Acta Psychologica Sinica ›› 2022, Vol. 54 ›› Issue (7): 748-760.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.00748
• Reports of Empirical Studies • Previous Articles Next Articles
YAN Chi1, GAO Yunfei1, HU Saisai1, SONG Fangxing2,3, WANG Yonghui1(), ZHAO Jingjing1()
Received:
2021-10-03
Published:
2022-07-25
Online:
2022-05-19
Contact:
WANG Yonghui,ZHAO Jingjing
E-mail:wyonghui@snnu.edu.cn;zhaojingjing_31@126.com
Supported by:
YAN Chi, GAO Yunfei, HU Saisai, SONG Fangxing, WANG Yonghui, ZHAO Jingjing. (2022). The impact and mechanism of gaze cues on object-based attention. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 54(7), 748-760.
Add to citation manager EndNote|Ris|BibTeX
URL: https://journal.psych.ac.cn/acps/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.00748
SOA | Direct gaze | Averted gaze | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Valid | Invalid same-object | Invalid different-object | Valid | Invalid same-object | Invalid different-object | |
100 ms | 358 ± 43 | 353 ± 47 | 360 ± 43 | 359 ± 41 | 351 ± 46 | 361 ± 48 |
300 ms | 321 ± 44 | 320 ± 45 | 333 ± 45 | 326 ± 41 | 327 ± 47 | 331 ± 46 |
600 ms | 350 ± 47 | 349 ± 47 | 350 ± 44 | 350 ± 47 | 346 ± 43 | 352 ± 47 |
Table 1 The mean response time (M ± SD) for SOA, cue position and cue validity in Experiment 1
SOA | Direct gaze | Averted gaze | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Valid | Invalid same-object | Invalid different-object | Valid | Invalid same-object | Invalid different-object | |
100 ms | 358 ± 43 | 353 ± 47 | 360 ± 43 | 359 ± 41 | 351 ± 46 | 361 ± 48 |
300 ms | 321 ± 44 | 320 ± 45 | 333 ± 45 | 326 ± 41 | 327 ± 47 | 331 ± 46 |
600 ms | 350 ± 47 | 349 ± 47 | 350 ± 44 | 350 ± 47 | 346 ± 43 | 352 ± 47 |
SOA | Direct gaze | Averted gaze | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Valid | Invalid same-object | Invalid different-object | Valid | Invalid same-object | Invalid different-object | |
100 ms | 382 ± 47 | 372 ± 42 | 388 ± 49 | 375 ± 44 | 371 ± 47 | 378 ± 46 |
300 ms | 339 ± 46 | 340 ± 42 | 354 ± 54 | 335 ± 41 | 341 ± 48 | 347 ± 49 |
600 ms | 368 ± 46 | 368 ± 47 | 369 ± 47 | 368 ± 46 | 368 ± 47 | 375 ± 50 |
Table 2 The mean response time (M ± SD) for SOA, cue position and cue validity in Experiment 2
SOA | Direct gaze | Averted gaze | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Valid | Invalid same-object | Invalid different-object | Valid | Invalid same-object | Invalid different-object | |
100 ms | 382 ± 47 | 372 ± 42 | 388 ± 49 | 375 ± 44 | 371 ± 47 | 378 ± 46 |
300 ms | 339 ± 46 | 340 ± 42 | 354 ± 54 | 335 ± 41 | 341 ± 48 | 347 ± 49 |
600 ms | 368 ± 46 | 368 ± 47 | 369 ± 47 | 368 ± 46 | 368 ± 47 | 375 ± 50 |
SOA | Direct gaze | Averted gaze | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Valid | Invalid same-object | Invalid different-object | Valid | Invalid same-object | Invalid different-object | |
100 ms | 389 ± 46 | 388 ± 54 | 400 ± 51 | 387 ± 44 | 388 ± 53 | 395 ± 52 |
300 ms | 348 ± 38 | 353 ± 51 | 367 ± 51 | 349 ± 46 | 363 ± 52 | 361 ± 55 |
600 ms | 381 ± 41 | 382 ± 42 | 384 ± 42 | 378 ± 39 | 380 ± 39 | 386 ± 39 |
Table 3 The mean response time (M ± SD) for SOA, cue position and cue validity in Experiment 3
SOA | Direct gaze | Averted gaze | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Valid | Invalid same-object | Invalid different-object | Valid | Invalid same-object | Invalid different-object | |
100 ms | 389 ± 46 | 388 ± 54 | 400 ± 51 | 387 ± 44 | 388 ± 53 | 395 ± 52 |
300 ms | 348 ± 38 | 353 ± 51 | 367 ± 51 | 349 ± 46 | 363 ± 52 | 361 ± 55 |
600 ms | 381 ± 41 | 382 ± 42 | 384 ± 42 | 378 ± 39 | 380 ± 39 | 386 ± 39 |
[1] |
Baldauf, D., & Desimone, R. (2014). Neural mechanisms of object- based attention. Science, 344(6182), 424-427.
doi: 10.1126/science.1247003 pmid: 24763592 |
[2] |
Bayliss, A. P., di Pellegrino, G., & Tipper, S. P. (2005). Sex differences in eye gaze and symbolic cueing of attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 58(4), 631-650.
doi: 10.1080/02724980443000124 URL |
[3] |
Bayliss, A. P., Frischen, A., Fenske, M. J., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). Affective evaluations of objects are influenced by observed gaze direction and emotional expression. Cognition, 104(3), 644-653.
pmid: 16950239 |
[4] |
Bayliss, A. P., Murphy, E., Naughtin, C. K., Kritikos, A., Schilbach, L., & Becker, S. I. (2013). “Gaze Leading”: Initiating simulated joint attention influences eye movements and choice behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(1), 76-92.
doi: 10.1037/a0029286 URL |
[5] |
Bayliss, A. P., Paul, M. A., Cannon, P. R., & Tipper, S. P. (2006). Gaze cuing and affective judgments of objects: I like what you look at. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(6), 1061-1066.
doi: 10.3758/BF03213926 URL |
[6] |
Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P. (2006). Gaze cues evoke both spatial and object-centered shifts of attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 68(2), 310-318.
doi: 10.3758/BF03193678 URL |
[7] |
Blais, C., Jack, R. E., Scheepers, C., Fiset, D., & Caldara, R. (2008). Culture shapes how we look at faces. PloS One, 3(8), e3022.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003022 URL |
[8] |
Bockler, A., van der Wel, R. P. R. D., & Welsh, T. N. (2014). Catching eyes: Effects of social and nonsocial cues on attention capture. Psychological Science, 25(3), 720-727.
doi: 10.1177/0956797613516147 URL |
[9] |
Bristow, D., Frith, C., & Rees, G.. (2005). Two distinct neural effects of blinking on human visual processing. Neuroimage, 27(1), 136-145.
pmid: 15893941 |
[10] |
Brown, J. M., & Denney, H. I. (2007). Shifting attention into and out of objects: Evaluating the processes underlying the object advantage. Perception & Psychophysics, 69(4), 606-618.
doi: 10.3758/BF03193918 URL |
[11] |
Chakravarthi, R., & Vanrullen, R.. (2012). Conscious updating is a rhythmic process. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(26), 10599-10604.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1121622109 URL |
[12] |
Chen, A. R., Dong, B., Fang, Y., Yu, C. Y., & Zhang, M. (2014). The role of cue type in the subliminal gaze-cueing effect. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 46(9), 1281-1288.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2014.01281 URL |
[13] |
Chen, Z. (2012). Object-based attention: A tutorial review. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 74(5), 784-802.
doi: 10.3758/s13414-012-0322-z URL |
[14] |
Chen, Z., Cave, K. R., Basu, D., Suresh, S., & Wiltshire, J. (2020). A region complexity effect masquerading as object-based attention. Journal of Vision, 20(7), 24.
doi: 10.1167/jov.20.7.24 pmid: 32692828 |
[15] |
Chita-Tegmark, M. (2016). Attention allocation in ASD: A review and meta-analysis of eye-tracking studies. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 3(3), 209-223.
doi: 10.1007/s40489-016-0077-x URL |
[16] |
Chou, W. L., & Yeh, S. L. (2018). Dissociating location-based and object-based cue validity effects in object-based attention. Vision Research, 143, 34-41.
doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2017.11.008 URL |
[17] |
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155- 159.
doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.112.1.155 pmid: 19565683 |
[18] |
Conci, M., & Muller, H. J. (2009). The "beam of darkness": Spreading of the attentional blink within and between objects. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 71(8), 1725-1738.
doi: 10.3758/APP.71.8.1725 URL |
[19] | Crouzet, S. M., Kirchner, H., & Thorpe, S. J. (2010). Fast saccades toward faces: Face detection in just 100 ms. Journal of vision, 10(4), 16. |
[20] |
Dalmaso, M., Castelli, L., & Galfano, G. (2017). Attention holding elicited by direct-gaze faces is reflected in saccadic peak velocity. Experimental Brain Research, 235(11), 3319-3332.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-017-5059-4 pmid: 28812119 |
[21] |
Desimone, R., & Duncan, J.. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18(1), 193-222.
doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205 URL |
[22] |
Drummond, L., & Shomstein, S. (2010). Object-based attention: Shifting or uncertainty? Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 72(7), 1743-1755.
doi: 10.3758/APP.72.7.1743 URL |
[23] |
Egly, R., Driver, J., & Rafal, R. D. (1994). Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: Evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123(2), 161-177.
doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.123.2.161 URL |
[24] |
Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M. H.. (2002). Eye contact detection in humans from birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99(14), 9602- 9605.
pmid: 12082186 |
[25] |
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149-1160.
doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149 URL |
[26] |
Fischer, M. H. (1999). An investigation of attention allocation during sequential eye movement tasks. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 52(3), 649-677.
doi: 10.1080/713755838 URL |
[27] |
Friesen, C. K., & Kingstone, A. (1998). The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 5(3), 490-495.
doi: 10.3758/BF03208827 URL |
[28] |
Friesen, C. K., Ristic, J., & Kingstone, A. (2004). Attentional effects of counterpredictive gaze and arrow cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30(2), 319-329.
doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.30.2.319 URL |
[29] |
Frischen, A., Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). Gaze cueing of attention: Visual attention, social cognition, and individual differences. Psychological Bulletin, 133(4), 694-724.
doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.4.694 URL |
[30] |
Green, J. J., Gamble, M. L., & Woldorff, M. G. (2013). Resolving conflicting views: Gaze and arrow cues do not trigger rapid reflexive shifts of attention. Visual Cognition, 21(1), 61-71.
doi: 10.1080/13506285.2013.775209 URL |
[31] |
Hietanen, J. K., Myllyneva, A., Helminen, T. M., & Lyyra, P. (2016). The effects of genuine eye contact on visuospatial and selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(9), 1102-1106.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000199 URL |
[32] |
Hu, S., Liu, D., Song, F., Wang, Y., & Zhao, J. (2020). The influence of object similarity on real object-based attention: The disassociation of perceptual and semantic similarity. Acta Psychologica, 205, 103046.
doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103046 URL |
[33] |
Hu, Z. H., Liu, Q., Zhao, G., & Li, H. (2013). The measurement of detection superiority of direct gaze affected by stimuli component information. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 45(11), 1217-1227.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2013.01217 URL |
[34] |
Hudson, M., Nicholson, T., Simpson, W. A., Ellis, R., & Bach, P. (2016). One step ahead: The perceived kinematics of others' actions are biased toward expected goals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(1), 1-7.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000126 URL |
[35] |
Huo, P. H., Feng, C.Z., & Chen, T. J. (2021). The influence of gazer and observer factors on gaze perception. Advances in Psychological Science, 29(2), 238-251.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2021.00238 URL |
[36] |
Itier, R. J., Alain, C., Sedore, K., & McIntosh, A. R. (2007). Early face processing specificity: It's in the eyes! Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(11), 1815-1826.
doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.11.1815 URL |
[37] |
Jeurissen, D., Self, M. W., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2016). Serial grouping of 2D-image regions with object-based attention in humans. Elife, 5, e14320.
doi: 10.7554/eLife.14320 URL |
[38] | Ji, H. Y., Wang, L, & Jiang, Y. (2017). The specific cognitive and neural mechanisms of social attention. Progress in Biochemistry and Biophysics, 44(11), 959-971 |
[39] |
Kawai, N. (2011). Attentional shift by eye gaze requires joint attention: Eye gaze cues are unique to shift attention. Japanese Psychological Research, 53(3), 292-301.
doi: 10.1111/j.1468-5884.2011.00470.x URL |
[40] |
Kleinke, C. L. (1986). Gaze and eye contact: A research review. Psychological Bulletin, 100(1), 78-100.
pmid: 3526377 |
[41] |
Korisky, U., & Mudrik, L. (2021). Dimensions of perception: 3D real- life objects are more readily detected than their 2D images. Psychological Science, 32(10), 1636-1648.
doi: 10.1177/09567976211010718 URL |
[42] |
Landau, A., & Fries, P. (2012). Attention samples stimuli rhythmically. Current Biology, 22(11), 1000-1004.
doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.054 URL |
[43] |
Langton, S. R., Law, A. S., Burton, A. M., & Schweinberger, S. R. (2008). Attention capture by faces. Cognition, 107(1), 330-342.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.012 URL |
[44] |
Lee, J., & Shomstein, S. (2013). The differential effects of reward on space-and object-based attentional allocation. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(26), 10625-10633.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5575-12.2013 URL |
[45] |
Lou, H., Lorist, M. M., & Pilz, K. S. (2021). Individual differences in the temporal dynamics of object-based attention. Journal of Vision, 21(9), 1953.
doi: 10.1167/jov.21.9.1953 URL |
[46] |
Malcolm, G. L., & Shomstein, S. (2015). Object-based attention in real- world scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(2), 257-263.
doi: 10.1037/xge0000060 URL |
[47] |
Murphy, J., & Cook, R. (2017). Revealing the mechanisms of human face perception using dynamic apertures. Cognition, 169, 25-35.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.08.001 URL |
[48] | Otsuka, Y., & Clifford, C. W. G. (2018). Influence of head orientation on perceived gaze direction and eye-region information. Journal of Vision, 18(12), 15. |
[49] |
Peng, X., Chang, R, S., Li, Q., Wang, A, J., & Tang, X,Y. (2019). Visually induced inhibition of return affects the audiovisual integration under different SOA conditions. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 51(7), 759-771.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.00759 URL |
[50] |
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1), 3-25.
doi: 10.1080/00335558008248231 URL |
[51] |
Ramamoorthy, N., Plaisted-Grant, K., & Davis, G. (2019). Fractionating the stare-in-the-crowd effect: Two distinct, obligatory biases in search for gaze. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45(8), 1015-1030.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000655 URL |
[52] |
Ricciardelli, P., Betta, E., Pruner, S., & Turatto, M. (2009). Is there a direct link between gaze perception and joint attention behaviours? Effects of gaze contrast polarity on oculomotor behaviour. Experimental Brain Research, 194(3), 347-357.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-1706-8 pmid: 19183970 |
[53] | Richard, A. M., Lee, H., & Vecera, S. P. (2008). Attentional spreading in object-based attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 34(4), 842-853. |
[54] | Ristic, J., & Kingstone, A. (2005). Taking control of reflexive social attention. Cognition, 94(3), B55-B65. |
[55] |
Senju, A., & Hasegawa, T. (2005). Direct gaze captures visuospatial attention. Visual Cognition, 12(1), 127-144.
doi: 10.1080/13506280444000157 URL |
[56] |
Shomstein, S. (2012). Object-based attention: Strategy versus automaticity. WIREs Cognitive Science, 3(2), 163-169.
doi: 10.1002/wcs.1162 URL |
[57] |
Shomstein, S., & Behrmann, M. (2008). Object-based attention: Strength of object representation and attentional guidance. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(1), 132-144.
doi: 10.3758/PP.70.1.132 URL |
[58] |
Shomstein, S., & Johnson, J. (2013). Shaping attention with reward: Effects of reward on space and object-based selection. Psychological Science, 24(12), 2369-2378.
doi: 10.1177/0956797613490743 pmid: 24121412 |
[59] |
Shomstein, S., & Yantis, S. (2004). Configural and contextual prioritization in object-based attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(2), 247-253.
doi: 10.3758/BF03196566 URL |
[60] |
Simpson, E. A., Husband, H. L., Yee, K., Fullerton, A., & Jakobsen, K. V. (2014). Visual search efficiency is greater for human faces compared to animal faces. Experimental Psychology, 61(6), 439-456.
doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000263 URL |
[61] | Song, F., Zhou, S., Gao, Y., Hu, S., Kong, F., & Zhao, J. (2020). Different temporal dynamics of object-based attentional allocation for reward and non-reward objects. Journal of Vision, 20(9), 17. |
[62] |
Song, F., Zhou, S., Gao, Y., Hu, S., Zhang, T., Kong, F., & Zhao, J. (2021). Are you looking at me? Impact of eye contact on object-based attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 47(6), 765-773.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000913 URL |
[63] |
Stephenson, L. J., Edwards, S. G., Howard, E. E., & Bayliss, A. P. (2017). Eyes that bind us: Gaze leading induces an implicit sense of agency. Cognition, 172, 124-133.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.12.011 URL |
[64] |
Uono, S., & Hietanen, J. K. (2015). Eye contact perception in the west and east: A cross-cultural study. PLoS ONE, 10(2), e0118094.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0118094 URL |
[65] |
Ushitani, T., Imura, T., & Tomonaga, M. (2010). Object-based attention in chimpanzees. Vision Research, 50(6), 577-584.
doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.01.003 URL |
[66] |
VanRullen, R. (2013). Visual attention: A rhythmic process? Current Biology, 23(24), R1110-R1112.
doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.11.006 URL |
[67] | Yeshurun, Y., & Rashal, E. (2017). The typical advantage of object- based attention reflects reduced spatial cost. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 43(1), 69-77. |
[68] | Zhang, Z. J., Tang, Y., Zhang, X. K., & Zhang, Z. H. (2015). The influence of dynamic and static gaze-cueing upon attention shift: Role of movement cues. Chinese Journal of Applied Psychology, 21(3), 195-202. |
[69] |
Zhao, J., Song, F., Zhou, S., Hu, S., Liu, D., Wang, Y., & Kong, F. (2020). The impact of monetary stimuli on object-based attention. British Journal of Psychology, 111(3), 460-472.
doi: 10.1111/bjop.12418 URL |
[70] |
Zhou, L. F., Wang, K., He, L., & Meng, M. (2021). Twofold advantages of face processing with or without visual awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 47(6), 784-794.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000915 URL |
[1] | JIN Xinyi, ZHOU Bingxin, MENG Fei. Level 2 visual perspective-taking at age 3 and the corresponding effect of cooperation [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(9): 1028-1039. |
[2] | JIN Yi; ZHANG Dandan; LIU Yunzhe; LUO Yuejia. An ERP Study of Disgust Processing [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2014, 46(11): 1682-1690. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||