Acta Psychologica Sinica ›› 2020, Vol. 52 ›› Issue (9): 1087-1104.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.01087
• Reports of Empirical Studies • Previous Articles Next Articles
WEN Fangfang1, MA Shuhan1, YE Hanxue1, QI Yue2(), ZUO Bin1()
Received:
2020-02-18
Published:
2020-09-25
Online:
2020-07-24
Contact:
QI Yue,ZUO Bin
E-mail:qiy@psych.ac.cn;zuobin@mail.ccnu.edu.cn
Supported by:
WEN Fangfang, MA Shuhan, YE Hanxue, QI Yue, ZUO Bin. (2020). “Psychological Typhoon Eye Effect” and “Ripple Effect”: Double perspective test of risk perception and anxiety characteristics of people in different COVID-19 severity regions. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 52(9), 1087-1104.
Author/year | Nature of the emergency | Specific measurement time | Measurement Index | Evaluation perspective | Psychological typhoo eye effect |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Neighborhood | Potential risk | Anxiety level | Actor | √ | |
Neighborhood | Potential risk | Risk perception | Actor | Part √ | |
Neighborhood | Potential risk | Anxiety level | Actor | × | |
SARS | During and after the disaster | Epidemic risk perception, psychological stress, epidemic development expectation, coping behavior, mental health, and economic development expectation | Bystander | √ | |
SARS | After the disaster | Levels of anxiety, and life disruption | Actor | √ | |
Earthquake | After the disaster | Recovery time and funding needed | Bystander | √ | |
Earthquake | After the disaster | The probability of post-disaster epidemic situation, the amount of first-aid measures needed in disaster areas, the number of (psychological) doctors needed per thousand residents in disaster areas, and the amount of drugs needed by residents in disaster areas | Actor (or imagining oneself as an ator) | √ | |
Earthquake | After the disaster | Same as | Actor (or imagining oneself as an ator) | √ | |
Earthquake | After the disaster | A willingness to buy insurance, and to vacate one’s place of residence | Actor (or imagining oneself as an ator) | √ | |
Risk perception of aftershocks | Actor (or imagining oneself as an ator) | √ | |||
Anxiety level (Chinese version of state trait anxiety inventory) | Actor (or imagining oneself as an ator) | × | |||
SARS | After the disaster | Anxiety level (Chinese version of state trait anxiety inventory) | Actor | √ | |
Adjacent shelter facilities | Potential risk | Risk perception, perceived benefits and harm | Actor | √ | |
COVID-19 | In a disaster | Two psychological indicators of subjective fear (estimation of the highest price people in Wuhan are willing to pay for masks and number of days of delay in starting school in Wuhan in 2020) | Bystander | √ |
Table 1 Research results of psychological typhoon eye effect
Author/year | Nature of the emergency | Specific measurement time | Measurement Index | Evaluation perspective | Psychological typhoo eye effect |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Neighborhood | Potential risk | Anxiety level | Actor | √ | |
Neighborhood | Potential risk | Risk perception | Actor | Part √ | |
Neighborhood | Potential risk | Anxiety level | Actor | × | |
SARS | During and after the disaster | Epidemic risk perception, psychological stress, epidemic development expectation, coping behavior, mental health, and economic development expectation | Bystander | √ | |
SARS | After the disaster | Levels of anxiety, and life disruption | Actor | √ | |
Earthquake | After the disaster | Recovery time and funding needed | Bystander | √ | |
Earthquake | After the disaster | The probability of post-disaster epidemic situation, the amount of first-aid measures needed in disaster areas, the number of (psychological) doctors needed per thousand residents in disaster areas, and the amount of drugs needed by residents in disaster areas | Actor (or imagining oneself as an ator) | √ | |
Earthquake | After the disaster | Same as | Actor (or imagining oneself as an ator) | √ | |
Earthquake | After the disaster | A willingness to buy insurance, and to vacate one’s place of residence | Actor (or imagining oneself as an ator) | √ | |
Risk perception of aftershocks | Actor (or imagining oneself as an ator) | √ | |||
Anxiety level (Chinese version of state trait anxiety inventory) | Actor (or imagining oneself as an ator) | × | |||
SARS | After the disaster | Anxiety level (Chinese version of state trait anxiety inventory) | Actor | √ | |
Adjacent shelter facilities | Potential risk | Risk perception, perceived benefits and harm | Actor | √ | |
COVID-19 | In a disaster | Two psychological indicators of subjective fear (estimation of the highest price people in Wuhan are willing to pay for masks and number of days of delay in starting school in Wuhan in 2020) | Bystander | √ |
Classification of epidemic situation of different degree | Regions | N | Sex (male/female) | Age (M ± SD) |
---|---|---|---|---|
Regions with different risk levels (geographical distance from hard-hit areas) | Wuhan | 468 | 134/334 | 35.74 ± 12.28 |
Other parts of Hubei | 867 | 317/550 | 33.13 ± 11.78 | |
Hubei Province bordering on the city | 327 | 110/217 | 28.82 ± 10.33 | |
Other provinces and cities in the country | 1019 | 278/741 | 31.46 ± 10.89 | |
Areas with different risk levels (daily cumulative confirmed cases) | High risk regions | 231 | 97/134 | 33.17 ± 12.14 |
Medium to high risk regions | 1145 | 372/773 | 34.00 ± 11.95 | |
Medium risk regions | 251 | 72/179 | 29.68 ± 10.57 | |
Low risk regions | 1054 | 298/756 | 31.20 ± 10.91 |
Table 2 Sample collection from January 24 to February 20 (N = 2681)
Classification of epidemic situation of different degree | Regions | N | Sex (male/female) | Age (M ± SD) |
---|---|---|---|---|
Regions with different risk levels (geographical distance from hard-hit areas) | Wuhan | 468 | 134/334 | 35.74 ± 12.28 |
Other parts of Hubei | 867 | 317/550 | 33.13 ± 11.78 | |
Hubei Province bordering on the city | 327 | 110/217 | 28.82 ± 10.33 | |
Other provinces and cities in the country | 1019 | 278/741 | 31.46 ± 10.89 | |
Areas with different risk levels (daily cumulative confirmed cases) | High risk regions | 231 | 97/134 | 33.17 ± 12.14 |
Medium to high risk regions | 1145 | 372/773 | 34.00 ± 11.95 | |
Medium risk regions | 251 | 72/179 | 29.68 ± 10.57 | |
Low risk regions | 1054 | 298/756 | 31.20 ± 10.91 |
Classification of epidemic severity | Region | Risk perception | Possibility | Severity | Unpredictability | Uncontrollability | Anxiety |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Regions with different risk levels (geographical distance from hard- hit areas) | Wuhan | 30.20 ± 5.32 | 2.29 ± 1.04 | 12.22 ± 2.26 | 8.78 ± 1.58 | 6.91 ± 2.04 | 20.37 ± 5.40 |
Other parts of Hubei | 29.05 ± 5.76 | 1.91 ± 0.88 | 11.84 ± 2.63 | 8.64 ± 1.78 | 6.66 ± 2.24 | 19.27 ± 4.87 | |
Hubei Province bordering on the city | 28.59 ± 5.35 | 1.99 ± 0.78 | 11.61 ± 2.35 | 8.28 ± 1.70 | 6.71 ± 2.03 | 19.30 ± 4.94 | |
Other provinces and cities in the country | 28.79 ± 5.45 | 1.92 ± 0.78 | 11.62 ± 2.47 | 8.38 ± 1.74 | 6.86 ± 2.00 | 19.07 ± 5.12 | |
Areas with different risk levels (daily cumulative confirmed cases) | High risk regions | 29.73 ± 5.32 | 2.01 ± 0.94 | 11.92 ± 2.42 | 8.75 ± 1.56 | 7.04 ± 2.19 | 19.06 ± 5.25 |
Medium to high risk regions | 29.32 ± 5.67 | 2.03 ± 0.95 | 11.95 ± 2.52 | 8.65 ± 1.74 | 6.69 ± 2.15 | 19.72 ± 5.02 | |
Medium risk regions | 28.28 ± 5.53 | 1.90 ± 0.76 | 11.42 ± 2.60 | 8.31 ± 1.80 | 6.64 ± 1.98 | 18.29 ± 4.87 | |
Low risk regions | 28.91 ± 5.42 | 1.96 ± 0.79 | 11.70 ± 2.41 | 8.38 ± 1.72 | 6.87 ± 2.02 | 19.37 ± 5.14 |
Table 3 Descriptive statistics of risk perception, anxiety, and psychological distance (M ± SD)
Classification of epidemic severity | Region | Risk perception | Possibility | Severity | Unpredictability | Uncontrollability | Anxiety |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Regions with different risk levels (geographical distance from hard- hit areas) | Wuhan | 30.20 ± 5.32 | 2.29 ± 1.04 | 12.22 ± 2.26 | 8.78 ± 1.58 | 6.91 ± 2.04 | 20.37 ± 5.40 |
Other parts of Hubei | 29.05 ± 5.76 | 1.91 ± 0.88 | 11.84 ± 2.63 | 8.64 ± 1.78 | 6.66 ± 2.24 | 19.27 ± 4.87 | |
Hubei Province bordering on the city | 28.59 ± 5.35 | 1.99 ± 0.78 | 11.61 ± 2.35 | 8.28 ± 1.70 | 6.71 ± 2.03 | 19.30 ± 4.94 | |
Other provinces and cities in the country | 28.79 ± 5.45 | 1.92 ± 0.78 | 11.62 ± 2.47 | 8.38 ± 1.74 | 6.86 ± 2.00 | 19.07 ± 5.12 | |
Areas with different risk levels (daily cumulative confirmed cases) | High risk regions | 29.73 ± 5.32 | 2.01 ± 0.94 | 11.92 ± 2.42 | 8.75 ± 1.56 | 7.04 ± 2.19 | 19.06 ± 5.25 |
Medium to high risk regions | 29.32 ± 5.67 | 2.03 ± 0.95 | 11.95 ± 2.52 | 8.65 ± 1.74 | 6.69 ± 2.15 | 19.72 ± 5.02 | |
Medium risk regions | 28.28 ± 5.53 | 1.90 ± 0.76 | 11.42 ± 2.60 | 8.31 ± 1.80 | 6.64 ± 1.98 | 18.29 ± 4.87 | |
Low risk regions | 28.91 ± 5.42 | 1.96 ± 0.79 | 11.70 ± 2.41 | 8.38 ± 1.72 | 6.87 ± 2.02 | 19.37 ± 5.14 |
Variables | F | df | p | R2 | Adjusted R2 | Β |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Risk perception | 15.74 | 1 | 0.000 | 0.006 | 0.006 | 0.077** |
Possibility | 77.86 | 1 | 0.000 | 0.029 | 0.028 | 0.17*** |
Severity | 7.32 | 1 | 0.007 | 0.003 | 0.002 | 0.053** |
Unpredictability | 11.76 | 1 | 0.001 | 0.004 | 0.004 | 0.067** |
Uncontrollability | 0.67 | 1 | 0.414 | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.016 |
Anxiety | 31.49 | 1 | 0.000 | 0.012 | 0.011 | 0.11*** |
Table 4 Regression analysis results of risk perception and anxiety with different subjective psychological distance
Variables | F | df | p | R2 | Adjusted R2 | Β |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Risk perception | 15.74 | 1 | 0.000 | 0.006 | 0.006 | 0.077** |
Possibility | 77.86 | 1 | 0.000 | 0.029 | 0.028 | 0.17*** |
Severity | 7.32 | 1 | 0.007 | 0.003 | 0.002 | 0.053** |
Unpredictability | 11.76 | 1 | 0.001 | 0.004 | 0.004 | 0.067** |
Uncontrollability | 0.67 | 1 | 0.414 | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.016 |
Anxiety | 31.49 | 1 | 0.000 | 0.012 | 0.011 | 0.11*** |
Classification of epidemic severity | Region | Quantity | Sex (male/female) | Age (M ± SD) |
---|---|---|---|---|
Regions with different risk levels (geographical distance from hard-hit areas) | Wuhan area | 331 | 119/212 | 27.26 ± 11.49 |
Other parts of Hubei | 300 | 105/195 | 23.13 ± 9.00 | |
Hubei Province bordering on the city | 305 | 118/187 | 29.46 ± 12.53 | |
Other provinces and cities in the country | 1211 | 387/824 | 24.23 ± 8.67 | |
Areas with different risk levels (daily cumulative confirmed cases) | High risk areas | 631 | 224/407 | 25.31 ± 10.59 |
Medium to high risk areas | 683 | 262/421 | 27.91 ± 11.23 | |
Medium risk areas | 529 | 149/380 | 23.71 ± 8.76 | |
Low risk areas | 301 | 94/207 | 21.96 ± 5.54 |
Table 5 Sampling conditions from 21 February to 5 March (N = 2152)
Classification of epidemic severity | Region | Quantity | Sex (male/female) | Age (M ± SD) |
---|---|---|---|---|
Regions with different risk levels (geographical distance from hard-hit areas) | Wuhan area | 331 | 119/212 | 27.26 ± 11.49 |
Other parts of Hubei | 300 | 105/195 | 23.13 ± 9.00 | |
Hubei Province bordering on the city | 305 | 118/187 | 29.46 ± 12.53 | |
Other provinces and cities in the country | 1211 | 387/824 | 24.23 ± 8.67 | |
Areas with different risk levels (daily cumulative confirmed cases) | High risk areas | 631 | 224/407 | 25.31 ± 10.59 |
Medium to high risk areas | 683 | 262/421 | 27.91 ± 11.23 | |
Medium risk areas | 529 | 149/380 | 23.71 ± 8.76 | |
Low risk areas | 301 | 94/207 | 21.96 ± 5.54 |
Variables | Geo-spatial distance | Cumulative confirmed cases | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Wuhan | Others in Hubei | Hubei border | The rest of the country | High risk areas | Medium to high risk areas | Medium risk areas | Low risk areas | |
Risk perception | 14.48 ± 3.15 | 13.58 ± 3.12 | 14.30 ± 3.03 | 14.07 ± 3.08 | 14.05 ± 3.17 | 14.30 ± 3.05 | 14.22 ± 3.09 | 13.53 ± 3.00 |
Possibility | 3.19 ± 1.04 | 3.02 ± 0.96 | 3.40 ± 0.98 | 3.35 ± 0.95 | 3.11 ± 1.01 | 3.39 ± 0.97 | 3.40 ± 0.94 | 3.29 ± 0.98 |
Severity | 4.05 ± 0.98 | 3.81 ± 0.97 | 3.88 ± 0.94 | 3.84 ± 0.92 | 3.93 ± 0.98 | 3.8 ± 0.93 | 3.89 ± 0.89 | 3.69 ± 0.95 |
Unpredictability | 4.18 ± 0.99 | 3.86 ± 1.04 | 4.08 ± 0.95 | 3.92 ± 0.98 | 4.03 ± 1.03 | 4.04 ± 0.97 | 3.95 ± 0.97 | 3.76 ± 0.98 |
Uncontrollability | 3.06 ± 1.07 | 2.89 ± 1.01 | 2.94 ± 1.10 | 2.96 ± 1.04 | 2.98 ± 1.05 | 2.99 ± 1.07 | 2.98 ± 1.06 | 2.86 ± 0.98 |
Anxiety | 8.53 ± 2.33 | 8.36 ± 2.27 | 9.17 ± 2.29 | 9.02 ± 2.18 | 8.45 ± 2.30 | 9.11 ± 2.29 | 9.08 ± 2.12 | 8.86 ± 2.15 |
Psychotherapist | 50.95 ± 29.76 | 52.88 ± 31.05 | 55.70 ± 30.49 | 60.37 ± 29.84 | 51.87 ± 30.37 | 56.77 ± 30.43 | 61.46 ± 30.38 | 62.23 ± 28.07 |
Doctors | 61.28 ± 32.51 | 59.75 ± 32.50 | 64.67 ± 31.61 | 67.47 ± 29.81 | 60.55 ± 32.49 | 65.55 ± 30.86 | 67.80 ± 30.06 | 68.37 ± 28.70 |
Table 6 Descriptive statistics on the evaluation of epidemic situation of residents in Wuhan (M ± SD)
Variables | Geo-spatial distance | Cumulative confirmed cases | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Wuhan | Others in Hubei | Hubei border | The rest of the country | High risk areas | Medium to high risk areas | Medium risk areas | Low risk areas | |
Risk perception | 14.48 ± 3.15 | 13.58 ± 3.12 | 14.30 ± 3.03 | 14.07 ± 3.08 | 14.05 ± 3.17 | 14.30 ± 3.05 | 14.22 ± 3.09 | 13.53 ± 3.00 |
Possibility | 3.19 ± 1.04 | 3.02 ± 0.96 | 3.40 ± 0.98 | 3.35 ± 0.95 | 3.11 ± 1.01 | 3.39 ± 0.97 | 3.40 ± 0.94 | 3.29 ± 0.98 |
Severity | 4.05 ± 0.98 | 3.81 ± 0.97 | 3.88 ± 0.94 | 3.84 ± 0.92 | 3.93 ± 0.98 | 3.8 ± 0.93 | 3.89 ± 0.89 | 3.69 ± 0.95 |
Unpredictability | 4.18 ± 0.99 | 3.86 ± 1.04 | 4.08 ± 0.95 | 3.92 ± 0.98 | 4.03 ± 1.03 | 4.04 ± 0.97 | 3.95 ± 0.97 | 3.76 ± 0.98 |
Uncontrollability | 3.06 ± 1.07 | 2.89 ± 1.01 | 2.94 ± 1.10 | 2.96 ± 1.04 | 2.98 ± 1.05 | 2.99 ± 1.07 | 2.98 ± 1.06 | 2.86 ± 0.98 |
Anxiety | 8.53 ± 2.33 | 8.36 ± 2.27 | 9.17 ± 2.29 | 9.02 ± 2.18 | 8.45 ± 2.30 | 9.11 ± 2.29 | 9.08 ± 2.12 | 8.86 ± 2.15 |
Psychotherapist | 50.95 ± 29.76 | 52.88 ± 31.05 | 55.70 ± 30.49 | 60.37 ± 29.84 | 51.87 ± 30.37 | 56.77 ± 30.43 | 61.46 ± 30.38 | 62.23 ± 28.07 |
Doctors | 61.28 ± 32.51 | 59.75 ± 32.50 | 64.67 ± 31.61 | 67.47 ± 29.81 | 60.55 ± 32.49 | 65.55 ± 30.86 | 67.80 ± 30.06 | 68.37 ± 28.70 |
Variables | F | df | p | R2 | Adjusted R2 | β |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Risk perception | 2.64 | 1 | 0.104 | 0.001 | 0.001 | 0.035 |
Possibility | 3.49 | 1 | 0.062 | 0.002 | 0.001 | -0.04 |
Severity | 13.36 | 1 | 0.000 | 0.006 | 0.006 | 0.079** |
Unpredictability | 8.93 | 1 | 0.003 | 0.004 | 0.004 | 0.064** |
Uncontrollability | 0.19 | 1 | 0.660 | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.009 |
Anxiety | 1.69 | 1 | 0.193 | 0.001 | 0.000 | -0.028 |
Number of counsellors required | 4.46 | 1 | 0.035 | 0.002 | 0.002 | -0.046* |
Number of doctors required | 3.85 | 1 | 0.05 | 0.002 | 0.001 | -0.042 |
Table 7 Results of regression analysis
Variables | F | df | p | R2 | Adjusted R2 | β |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Risk perception | 2.64 | 1 | 0.104 | 0.001 | 0.001 | 0.035 |
Possibility | 3.49 | 1 | 0.062 | 0.002 | 0.001 | -0.04 |
Severity | 13.36 | 1 | 0.000 | 0.006 | 0.006 | 0.079** |
Unpredictability | 8.93 | 1 | 0.003 | 0.004 | 0.004 | 0.064** |
Uncontrollability | 0.19 | 1 | 0.660 | 0.000 | 0.000 | 0.009 |
Anxiety | 1.69 | 1 | 0.193 | 0.001 | 0.000 | -0.028 |
Number of counsellors required | 4.46 | 1 | 0.035 | 0.002 | 0.002 | -0.046* |
Number of doctors required | 3.85 | 1 | 0.05 | 0.002 | 0.001 | -0.042 |
[1] |
Burns, W. J., & Slovic, P. (2012). Risk perception and behaviors: anticipating and responding to crises. Risk Analysis, 32(4), 579-582.
doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2012.01791.x URL pmid: 22500649 |
[2] | Chen, Y. Q., & He, Y. S. (2008). Research on the difference between the Internet survey and the traditional paper survey. Statistic and Decision Making, (16), 32-34. |
[3] |
Cho, J., & Lee, J. (2006). An integrated model of risk and risk-reducing strategies. Journal of Business Research, 59(1), 112-120.
doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2005.03.006 URL |
[4] |
Erdem, T., & Swait, J. (2004). Brand credibility, brand consideration, and choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(1), 191-198.
doi: 10.1086/383434 URL |
[5] | Forsythe, S. M., & Shi, B. (2003). Consumer patronage and risk perceptions in Internet shopping.[J] ournal of Business Research, 56(11), 867-875. |
[6] | Guedeney, C., & Mendel, G. (1973). L'angoisse atomique et les centrales nucléaires: Contribution psychanalytique et sociopsychanalytique à l’ étude d’ un phenomena collecti. Paris, Payot. |
[7] | Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1972). The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. In E. E. Jones et al. (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behavior. Morristown, N.J.: General Learning Press. |
[8] |
Kasperson, R. E., Renn, O., Slovic, P., Brown, H. S., Emel, J., Goble, R., … Ratick, S. (1988). The social amplification of risk: A conceptual framework. Risk Analysis, 8(2), 177-187.
doi: 10.1111/risk.1988.8.issue-2 URL |
[9] | Li, H. Q., Wang, H. S., Jia, J. M., & Fan, C. M. (2008). Demand survey and satisfaction analysis of Dujiangyan victims in Wenchuan earthquake. Journal of Huazhong University of science and Technology (Social Science Edition), 22(6), 49-53. |
[10] |
Li, S., Liu, H., Bai, X. W., Ren, X. P., Zheng, R., Li, J. Z., … Wang, Z. J. (2009). Psychological typhoon eye in 2008 Wenchuan earthquake of May 12. Science of Technology, 27(903), 87-89.
doi: 10.1021/es00038a008 URL |
[11] |
Li, S., Rao, L.-L., Bai, X.-W., Zheng, R., Ren, X.-P. Li, J.-Z. … Zhang, K. (2010) Progression of the “Psychological Typhoon Eye” and variations since the Wenchuan earthquake. PLoS ONE, 5(3), e9727. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009727
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009727 URL pmid: 20305817 |
[12] |
Li, S., Rao, L.-L., Ren, X.-P., Bai, X.-W., Zheng, R., Li, J.-Z., … Liu, H. (2009). Psychological typhoon eye in the 2008 Wenchuan earthquake. PLoS ONE, 4(3), e4964. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004964
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004964 URL pmid: 19305501 |
[13] | Li, W. J. (2016). Study on Psychological typhoon eye effect in avoiding group events. Learning Forum, 32(1), 73-77. |
[14] | Liang, Z., Xu, J. H., LI, S., Sun, Y., Liu, C. J., & Ye, X. B. (2008). Perplexing problems in risk communication of emergent public security events: A psychological perspective. Journal of Natural Disasters, 17(2), 25-30. |
[15] |
Lima, M. L. (2004). On the influence of risk perception on mental health: Living near an incinerator. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24(1), 71-84.
doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(03)00026-4 URL |
[16] |
Lindell, M. K., & Earle, T. C. (1983). How close is close enough: public perceptions of the risks of industrial facilities. Risk Analysis, 3(4), 245-253.
doi: 10.1111/risk.1983.3.issue-4 URL |
[17] |
Maderthaner, R., Guttman, G., Swaton, E., & Otway, H. J. (1978). Effect of distance upon risk perception. Journal of Applied Psychology, 63(3), 380-382.
doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.63.3.380 URL |
[18] | Melber, B. D., Nealey, S. M., Hammersla, J., & Rankin, W. L. (1977). Nuclear power and the public: Analysis of collected survey research. Seattle: Battelle Memorial Institute, Human Affairs Research Center. |
[19] |
Okeke, C. U., Armour, A. (2000). Post-landfill siting perceptions of nearby residents: A case study of Halton landfill. Applied Geography, 20(2), 137-154.
doi: 10.1016/S0143-6228(00)00003-5 URL |
[20] |
Otway, H. J., & von Winterfeldt, D. (1982). Beyond acceptable risk: On the social acceptability of technologies. Policy Sciences, 14(3), 247-256.
doi: 10.1007/BF00136399 URL |
[21] | Pidgeon, N. I. C. K., Turner, B. A. R. R. Y., Toft, B. R. I. A. N., & Blockley, D. A. V. I. D. (1992). Hazard management and safety culture. In D. J. Parker & J. W. Handmer (Eds) Hazard Management and Emergency Planning: Perspectives on Britain. James and James, London. |
[22] |
Slovic, P. (1987). Perception of risk. Science, 236(4799), 280-285.
doi: 10.1126/science.3563507 URL pmid: 3563507 |
[23] |
Slovic, P. (2000). What does it mean to know a cumulative risk? Adolescents' perceptions of short-term and long-term consequences of smoking. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 13(2), 259-266.
doi: 10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0771 URL |
[24] | Shi, K., Chen, X. F., Hu, W. P., Jia, J. M., Gao, J., Li, W. D. … Zhang, L. H. (2003). A study on the risk cognitive characteristics of SARS epidemic in Beijing citizens. Population Research, 27(4), 42-46. |
[25] |
Sitkin, S. B., & Pablo, A. L. (1992). Reconceptualizing the determinants of risk behavior. Academy of Management Review, 17(1), 9-38.
doi: 10.5465/amr.1992.4279564 URL |
[26] | Tversky, A., & Griffin, D. (1991). 12 Endowment and contrast in judgments of well-being. Strategy and Choice, 21, 297-318. |
[27] | Wang, L., & Jia, J. M. (2014). Dynamic characteristics of risk perception of sudden disaster events--Evidence from Internet search. Management Review, 26(5), 169. |
[28] | Wiegman, O., Gutteling, J. M., Boer, H. (1991). Verification of information through direct experiences with an Industrial hazard. Basic & Applied Social Psychology, 12(3), 325-339. |
[29] | Wu, L., & Zhang, X. (2012). Psychometric paradigm in risk perception research. Journal of Nanjing Normal University (Social Sciences),(2), 95-102. |
[30] | Xie, J. Q., Xie, X. F., Gan, Y. Q. (2011). Psychological typhoon eye effect in the Wenchuan earthquake. Acta Scicentiarum Naturalum Universitis Pekinesis, 47(5), 944-952. |
[31] | Xie, X. F. (1995). Research of risk decision-making mode of company managers. Research of Scientific Management, 1(6), 1. |
[32] | Xie, X. F., & Lin, J. (2012). Review of psychological typhoon eye effect. China Emergency Management, 21-25. |
[33] |
Xie, X. F., Stone, E., Zheng, R., & Zhang, R. G. (2011). The “Typhoon Eye Effect”: Determinants of distress during the SARS epidemic. Journal of Risk Research, 14(9), 1091-1107.
doi: 10.1080/13669877.2011.571790 URL |
[34] | Xie, X. F., Xie, D. M., Zheng, R. & Zhang, L. S. (2003). First exploration on the characteristics of public reason in the SARS crisis. Management Review, 15(4), 8-14+65. |
[35] | Xie, X. F., & Xu, L. C. (1996). Public bias in risk cognition. Journal of Developments in Psychology, 14(2), 23-26. |
[36] | Xie, X. F., & Xu, L. C. (2000). Research of risk cognition of managers in work condition. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 32(1), 115-120. |
[37] | Xie, X. F., Zheng, R., Xie, D. M. & Wang, H. (2005). Analysis of psychological panic in SARS. Acta Scicentiarum Naturalum Universitis Pekinesis, 41(4), 628-638. |
[38] | Xu, M. X., Zheng, R., Rao, L. L., Kuang, Y., Yang, S. W., Ding, Y., … Li, S. (2020). The pscyhological typhoon eye effect in the epidemic situation should be properly dealt with. Bulletin of Chinese Academy of Sciences, 35(3), 273-282. |
[39] | Xue, L., & Zhong, K. B. (2005). Classification, grading, and staging of public emergencies: The management foundation of an emergency system. China Administration,(2), 102-107. |
[40] | Yu, H. Y., & Huang, X. Z. (2011). The difference between written survey and online survey -- A comparison of two data collection methods. Statistics & Information Forum, 26(10), 97-103. |
[41] | Yu, Q. Y., & Xie, X. F. (2006). The cognitive characteristics of risk in environment. Journal of Psychological Science, 29(2), 362-365. |
[42] |
Zhang, S. X., Huang, H., Wei, F. (2020). Geographical distance to the epicenter of Covid-19 predicts the burnout of the working population: Ripple effect or typhoon eye effect?. Psychiatry Research, 288. https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.psychres.2020.112998
doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112966 URL pmid: 32334276 |
[43] |
Zheng, R., Rao, L. L., Zheng, X. L., Cai, C., Wei, Z. H., Xuan, Y. H., & Li, S. (2015). The more involved in lead-zinc mining risk the less frightened: A psychological typhoon eye perspective. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 44, 126-134.
doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.10.002 URL pmid: 32287833 |
[44] |
Zung, W. W. (1971). A rating instrument for anxiety disorders. Psychosom, 12(6), 371-379.
doi: 10.1016/S0033-3182(71)71479-0 URL |
[45] | Zuo, B. (2009). Social psychology. Beijing: Higher Education Press. |
[46] |
Zuo, B., Zhang, X., Wen, F. F., & Zhao, Y. (2020). The influence of stressful life events on depression among Chinese university students: Multiple mediating roles of fatalism and core self-evaluations. Journal of Affective Disorders, 260, 84-90.
doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.08.083 URL pmid: 31493644 |
[1] | SI Jiwei, GUO Kaiyue, ZHAO Xiaomeng, ZHANG Mingliang, LI Hongxia, HUANG Bijuan, XU Yanli. Transition of latent classes of children’s mathematics anxiety in primary school and the distinctive effects of parental educational involvement: A three-wave longitudinal study [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2022, 54(4): 355-370. |
[2] | LI Wanyue, LIU Shen, HAN Shangfeng, ZHANG Lin, XU Qiang. Emotional bias of trait anxiety on pre-attentive processing of facial expressions: ERP investigation [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2022, 54(1): 1-11. |
[3] | LAI Lizu, REN Zhihong, YAN Yifei, NIU Gengfeng, ZHAO Chunxiao, LUO Mei, ZHANG Lin. The double-edged-sword effect of empathy: The secondary traumatic stress and vicarious posttraumatic growth of psychological hotline counselors during the outbreak of COVID-19 [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2021, 53(9): 992-1002. |
[4] | HOU Juan, ZHU Yingge, FANG Xiaoyi. Mobile phone addiction and depression: Multiple mediating effects of social anxiety and attentional bias to negative emotional information [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2021, 53(4): 362-373. |
[5] | LIU Jie, LI Jinqi, SHEN Chaoran, HU Xiaohui, ZHAO Tinghao, GUAN Qing, LUO Yuejia. The neural mechanism of approximate number processing for mathematical anxious individuals: An EEG study [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2020, 52(8): 958-970. |
[6] | ZHANG Jijia, LU Yutong, ZHANG Qirui, ZHANG Jinqiao. The effects of foreign language anxiety, nervousness and cognitive load on foreign language lying: Evidence from Chinese-English bilinguals [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2020, 52(7): 861-873. |
[7] | Shifeng LI, Yiling WU, Fumin ZHANG, Qiongying XU, Aibao ZHOU. Self-affirmation buffering by the general public reduces anxiety levels during the COVID-19 epidemic [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2020, 52(7): 886-894. |
[8] | ZHU Zhenzhong, LI Xiaojun, LIU Fu, Haipeng CHEN. How visual novelty affects consumer purchase intention: The moderating effects of self-construal and product type [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2020, 52(11): 1352-1364. |
[9] | LUO Yijun, NIU Gengfeng, CHEN Hong. Early life environmental unpredictability and overeating: Based on life history theory [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2020, 52(10): 1224-1236. |
[10] | LI Wanyue, HAN Shangfeng, LIU Shen, YANG Yaping, ZHANG Lin, XU Qiang. Scene effects on facial expression detection: The moderating effects of trait anxiety [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(8): 869-878. |
[11] | LIU Jingyuan,LI Hong. How state anxiety influences time perception: Moderated mediating effect of cognitive appraisal and attentional bias [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(7): 747-758. |
[12] | ZHANG Wenpei, SHEN Qunlun, SONG Jintao, ZHOU Renlai. Classification of test-anxious individuals using Event-Related Potentials (ERPs): The effectiveness of machine learning algorithms [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(10): 1116-1127. |
[13] | Huini PENG, Jianhui WU, Xiaofang SUN, Qing GUAN, Yuejia LUO. Trait anxiety predicts the response to acute psychological stress [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(9): 997-1006. |
[14] | REN Zhihong, Zhang Yawen, JIANG Guangrong. Effectiveness of mindfulness meditation in intervention for anxiety: A meta-analysis [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(3): 283-305. |
[15] | HUANG Minxue; WANG Yiting; LIAO Junyun; LIU Maohong. Mixed effects of inconsistent reviews on consumers: The moderating roles of product attributes and regulatory focus [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(3): 370-382. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||