Acta Psychologica Sinica ›› 2025, Vol. 57 ›› Issue (4): 544-558.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2025.0544
• Special Issue on Prosocial Behavior (Part Ⅱ) • Previous Articles Next Articles
HUA Shan1,2, JIANG Xintong1,2, GAO Yangzhenyu4, MU Yan1,2(), DU Yi1,2,3(
)
Published:
2025-04-25
Online:
2025-02-11
Contact:
MU Yan,DU Yi
E-mail:muy@psych.ac.cn;duyi@psych.ac.cn
HUA Shan, JIANG Xintong, GAO Yangzhenyu, MU Yan, DU Yi. (2025). The impacts of music training and music sophistication on empathy. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 57(4), 544-558.
Figure 1. Differences in trait empathy and musical sophistication between musicians and non-musicians. A. Comparison of trait empathy dimensions between musicians and non-musicians. B. Comparison of overall musical sophistication (GSI) between musicians and non-musicians. Note: **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.
Variables | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Musical Sophistication | — | ||||||||||
2. Perspective-Taking | 0.30*** | — | |||||||||
3. Fantasy | 0.39*** | 0.39*** | — | ||||||||
4. Empathic Concern | 0.18*** | 0.30*** | 0.58*** | — | |||||||
5. Personal Distress | ?0.22** | ?0.14* | ?0.15* | ?0.10 | — | ||||||
6. Openness | 0.45*** | 0.34*** | 0.36*** | 0.19** | ?0.22*** | — | |||||
7. SSS | 0.56*** | 0.27*** | 0.23*** | 0.16* | ?0.17** | 0.39*** | — | ||||
8. STAI-S | ?0.29*** | ?0.37*** | ?0.32*** | ?0.24*** | 0.43*** | ?0.35*** | ?0.29*** | — | |||
9. STAI-T | ?0.32*** | ?0.36*** | ?0.37*** | ?0.26*** | 0.56*** | ?0.38*** | ?0.33*** | 0.85*** | — | ||
10. TAS-20 | ?0.25*** | ?0.37*** | ?0.39*** | ?0.33*** | 0.63*** | ?0.30*** | ?0.20** | 0.47*** | 0.57*** | — | |
11. BDI | ?0.22*** | ?0.24*** | ?0.21** | ?0.21*** | 0.51*** | ?0.31*** | ?0.29*** | 0.55*** | 0.66*** | 0.54*** | — |
M | 77.08 | 14.75 | 16.73 | 16.34 | 9.37 | 9.92 | 44.76 | 34.67 | 37.68 | 48.38 | 4.09 |
SD | 19.59 | 3.25 | 4.42 | 4.28 | 4.91 | 2.64 | 8.35 | 10.66 | 10.29 | 12.67 | 5.34 |
Table 1 Descriptive statistics of research variables and control variables, and Spearman partial correlation coefficients after controlling for gender (n = 251)
Variables | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Musical Sophistication | — | ||||||||||
2. Perspective-Taking | 0.30*** | — | |||||||||
3. Fantasy | 0.39*** | 0.39*** | — | ||||||||
4. Empathic Concern | 0.18*** | 0.30*** | 0.58*** | — | |||||||
5. Personal Distress | ?0.22** | ?0.14* | ?0.15* | ?0.10 | — | ||||||
6. Openness | 0.45*** | 0.34*** | 0.36*** | 0.19** | ?0.22*** | — | |||||
7. SSS | 0.56*** | 0.27*** | 0.23*** | 0.16* | ?0.17** | 0.39*** | — | ||||
8. STAI-S | ?0.29*** | ?0.37*** | ?0.32*** | ?0.24*** | 0.43*** | ?0.35*** | ?0.29*** | — | |||
9. STAI-T | ?0.32*** | ?0.36*** | ?0.37*** | ?0.26*** | 0.56*** | ?0.38*** | ?0.33*** | 0.85*** | — | ||
10. TAS-20 | ?0.25*** | ?0.37*** | ?0.39*** | ?0.33*** | 0.63*** | ?0.30*** | ?0.20** | 0.47*** | 0.57*** | — | |
11. BDI | ?0.22*** | ?0.24*** | ?0.21** | ?0.21*** | 0.51*** | ?0.31*** | ?0.29*** | 0.55*** | 0.66*** | 0.54*** | — |
M | 77.08 | 14.75 | 16.73 | 16.34 | 9.37 | 9.92 | 44.76 | 34.67 | 37.68 | 48.38 | 4.09 |
SD | 19.59 | 3.25 | 4.42 | 4.28 | 4.91 | 2.64 | 8.35 | 10.66 | 10.29 | 12.67 | 5.34 |
Figure 2. Path analysis of the effects of musical training and musical sophistication on trait empathy. Note: Solid lines represent significant direct paths, bold lines represent significant mediation paths, while dashed lines indicate non-significant direct or mediation paths. ***p < 0.001.
Variables | β | SE | [95% CI of β] | t | df | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Musical Training (musician?non-musician) | 0.16 | 0.07 | [ 0.01, 0.30] | 2.08 | 113 | 0.040* |
Musical Sophistication (after controlled) | 0.11 | 0.07 | [?0.04, 0.25] | 1.39 | 113 | 0.167 |
Stimulus Condition (PNE?PE) | ?0.30 | 0.05 | [?0.40, ?0.20] | ?6.01 | 117 | < 0.001*** |
Musical Training (musician?non-musician)×Stimulus Condition (PNE?PE) | ?0.04 | 0.05 | [?0.14, 0.06] | ?0.87 | 117 | 0.384 |
Stimulus Condition (PNE?PE) × Musical Sophistication (after controlled) | ?0.07 | 0.05 | [?0.17, 0.03] | ?1.33 | 117 | 0.185 |
Gender (female?male) | 0.07 | 0.07 | [?0.08, 0.22] | 0.94 | 113 | 0.347 |
TAS20 | 0.05 | 0.09 | [?0.13, 0.22] | 0.55 | 113 | 0.581 |
STAI_T | ?0.11 | 0.11 | [?0.32, 0.11] | ?0.98 | 113 | 0.328 |
BDI | 0.04 | 0.10 | [?0.15, 0.23] | 0.43 | 113 | 0.668 |
Table 2 Fixed effects results of the linear mixed-effects model on the impact of musical training, musical sophistication, and stimulus condition on the degree of pain empathy
Variables | β | SE | [95% CI of β] | t | df | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Musical Training (musician?non-musician) | 0.16 | 0.07 | [ 0.01, 0.30] | 2.08 | 113 | 0.040* |
Musical Sophistication (after controlled) | 0.11 | 0.07 | [?0.04, 0.25] | 1.39 | 113 | 0.167 |
Stimulus Condition (PNE?PE) | ?0.30 | 0.05 | [?0.40, ?0.20] | ?6.01 | 117 | < 0.001*** |
Musical Training (musician?non-musician)×Stimulus Condition (PNE?PE) | ?0.04 | 0.05 | [?0.14, 0.06] | ?0.87 | 117 | 0.384 |
Stimulus Condition (PNE?PE) × Musical Sophistication (after controlled) | ?0.07 | 0.05 | [?0.17, 0.03] | ?1.33 | 117 | 0.185 |
Gender (female?male) | 0.07 | 0.07 | [?0.08, 0.22] | 0.94 | 113 | 0.347 |
TAS20 | 0.05 | 0.09 | [?0.13, 0.22] | 0.55 | 113 | 0.581 |
STAI_T | ?0.11 | 0.11 | [?0.32, 0.11] | ?0.98 | 113 | 0.328 |
BDI | 0.04 | 0.10 | [?0.15, 0.23] | 0.43 | 113 | 0.668 |
Figure 4. The impact of musical training and stimulus condition on pain ratings and the degree of pain empathy. A. Mean pain ratings for musicians and non-musicians from different evaluation perspectives. B. Mean pain ratings under different stimulus conditions and perspectives. C. Degree of pain empathy between musicians and non-musicians. D. Degree of pain empathy under different stimulus conditions. Note: Error bars represent M ± 1 SE. *p < 0.05; ***p < 0.001.
Figure 5. Path analysis diagram of musical training, musical sophistication, trait empathy, and pain empathy. Note: Solid lines represent significant direct paths, bold lines represent significant mediation paths, while dashed lines indicate non-significant direct or mediation paths. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.
[1] |
Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D., & Taylor, G. J. (1994). The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale—I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38(1), 23-32.
doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90005-1 pmid: 8126686 |
[2] | Bailey, J., & Penhune, V. (2013). The relationship between the age of onset of musical training and rhythm synchronization performance: Validation of sensitive period effects. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7, Article 227. |
[3] | Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1-48. |
[4] |
Batson, C. D., Eklund, J. H., Chermok, V. L., Hoyt, J. L., & Ortiz, B. G. (2007). An additional antecedent of empathic concern: Valuing the welfare of the person in need. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(1), 65-74.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.1.65 pmid: 17605589 |
[5] | Burton, L., & Nkwo, C. (2022). Gender differences in the relationship between state and trait anxiety and empathy. Current Psychology, 41(12), 8368-8373. |
[6] |
Chen. W. Y., Lu. J. M., Liu. L. Q., & Lin.W. Y. (2014). Gender differences of empathy. Advances in Psychological Science, 22(9), 1423-1434.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.01423 |
[7] | Cheng. G., Chen. Y. H., Guan. Y. S., & Zhang. D. J. (2015). On composition of college students’ subjective social status indexes and their characteristics. Journal of Southwest University (Natural Science Edition), 37(6), 156-162. |
[8] | Collet, L., & Cottraux, J. (1986). The shortened Beck Depression Inventory (13 items). Study of the concurrent validity with the Hamilton scale and Widlöcher’s retardation scale. L’Encéphale, 12(2), 77-79. |
[9] | Cross, I., Laurence, F., & Rabinowitch, T.-C. (2012). Empathy and creativity in group musical practices: Towards a concept of empathic creativity. In G. E. McPherson & G. F. Welch (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of music education (Vol. 2, pp. 337-353). Oxford University Press. |
[10] | Cuff, B. M. P., Brown, S. J., Taylor, L., & Howat, D. J. (2016). Empathy: A review of the concept. Emotion Review, 8(2), 144-153. |
[11] | Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 44(1), 113-126. |
[12] |
Dziobek, I., Rogers, K., Fleck, S., Bahnemann, M., Heekeren, H. R., Wolf, O. T., & Convit, A. (2008). Dissociation of cognitive and emotional empathy in adults with Asperger syndrome using the Multifaceted Empathy Test (MET). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(3), 464-473.
doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0486-x pmid: 17990089 |
[13] | Eerola, T., Vuoskoski, J. K., & Kautiainen, H. (2016). Being moved by unfamiliar sad music is associated with high empathy. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, Article 1176. |
[14] |
Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P. A. (1987). The relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 101(1), 91-119.
pmid: 3562705 |
[15] | Epperson, G. (1967). The musical symbol: A study of the philosophic theory of music. Wiley-Blackwell. |
[16] | Fan, Y., Duncan, N. W., de Greck, M., & Northoff, G. (2011). Is there a core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(3), 903-911. |
[17] |
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 1149-1160.
doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149 pmid: 19897823 |
[18] | Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. B. (2003). A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37(6), 504-528. |
[19] |
Green, A. D., Tripp, D. A., Sullivan, M. J. L., & Davidson, M. (2009). The relationship between empathy and estimates of observed pain. Pain Medicine, 10, 381-392.
doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4637.2009.00563.x pmid: 19254339 |
[20] | Greenberg, D. M., Müllensiefen, D., Lamb, M. E., & Rentfrow, P. J. (2015). Personality predicts musical sophistication. Journal of Research in Personality, 58, 154-158. |
[21] | Greenberg, D. M., Rentfrow, P. J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2015). Can music increase empathy? Interpreting musical experience through the Empathizing-Systemizing (E-S) Theory: Implications for Autism. Empirical Musicology Review, 10(1-2), 80-95. |
[22] | Han, S., Fan, Y., Xu, X., Qin, J., Wu, B., Wang, X.,... Mao, L. (2009). Empathic neural responses to others’ pain are modulated by emotional contexts. Human Brain Mapping, 30(10), 3227-3237. |
[23] | Hekmat, H., Khajavi, F., & Mehryar, A. H. (1975). Some personality correlates of empathy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43(1), 89. |
[24] | Huang, H. Q., & Su, Y. J. (2010). Interaction between the cognitive and emotional components of empathy. Journal of Southwest University (Social Sciences Edition), 36(6), 13-19. |
[25] |
Jackendoff, R., & Lerdahl, F. (2006). The capacity for music: What is it, and what’s special about it? Cognition, 100(1), 33-72.
pmid: 16384553 |
[26] | Kalliopuska, M., & Ruókonen, I. (1986). Effects of music education on development of holistic empathy. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 62(1), 187-191. |
[27] |
Kalliopuska, M., & Ruókonen, I. (1993). A study with a follow-up of the effects of music education on holistic development of empathy. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 76(1), 131-137.
pmid: 8451117 |
[28] | Kalliopuska, M., & Tiitinen, U. (1991). Influence of two developmental programmes on the empathy and prosociability of preschool children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 72(1), 323-328. |
[29] | Kawakami, A., & Katahira, K. (2015). Influence of trait empathy on the emotion evoked by sad music and on the preference for it. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 1541. |
[30] | Klarlund, M., Brattico, E., Pearce, M., Wu, Y., Vuust, P., Overgaard, M., & Du, Y. (2023). Worlds apart? Testing the cultural distance hypothesis in music perception of Chinese and Western listeners. Cognition, 235, Article 105405. |
[31] | Koivisto, M., Virkkala, M., Puustinen, M., & Aarnio, J. (2023). Open and empathic personalities see two things at the same time: The relationship of big-five personality traits and cognitive empathy with mixed percepts during binocular rivalry. Current Psychology, 42(11), 9552-9562. |
[32] | Kraus, N., & Chandrasekaran, B. (2010). Music training for the development of auditory skills. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(8), Article 8. |
[33] | Li. J., D. (2013). Psychometric properties of Ten-Item Personality Inventory in China. China Journal of Health Psychology, 21(11), 1688-1692. |
[34] | Lin, H. R., Kopiez, R., Müllensiefen, D., & Wolf, A. (2021). The Chinese version of the Gold-MSI: Adaptation and validation of an inventory for the measurement of musical sophistication in a Taiwanese sample. Musicae Scientiae, 25(2), 226-251. |
[35] | Meng. J., Chen. Y. G., & Huang. X. T. (2010). Influencing factors and the mechanism of empathy for pain. Advances in Psychological Science, 18(3), 432-440. |
[36] | Müllensiefen, D., Gingras, B., Musil, J., & Stewart, L. (2014). The musicality of non-musicians: An index for assessing musical sophistication in the general population. PloS One, 9(2), Article e89642. |
[37] | Olszewska, A. M., Gaca, M., Herman, A. M., Jednoróg, K., & Marchewka, A. (2021). How musical training shapes the adult brain: Predispositions and neuroplasticity. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 15, Article 630829. |
[38] | Parsons, C. E., Young, K. S., Jegindø, E.-M. E., Vuust, P., Stein, A., & Kringelbach, M. L. (2014). Music training and empathy positively impact adults’ sensitivity to infant distress. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 1440. |
[39] | Perlovsky, L. (2012). Cognitive function, origin, and evolution of musical emotions. Musicae Scientiae, 16(2), 185-199. |
[40] | Picerni, E., Laricchiuta, D., Piras, F., Vecchio, D., Petrosini, L., Cutuli, D., & Spalletta, G. (2021). Macro- and micro-structural cerebellar and cortical characteristics of cognitive empathy towards fictional characters in healthy individuals. Scientific Reports, 11(1), Article 8804. |
[41] |
Preston, S. D., & Waal, F. B. M. de. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25(1), 1-20.
doi: 10.1017/s0140525x02000018 pmid: 12625087 |
[42] | Rabinowitch, T.-C., Cross, I., & Burnard, P. (2013). Long-term musical group interaction has a positive influence on empathy in children. Psychology of Music, 41(4), 484-498. |
[43] | Rabinowitch, T.-C., & Knafo-Noam, A. (2015). Synchronous rhythmic interaction enhances children’s perceived similarity and closeness towards each other. PloS One, 10(4), Article e0120878. |
[44] | Rabinowitch, T.-C., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2017). Joint rhythmic movement increases 4-year-old children’s prosocial sharing and fairness toward peers. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, Article 1050. |
[45] | Ren. Q. Y., Sun. Y. M., Lu. X. J., Huang. C., & Hu. L. (2019). Empathy: Methodologies and characteristics from a psychophysiological perspective. Chinese Science Bulletin, 64(22), 2292-2304. |
[46] | Roberts, W., & Strayer, J. (1996). Empathy, emotional expressiveness, and prosocial behavior. Child Development, 67(2), 449-470. |
[47] | Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2), 1-36. |
[48] | Savage, P. E. (2019). Cultural evolution of music. Palgrave Communications, 5(1), Article 16. |
[49] | Savage, P. E., Loui, P., Tarr, B., Schachner, A., Glowacki, L., Mithen, S., & Fitch, W. T. (2021). Music as a coevolved system for social bonding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 44, Article e59. |
[50] | Schellenberg, E. G., Corrigall, K. A., Dys, S. P., & Malti, T. (2015). Group music training and children’s prosocial skills. Plos One, 10(10), Article e0141449. |
[51] | Schellenberg, E. G., & Lima, C. F. (2024). Music training and nonmusical abilities. Annual Review of Psychology, 75(1), 87-128. |
[52] | Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Aharon-Peretz, J., & Perry, D. (2009). Two systems for empathy: A double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Brain, 132(3), 617-627. |
[53] | Spielberger, C., Gorsuch, R., Lushene, R., Vagg, P., & Jacobs, G. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press. |
[54] | Steele, J. R. (2019). The relationship between music aptitude, empathy, and sensitivity to emotional prosody: An ERP investigation (Unpublished master’s thesis). Middle Tennessee State University. |
[55] | Stotland, E. (1978). Empathy, fantasy, and helping. Sage Publications. |
[56] |
Stupacher, J., Mikkelsen, J., & Vuust, P. (2022). Higher empathy is associated with stronger social bonding when moving together with music. Psychology of Music, 50(5), 1511-1526.
doi: 10.1177/03057356211050681 pmid: 36097608 |
[57] | The jamovi project. (2023). jamovi. (Version 2.4) [Computer Software]. |
[58] | Wallentin, M., Højlund, A., Friis-Olivarius, M., Vuust, C., & Vuust, P. (2010). The Musical Ear Test, a new reliable test for measuring musical competence. Learning and Individual Differences, 20(3), 188-196. |
[59] | Walter, H. (2012). Social cognitive neuroscience of empathy: Concepts, circuits, and genes. Emotion Review, 4(1), 9-17. |
[60] | Wan, Y., & Zhu, L. (2021). Effects of rhythmic turn-taking coordination on five-year-old children’s prosocial behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 57(11), 1787-1795. |
[61] | Wang, X. D., Wang, X. L., & Ma, H. (1999). Rating scales for mental health. Chinese Mental Health Journal Publisher. |
[62] |
Watanabe, D., Savion-Lemieux, T., & Penhune, V. B. (2007). The effect of early musical training on adult motor performance: Evidence for a sensitive period in motor learning. Experimental Brain Research, 176(2), 332-340.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0619-z pmid: 16896980 |
[63] | Wei, B., Zhang, X., Cui, D., & Li, Y. (2023). Linking objective and subjective social status to altruistic sharing in China: The role of empathy. Current Psychology, 42, 27401-27414. |
[64] | Wöllner, C. (2012). Is empathy related to the perception of emotional expression in music? A multimodal time-series analysis. Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity and the Arts, 6(3), 214-223. |
[65] | Wu, X., & Lu, X. (2021). Musical training in the development of empathy and prosocial behaviors. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, Article 661769. |
[66] | Yan, Z., Zeng, X., Su, J., & Zhang, X. (2021). The dark side of empathy: Meta-analysis evidence of the relationship between empathy and depression. PsyCh Journal, 10(5), 794-804. |
[67] | Yu, C.-L., & Chou, T.-L. (2018). A dual route model of empathy: A neurobiological prospective. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 2212. |
[68] | Zhang, F. F., Dong, Y., Wang, K., Zhan, Z. Y., & Xie, L. F. (2010). Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index-C. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 18(2), 155-157. |
[69] |
Zhang, W. X., Li, X., Chen, G. H., & Cao, Y. M. (2021). The relationship between positive parenting and adolescent prosocial behaviour: The mediating role of empathy and the moderating role of the oxytocin receptor gene. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 53(9), 976-991.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00976 |
[70] | Zhong, Y. P., Yang, Z. L., & Fan, W. (2015). The effects of self-other overlap on helping behavior: Moderating of perspective taking. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 47(8), 1050-1057. |
[71] | Zhou, H., & Long, L. R. (2004). Statistical remedies for common method biases. Advances in Psychological Science, 12(6), 942-950. |
[1] | ZHANG Wenyun, ZHUO Shiwei, ZHENG Qianqian, GUAN Yinglin, PENG Weiwei. Autistic traits influence pain empathy: The mediation role of pain-related negative emotion and cognition [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2023, 55(9): 1501-1517. |
[2] | CHEN Jie, WU Ke, SHI Yupeng, AI Xiaoqing. The relationship between dispositional self-construal and empathy for ingroup and outgroup members’ pain: evidence from ERPs [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2021, 53(6): 629-638. |
[3] | ZHANG Zhenghua, HAN Mei, ZHANG Fang, LI Weijun. Musical training improves rhythm integrative processing of classical Chinese poem [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2020, 52(7): 847-860. |
[4] | CHENG Jiaping; LUO Yuejia; CUI Fang. Empathy for pain influenced by cognitive load: Evidence from an ERP study [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(5): 622-630. |
Viewed | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Full text |
|
|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Abstract |
|
|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||