[1] |
Bermeitinger C., Wentura D., Koppermann C., Hauser M., Grass B., & Frings C. (2012). The direction of masked auditory category priming correlates with participants’ prime discrimination ability. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 8(3), 210-217. doi: 10.2478/v10053-008-0116-6
|
[2] |
Bittner R., Humphrey E., & Bello J. (2016). Pysox: Leveraging the audio signal processing power of sox in python. In Proceedings of the International Society for Music Information Retrieval Conference Late Breaking and Demo Papers.
|
[3] |
Davis C., & Kim J. (2021). Bilingual lexical representation: We have some words to say. Journal of Second Language Studies, 4(2), 353-374. doi: 10.1075/jsls.21015.dav
|
[4] |
Davis C., Kim J., & Barbaro A. (2010). Masked speech priming: neighborhood size matters. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 127(4), 2110-2113. doi: 10.1121/1.3353116
|
[5] |
Darwin C. J., Turvey M. T., & Crowder R. G. (1972). An auditory analogue of the Sperling partial report procedure: Evidence for brief auditory storage. Cognitive Psychology, 3(2),255-267. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(72)90007-2
|
[6] |
Degner J. (2011). Affective priming with auditory speech stimuli. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26(10), 1710-1735. doi: 10.1080/01690965.2010.532625
|
[7] |
Dehaene S., Jobert A., Naccache L., Ciuciu P., Poline J. B., Le Bihan D., & Cohen L. (2004). Letter binding and invariant recognition of masked words: behavioral and neuroimaging evidence. Psychological Science, 15(5), 307-313. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00674.x
|
[8] |
Dupoux E., de Gardelle V., & Kouider S. (2008). Subliminal speech perception and auditory streaming. Cognition, 109(2), 267-273. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.06.012
|
[9] |
Dupoux E., Kouider S., & Mehler J. (2003). Lexical access without attention? Explorations using dichotic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(1), 172-184. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.29.1.172
|
[10] |
Elgendi M., Kumar P., Barbic S., Howard N., Abbott D., & Cichocki A. (2018). Subliminal priming—state of the art and future perspectives. Behavioral Sciences, 8(6), 54-77. doi: 10.3390/bs8060054
|
[11] |
Ferrand L. (1996). The masked repetition priming effect dissipates when increasing the inter-stimulus interval: Evidence from word naming. Acta Psychologica, 91(1),15-25. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(95)00010-0
|
[12] |
Faul F., Erdfelder E., Lang A. G., & Buchner A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146
|
[13] |
Ferrand L., & Grainger J. (1992). Phonology and orthography in visual word recognition: Evidence from masked non-word priming. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 45(3), 353-372. doi: 10.1080/02724989208250619
|
[14] |
Forbach G. B., Stanners R. F., & Hochhaus L. (1974). Repetition and practice effects in a lexical decision task. Memory & Cognition, 2(2), 337-339. doi: 10.3758/BF03209005
|
[15] |
Forster K. I., & Davis C. (1984). Repetition priming and frequency attenuation in lexical access. Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10(4), 680-698. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.10.4.680
|
[16] |
Geary J., & Ussishkin A. (2019). Morphological priming without semantic relationship in Hebrew spoken word recognition. Proceedings of the Linguistic Society of America, 4(9), 1-10. doi: 10.3765/PLSA.V4I1.4509
|
[17] |
Grainger J., Colé P., & Segui J. (1991). Masked morphological priming in visual word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 30(3),370-384. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(91)90042-I
|
[18] |
Greenwald A. G., Draine S. C., & Abrams R. L. (1996). Three cognitive markers of unconscious semantic activation. Science, 273(5282), 1699-1702. doi: 10.1126/science.273.5282.1699
|
[19] |
Jin G. J., Shang H., Fu L., & Zhang Y. F. (2005). Construction and Deep Processing of Modern Chinese Corpus. Applied Linguistics, 14(2), 111-120. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1003-5397.2005.02.017
|
[20] |
Jiang Y. C., Cai X., & Zhang Q. F. (2020). Theta band (4-8 Hz) oscillations reflect syllables processing in Chinese spoken word production. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 52(10), 1199-1211. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.01199
|
[21] |
Kiefer M. (2002). The N400 is modulated by unconsciously perceived masked words: Further evidence for an automatic spreading activation account of N400 priming effects. Cognitive Brain Research, 13(1), 27-39. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(01)00085-4
|
[22] |
Kopeikina E. A., Choroshich V. V., Aleksandrov A. Y., & Ivanova V. Y. (2015). Effects of unconscious perception of acoustic stimuli on event-related potential parameters. Human Physiology, 41(3), 242-250. doi: 10.1134/S0362119715020085
|
[23] |
Kouider S., & Dehaene S. (2007). Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: a critical review of visual masking. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362(1481), 857-875. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2093
|
[24] |
Kouider S., & Dupoux E. (2005). Subliminal speech priming. Psychological Science, 16(8), 617-625. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01584.x
|
[25] |
Lähteenmäki M., Kauramäki J., Sauter D. A., & Nummenmaa L. (2019). Auditory affective processing requires awareness. Emotion, 19(1), 53-69. doi: 10.1037/emo000038
|
[26] |
Latinus M., & Belin P. (2011). Human voice perception. Current Biology, 21(4), 143-145. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.12.033
|
[27] |
Lupker S. J. (1984). Semantic priming without association: A second look. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23(6), 709-733. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(84)90434-1
|
[28] |
Ma L. J., & Zhang J. J. (2011). The psychological mechanisms of subliminal priming of information processing. Journal of Psychological Science, 34(5), 1040-1045. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.2011.05.020
|
[29] |
Marcel A. J. (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 15(2),197-237. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(83)90009-9
|
[30] |
Pasinski A. C., McAuley J. D., & Snyder J. S. (2016). How modality specific is processing of auditory and visual rhythms?. Psychophysiology, 53(2), 198-208. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12559
|
[31] |
Peirce J. W. (2007). PsychoPy—psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162(1-2), 8-13. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017
|
[32] |
Rastle K., Davis M. H., Marslen-Wilson W. D., & Tyler L. K. (2000). Morphological and semantic effects in visual word recognition: A time-course study. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15(4-5), 507-537. doi: 10.1080/01690960050119689
|
[33] |
Schluter K. T. (2013). Hearing words without structure: subliminal speech priming and the organization of the moroccan arabic lexicon. (PhD), University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ.
|
[34] |
Shelton J. R., & Martin R. C. (1992). How semantic is automatic semantic priming?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(6), 1191-1210. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.18.6.1191
|
[35] |
Shen D., & Forster K. I. (1999). Masked phonological priming in reading Chinese words depends on the task. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14(5-6), 429-459. doi: 10.1080/016909699386149
|
[36] |
Shen M. W., Gao T., & Ding H. J. (2004). A study of Chinese and Arabic numerals' subliminal priming effects. Psychological Science. 27(1), 13-17. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.2004.01.004
|
[37] |
Shui R. D., Ding H. J., & Shen M. W. (2003). Unconscious Mechanisms of Chinese Characters in Subliminal Semantic Activation. Psychological Science, 26(6), 1025-1029. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.2003.06.016
|
[38] |
Song J., & Lv Y. (2015). Influence of top-down modulation on automatic process in masked priming. Advances in Psychological Science, 23(5), 766-773. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2015.00766
|
[39] |
Sperling G. (1960). The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 74(11), 1-29. doi: 10.1037/h0093759
|
[40] |
Sweeny T. D., Grabowecky M., Suzuki S., & Paller K. A. (2009). Long-lasting effects of subliminal affective priming from facial expressions. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(4), 929-938. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2009.07.011
|
[41] |
Ussishkin A., Dawson C. R., Wedel A., & Schluter K. (2015). Auditory masked priming in Maltese spoken word recognition. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30(9), 1096-1115. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2015.1005635
|
[42] |
Verhelst W., & Roelands M. (1993). An overlap-add technique based on waveform similarity (WSOLA) for high quality time-scale modification of speech. In 1993 IEEE International Conference on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing, 2, 554-557. doi: 10.1109/ICASSP.1993.319366
|
[43] |
Wang P., & Lu C. X. (2005). The Semantic Categories in Subliminal Priming. Psychological Science, 28(6), 1344-1346. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.2005.06.015
|
[44] |
Wang J., Tian J., Han W., Liversedge S. P., & Paterson K. B. (2014). Inhibitory stroke neighbour priming in character recognition and reading in Chinese. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(11), 2149-2171. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.909507
|
[45] |
Wang Y., & Zhang J. J. (2013). The Interaction of Semantic and Syntactic Information in Chinese-English Bilinguals’ Early Recognition of Polysemous Words. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 45(3), 298-309. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2013.00298
|
[46] |
Wang Y., & Zhang J. J. (2014). The Masked Translation Effect with Homograph and Non-homograph in Non-proficient Chinese- Japanese Bilinguals. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 46(6), 765-776. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2014.00765
|
[47] |
Weekes B. S., Chen M. J., & Lin Y. B. (1998). Differential effects of phonological priming on Chinese character recognition. Reading and Writing, 10(3), 201-221. doi: 10.1023/A:1008087715413
|
[48] |
Wu S. S., Tan J. F., Wang L. J., & Chen A. H. (2013). The influencing factors of subliminal semantic priming effects. Advances in Psychological Science, 21(4), 626-636. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.00626
|
[49] |
Wu C., Zhang J., & Yuan Z. (2020). Affective picture processing is modulated by emotion word type in masked priming paradigm: An event-related potential study. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 32(3), 287-297. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2020.1745816
|
[50] |
Zhang J., Wu C., Zhou T., & Meng Y. (2019). Cognate facilitation priming effect is modulated by writing system: Evidence from Chinese-English bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 23(2), 553-566. doi: 10.1177/1367006917749062
|
[51] |
Zhang Q., & Damian M. F. (2019). Syllables constitute proximate units for Mandarin speakers: Electrophysiological evidence from a masked priming task. Psychophysiology, 56(4), e13317. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13317
|
[52] |
Zhao S. M., Wu Y., Li T. H., & Guo Q. T. (2017). Morpho-semantic processing in Chinese word recognition: An ERP study. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 49(3), 296-306. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2017.00296
|
[53] |
Zhou X., & Marslen-Wilson W. (1994). Words, morphemes and syllables in the Chinese mental lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 9(3), 393-422. doi: 10.1080/01690969408402125
|
[54] |
Zhou X., & Marslen-Wilson W. (1995). Morphological structure in the Chinese mental lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 10(6), 545-600. doi: 10.1080/01690969508407114
|
[55] |
Zhou X., Marslen-Wilson W., Taft M., & Shu H. (1999). Morphology, orthography, and phonology reading Chinese compound words. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14(5-6), 525-565. doi: 10.1080/016909699386185
|
[56] |
Zou L. J., & Shu H. (2013). The cognitive and neural mechanisms of morphological processing during word recognition. Advances in Psychological Science, 21(9), 1570-1577. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2013.01570
|