1), 1-30.
doi: 10.1037/pspa0000119 pmid: 29911880 |
[29] |
Platow, M. J., Grace, D. M., & Smithson, M. J. (2012). Examining the preconditions for psychological group membership: Perceived social interdependence as the outcome of self-categorization. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(1), 5-13.
doi: 10.1177/1948550611407081 URL |
[30] |
Rhodes, M. (2013). How two intuitive theories shape the development of social categorization. Child Development Perspectives, 7(1), 12-16.
doi: 10.1111/cdep.12007 URL |
[31] |
Rhodes, M., & Baron, A. (2019). The development of social categorization. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 1, 359-386.
doi: 10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121318-084824 pmid: 33103119 |
[32] |
Rhodes, M., & Chalik, L. (2013). Social categories as markers of intrinsic interpersonal obligations. Psychological Science, 24(6), 999-1006.
doi: 10.1177/0956797612466267 pmid: 23613213 |
[33] |
Rhodes, M., Rizzo, M. T., Foster-Hanson, E., Moty, K., Leshin, R. A., Wang, M., Benitez, J., & Ocampo, J. D. (2020). Advancing developmental science via unmoderated remote research with children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 21(4), 477-493.
doi: 10.1080/15248372.2020.1797751 pmid: 32982602 |
[34] |
Roberts, S. O. (2021). Descriptive-to-prescriptive (D2P) reasoning: An early emerging bias to maintain the status quo. European Review of Social Psychology, 33(3), 1-34.
doi: 10.1080/10463283.2021.1954379 URL |
[35] | Roberts, S. O., Gelman, S. A., & Ho, A. K. (2017). So it is, so it shall be: Group regularities license children 's prescriptive judgments. Cognitive Science, 41 (S3), 576-600. |
[36] | Roberts, S. O., Ho, A. K., & Gelman, S. A. (2020). Should individuals think like their group? A descriptive-to-prescriptive tendency toward group-based beliefs. Child Development, 92(2), e201-e220. |
[37] |
Rutchick, A. M., Hamilton, D. L., & Sack, J. D. (2008). Antecedents of entitativity in categorically and dynamically construed groups. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38(6), 905-921.
doi: 10.1002/ejsp.v38:6 URL |
[38] | Shutts, K. (2015). Young children's preferences: Gender, race, and social status. Child Development Perspectives, 9(4), 262-266. |
[39] |
Switzer, J. L., San Juan, V., & Graham, S. A. (2020). Preschoolers use helpful and harmful interactions to predict social category membership. Developmental Psychology, 56(6), 1057-1072.
doi: 10.1037/dev0000925 pmid: 32324014 |
[40] | Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behaviour. In S.Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 7-24). Chicago: Nelson. |
[41] | Turner, J. C. (1981). Towards a cognitive redefinition of the social group. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/Current Psychology of Cognition, 1(2), 93-118. |
[42] | Wang, Y., Qian, M., Nabbijohn, A. N., Wen, F., Fu, G., Zuo, B., & VanderLaan, D. P. (2022). Culture influences the development of children's gender-related peer preferences: Evidence from China and Thailand. Developmental Science, 25(4), e13221. |
[43] | Wen, F. F., & Zuo, B. (2019). Concepts, cues and influential mechanisms of social categorization. Journal of Psychological Science, 42(2), 395-401. |
[44] | Weisgram, E. S., Fulcher, M., & Dinella, L. M. (2014). Pink gives girls permission: Exploring the roles of explicit gender labels and gender-typed colors on preschool children's toy preferences. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 35(5), 401-409. |
[45] |
Weisman, K., Johnson, M. V., & Shutts, K. (2015). Young children's automatic encoding of social categories. Developmental Science, 18(6), 1036-1043.
doi: 10.1111/desc.12269 pmid: 25483012 |
[46] | Wilks, M., Kirby, J., & Nielsen, M. (2019). Developmental changes in young children's willingness to copy the antisocial actions of ingroup members in a minimal group context. Developmental Psychology, 55(4), 709-721. |
[47] |
Xu, Y., Burns, M., Wen, F., Thor, E. D., Zuo, B., Coley, J. D., & Rhodes, M. (2022). How culture shapes social categorization and inductive reasoning: A developmental comparison between the United States and China. Journal of Cognition and Development, 23(5), 644-659.
doi: 10.1080/15248372.2022.2085708 URL |
[48] |
Yang, L. P., Qi, L. D., & Zhang, B. (2022). Concepts and evaluation of saturation in qualitative research. Advances in Psychological Science, 30(3), 511-521.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2022.00511 |
[49] |
Yang, X. L., Liu, L., Li, Q., & Wan, M. N. (2012). The entitativity of social group: Retrospect and prospect. Advances in Psychological Science, 20(8), 1314-1321.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2012.01314 URL |
[50] |
Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols, 1(1), 297-301.
doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.46 pmid: 17406248 |
[51] |
Zuo, B., Wen, F. F., Song, J. J., & Dai, T. T. (2019). The characteristics, dimensions and psychological effect of social categorization. Advances in Psychological Science, 27(1), 141-148.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2019.00141 |
[1] | LIU Yong-Fang,SU Li-Na,WANG Huai-Yong. Women’s Cue Preferences and Information Processing Mode in Mate Choice [J]. , 2011, 43(01): 21-29. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||