ISSN 0439-755X
CN 11-1911/B
主办:中国心理学会
   中国科学院心理研究所
出版:科学出版社

心理学报, 2020, 52(1): 12-25 doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.00012

研究报告

认知重评和表达抑制情绪调节策略的脑网络分析:来自EEG和ERP的证据

孙岩,, 薄思雨, 吕娇娇

辽宁师范大学心理学院, 大连 116029

Brain network analysis of cognitive reappraisal and expressive inhibition strategies: Evidence from EEG and ERP

SUN Yan,, BO Siyu, LV Jiaojiao

School of Psychology, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian 116029, China

通讯作者: 孙岩, E-mail:sunyan@lnnu.edu.cn

收稿日期: 2018-09-21   网络出版日期: 2020-01-25

基金资助: * 教育部人文社会科学规划基金项目.  17YJA190011
辽宁省教育厅项目.  WJ2019015

Received: 2018-09-21   Online: 2020-01-25

摘要

本文旨在对认知重评和表达抑制两种常用情绪调节策略的自发脑网络特征及认知神经活动进行深入探讨。研究采集36名在校大学生的静息态和任务态脑电数据, 经过源定位和图论分析发现节点效率与两种情绪调节显著相关的脑区, 以及脑区之间的功能连接。研究结果表明, 在使用认知重评进行情绪调节时会激活前额叶皮质、前扣带回、顶叶、海马旁回和枕叶等多个脑区, 在使用表达抑制进行情绪调节时会激活前额叶皮质、顶叶、海马旁回、枕叶、颞叶和脑岛等多个脑区。因此, 这些脑区的节点效率或功能连接强度可能成为评估个体使用认知重评和表达抑制调节情绪效果的指标。

关键词: 情绪调节 ; 认知重评 ; 表达抑制 ; 功能连接 ; 图论

Abstract

The ability to regulate emotions is related to psychological, social, and physical health. The two major emotion regulation strategies are cognitive reappraisal (CR) and expressive suppression (ES). Research suggests that CR produces affective, cognitive, and social consequences that are more beneficial to the individual, whereas ES has been consistently linked to more detrimental consequences. Although an increasing number of studies have begun to focus on the neural mechanisms of different types of emotion regulation, there has not yet been systematic research on the spontaneous brain activity associated with CR and ES. Resting activity has been shown to predict performance outcomes, highlight the functional relevance of the brain’s intrinsic fluctuations in response outputs. However, to date, there have been no studies to explore the relationship between the cognitive process of emotion regulation and the brain's resting EEG activity.
The current study explored the neural mechanisms of spontaneous brain activity during two emotion regulation strategies. Electroencephalography (EEG) enables direct measurement of neuronal activity, allowing characterization of the intrinsic neural cognitive network. Thirty-six college students (17 males and 19 females, aged 17~28 years old) participated in this study. For the first part of the study, EEG data was collected from participants with closed eyes; EEG collection occurred for a duration of 6 minutes. Neurological studies of resting state EEG have identified the predominant role of theta waves in determining cognitive control effort and behavioral performance. In the current study, source localization and graph theory analysis revealed that node efficiency was significantly correlated with the two major emotion regulation strategies, and there were functional connections between brain regions in the theta band.
Then, in order to improve the reliability of the resting result obtained above, a within subjects experiment was carried out. This experiment required subjects to watch emotional pictures under four emotion regulation conditions (watching neutral, watching negative, reappraisal negative, suppressing negative). The Late-positive potential (LPP) amplitude was obtained when viewing the emotional pictures under the four conditions. LPP is an effective physiological indicator of the emotion regulation effect. It allowed us to explore the emotion regulation effect under different emotion regulation strategies, and the intrinsic functional connections and node efficiency of the brain.
The results showed that the habitual use of CR was significantly correlated with several brain regions. Specifically, the prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate, and parietal cortex. Moreover, the brain regions significantly correlated with the LPP amplitude under CR were the parietal cortex, prefrontal cortex, parahippocampal gyrus, and occipital cortex. The brain regions that were significantly correlated with habitual use of ES included the prefrontal cortex, parietal cortex, insula, and parahippocampal gyrus. Finally, the brain regions that were significantly associated with LPP amplitude under ES included the prefrontal cortex, parietal cortex, parahippocampal gyrus, temporal cortex, and occipital cortex. Thus, these findings reveal that many brain regions are involved in these two mood regulation strategies, including the prefrontal cortex, parahippocampal gyrus, parietal cortex, and occipital cortex. In addition, the brain regions related to the different emotion regulation strategies differed slightly; specifically, CR was significantly associated with the anterior cingulate cortex while ES was related to temporal lobe and insula activation.
In conclusion, the results of this study indicate that use of CR for emotion regulation is associated with activation of multiple brain regions including the prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, parietal cortex, parahippocampal gyrus and occipital cortex. On the other hand, the use of ES for emotional regulation was associated with activation of various brain regions including the prefrontal cortex, parietal cortex, parahippocampal gyrus, occipital cortex, temporal cortex and insula. Node efficiency or functional connectivity of these brain regions appears to be a suitable indicator for assessing the effects of the ES and CR emotion regulation strategies.

Keywords: emotion regulation ; cognitive reappraisal ; expressive suppression ; functional connection ; graph theory

PDF (2059KB) 元数据 多维度评价 相关文章 导出 EndNote| Ris| Bibtex  收藏本文

本文引用格式

孙岩, 薄思雨, 吕娇娇. 认知重评和表达抑制情绪调节策略的脑网络分析:来自EEG和ERP的证据. 心理学报[J], 2020, 52(1): 12-25 doi:10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.00012

SUN Yan, BO Siyu, LV Jiaojiao. Brain network analysis of cognitive reappraisal and expressive inhibition strategies: Evidence from EEG and ERP. Acta Psychologica Sinica[J], 2020, 52(1): 12-25 doi:10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.00012

1 引言

有效的情绪调节是情商的核心。情绪调节是指个体对情绪的发生、体验与表达施加影响的过程(Gross, 1998)。人类每天都在有意识或无意识的对情绪进行调节(Gross, 1998), 以求适应环境的要求。Gross提出以下5种情绪调节策略, 包括情景选择、情景修正、注意分配、认知重评和表达抑制(Gross, 1998)。其中认知重评(cognitive reappraisal, CR)和表达抑制(expressive suppression, ES)是两种常见的情绪调节策略(娄熠雪, 蔡阿燕, 杨洁敏, 袁加锦, 2014)。CR是一种先行关注(antecedent- focused)的策略, 是指对情绪事件的重新解释, 改变情绪反应的轨迹; 而ES则是一种反应关注(response-focused)的策略, 是指抑制与情绪反应相关的行为(Goldin, McRae, Ramel, & Gross, 2008)。例如, 个体可以说服自己这是为了让自己得到进步获得更好发展的一种激励, 这种策略是CR; 当个体被严厉的批评后感到愤怒时, 会对愤怒的情绪进行抑制, 这种策略是ES。

近几年, 随着神经生理手段的发展, 为了探索两种情绪调节策略之间差异的根源以及两种策略之间思维方式的神经相关性。一部分研究是通过对负性情绪图片或电影片段进行CR和ES时的神经活动变化, 探索两种情绪调节策略之间的异同。结果发现, 两种情绪调节策略的使用均与前额叶皮质系统的神经活动高度相关, 包括背外侧前额叶皮层(dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, DLPFC), 前扣带皮层(dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, dACC)和腹内侧前额叶皮层(ventromedial prefrontal cortex, VMPFC) (Giuliani, Drabant, & Gross, 2011b; Hermann, Bieber, Keck, Vaitl, & Stark, 2014; Ochsner, Bunge, Gross, & Gabrieli, 2002; Ohira et al., 2006; Phan et al., 2005)。而与两种情绪调节策略相关脑区不一致的是, 许多功能成像研究显示背内、外侧前额叶皮质(prefrontal cortex, PFC)、腹外侧PFC、脑岛、颞叶、顶叶和杏仁核参与CR (Buhle et al., 2014; Gross, 2015; Hermann et al., 2014); 腹外侧前额叶皮质、额下回、脑岛和杏仁核参与ES (Goldin et al., 2008; Lee, Dolan, & Critchley, 2008)。

除此之外也有研究探讨情绪调节的个体差异, 即CR和ES的习惯性使用与大脑结构和功能的关系, 通常将脑区的激活和灰质体积与情绪调节问卷做相关分析。结果发现, CR与背外侧前额叶皮层、腹内侧PFC、背侧前扣带皮层和杏仁核的结构变化有关(Giuliani et al., 2011b; Hermann et al., 2014; 2013; Scult, Knodt, Swartz, Brigidi, & Hariri, 2017), 而腹内侧PFC、背侧前扣带皮层、背内侧PFC、额上回和脑岛的结构变化与ES相关(Giuliani, Drabant, Bhatnagar, & Gross, 2011a; Hermann et al., 2014; Li et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2017)。另外, 习惯性使用CR与杏仁核激活减少有关(Drabant, Mcrae, Manuck, Hariri, & Gross, 2008), ES与前额叶和顶叶激活增加有关(Pan et al., 2018)。此外, 还发现CR的习惯性使用与dACC体积呈正相关(Giuliani et al., 2011b)。习惯性使用ES与内侧前额叶皮质激活减少有关(Hermann et al., 2014), 并与脑岛体积之间呈正相关关系(Giuliani et al, 2011a)。最近有研究基于图论分析发现ES与前额网络和默认模式网络的全局效率显著相关(Pan et al., 2018)。该发现增强了对情绪调节神经机制的理解, 揭示了ES与脑网络的有效组织之间的关系。

综上所述, 我们发现, CR和ES这两种情绪调节策略的神经机制既有相似性又各具差异性。尽管通过这两种策略的研究已经发现了前额叶皮质、前扣带回、脑岛等脑区在情绪调节中的重要作用, 但是未发现有文献对情绪调节的认知过程与大脑静息状态EEG活动之间的关系进行研究。因此, 为了探索两种情绪调节策略之间的自发脑网络特征, 本研究尝试从时间序列角度, 进一步丰富两种策略脑机制的异同。另外, 认知神经科学研究和实践依赖于认知任务测量作为大脑功能的指标, 可以用事件相关电位(event related potential, ERP)评估刺激-反应过程中的神经活动。但是这些认知过程与大脑静息状态EEG活动之间的关系是不清楚的。近年来已经有研究发现这种静息活动可以预测行为结果, 揭示大脑内在活动对反应输出的功能相关性(Karamacoska, Barry, & Steiner, 2017; Karamacoska, Barry, Steiner, Coleman, & Wilson, 2018)。为了增加结果的可靠性, 本研究将深入探讨CR和ES的神经活动变化, 及其与大脑内在网络特征的关系。基于此, 本研究假设两种情绪调节都会涉及与情绪认知控制相关的脑区, 但与两种情绪调节神经活动相关的功能脑区也有显著差异, 这种差异与它们不同的情绪调节效果显著相关。

以往神经影像学研究表明, 由于人类大脑是一个高度整合和相互协作的复杂系统, 在这个系统中, 即使在静息状态, 信息也会不断地被整合, 这种正在进行的信息整合使我们能够评估周围的世界, 并迅速和灵活地对复杂情况作出反应(van den Heuvel, Stam, Kahn, & Hulshoff Pol, 2009), 探索大脑区域之间相互整合的静息状态, 有助于我们理解功能脑网络的基础架构以及大脑区域之间传递信息的方式(van den Heuvel & Hulshoff Pol, 2010)。目前人们越来越关注基于图论的分析来研究大脑的结构和功能, 这种分析认为大脑是由节点(大脑区域)和边(区域之间的结构或功能连接)组成的复杂网络; 复杂网络的拓扑性质可以通过各种测量来评估, 其中网络效率表示大脑区域之间信息交换效率的一种度量标准, 可用于衡量大脑网络的全局功能(Pan et al., 2018), 效率是更具生物学相关性的度量(Wang, Zuo, & He, 2010)。本研究的目标是探讨与两种情绪调节策略相关的自发大脑网络之间的信息交换效率, 以及它们之间的差异性, 因此选择节点效率作为探讨两种情绪调节策略的度量指标。

脑电图(electrophysiological, EEG)的时间序列数据与大脑皮质中动态突触后活动(dynamic postsynaptic activity)直接相关, 可以直接测量神经元活动, 具有较高的时间分辨率(Canuet et al., 2011)。并且EEG可以评估快速和滞后的自发性大脑活动的时间和频率, 从而能够表征内在的功能脑网络(Pei, Wang, Deng, Wei, & Yu, 2014; van Diessen et al., 2015)。另外, 大脑采用不同频率的同步活动来实现其功能, 根据以往的研究发现在情绪调节中, 相关的EEG信息主要在低频带编码, 特别是在theta范围内(Balconi, Grippa, & Vanutelli, 2015; Ertl, Hildebrandt, Ourina, Leicht, & Mulert, 2013; Qian et al., 2014; Xing et al., 2016), 该频带是神经系统中功能信号的重要成分之一(Zhang et al., 2013), 它与注意力和情绪有关(Balconi et al., 2015), 能反映情绪调节神经机制(Zhang et al., 2013)。因此本研究选择theta频带探讨两种情绪调节自发脑功能活动以及它们的网络效率。

最近已有研究使用功能性核磁共振(functional magnetic resonance imaging, fMRI)和情绪调节问卷进行研究, 结果发现额上回与包括内侧PFC、楔前叶和海马旁回在内的区域之间的功能连接与ES的性别差异有关(Wang et al., 2017)。功能连接是指在不同大脑区域测得的神经生理指标的时间相关性(Nallasamy & Tsao, 2011)。为了揭示与认知重评和表达抑制相关的功能网络的协同工作方式, 本研究采用连通性分析找到与两种情绪调节心理估量相关脑区的功能连接, 分析比较两者之间的异同, 将帮助我们了解两种情绪调节策略的自发脑机制, 从而将神经机制与个人的情感体验、社会行为相结合, 有助于临床实践和心理健康的干预。同时使用图论中的节点效率来度量与两种情绪调节相关脑区的网络效率, 并分析其随着情绪调节策略的不同其心理估量的变化趋势, 有助于明确CR和ES调节效率的生物标记脑区。LPP一般开始于刺激呈现后约300 ms, 并能在整个刺激呈现期间持续(王敬欣, 王春梅, 谢芳, 常敏, 张阔, 2015)。随着CR和ES认知情绪调节策略的使用, LPP波幅减少(Dennis & Hajcak, 2009; Foti & Hajcak, 2008; Hajcak & Nieuwenhuis, 2006; Moser, Hajcak, Bukay, & Simons, 2006)。因此本研究使用两种情绪调节状态下的LPP 波幅, 进一步探讨情绪调节策略与大脑内在的功能连接和节点效率之间的关系。

2 材料和方法

2.1 被试

共招募41名在校大学生有偿参加实验。实验在一个隔音良好的房间进行, 被试舒适地坐在椅子上。本研究是被试内设计, 在实验开始前向被试说明实验要求, 采集每位被试的静息EEG数据, 然后每位被试参加图片观看任务。其中3名被试因数据缺失被删除, 有2名被试由于脑电数据伪迹较多被删除。最后36名有效被试(男17人, 女19人, 年龄范围17~28岁, 平均年龄21.8 ± 2.5岁)纳入随后的数据分析中。所有被试自我报告无情感障碍历史和使用精神药物的情况, 均为右利手, 视力正常或矫正视力正常。本研究得到伦理审查委员会的批准, 并且每名被试在实验前签署知情同意书。

2.2 研究工具和程序

2.2.1 情绪调节问卷

采用Gross和John (2003)编制的情绪调节问卷, 该问卷旨在评估个体习惯性使用CR和ES两种情绪调节策略的水平。问卷包括10个项目, 其中项目1、3、5、7、8和10测量认知重评; 项目2、4、6和9测量表达抑制。问卷为7点Likert量表, 要求被试根据自身的情况从1 (完全不同意)到7 (完全同意)对每个项目进行评分。该问卷中文版信效度良好。问卷CR维度的内部一致性信度为0.85, 重测信度为0.82; ES维度的内部一致性信度为0.77, 重测信度为0.79 (王力, 柳恒超, 李中权, 杜卫, 2007)。问卷已经被广泛使用(崔向军, 逯春洁, 郭永芳, 石贺敏, 2012; 李中权, 王力, 张厚粲, 柳恒超, 2010)。

2.2.2 实验刺激和任务

本研究采用block设计的图片观看任务。任务有4种类型:观看(中性图片), 观看(负性图片), CR (负性图片)和ES (负性图片)。对于观看的任务, 要求被试简单地观看所呈现的图片, 使他们体验自身的想法和感受。对于CR任务, 要求被试重新解释负性图片的含义, 为其赋予更加积极的意义或者以独立的观察者视角看待负性图片。对于ES任务, 要求被试尽量抑制当前或即将发生的情绪波动。

本实验采用伪随机依次呈现这4种类型的图片, 本研究的刺激任务包括4个block, 每个block包括40张图片。第一个block由中性图片组成, 作为一个无情绪的基线水平, 以检验后面条件的情绪效应(观看中性)。在第二个block中, 被试在不使用任何情绪调节策略的情况下观看40张负性图片(观看负性)。最后两个block分别要求被试在使用CR (重评负性)或ES (抑制负性)策略的情况下观看40张负性图片。所有图片均选自中国情绪图片系统(Chinese Affective Picture System, CAPS) (甘甜, 罗跃嘉, 张志杰, 2009)。CR条件和ES条件的顺序在被试间平衡, 从而避免负性情绪图片对中性刺激的影响, 以及采用两种情绪调节策略先后顺序可能造成的误差。负性图片包括令人惊恐的动物、人类的攻击行为和身体残缺的场景, 而中性图片则描述了中性的动物和人活动的场景。

2.2.3 实验程序

采用E-Prime 2.0专业软件编制实验程序, 所有词汇通过电脑屏幕呈现, 根据指导语的不同, 被试做出相应的反应。每个trial开始时, 在黑色屏幕中央呈现白色注视点, 持续1000 ms, 然后出现另一个1000 ms的指令提示(简单观看, 认知重评, 表达抑制), 提示词消失后, 在黑色电脑屏幕背景上呈现2000 ms的图片。图片呈现期间, 要求被试按照指令认真观看每一张图片。为了帮助被试在不同的任务类型之间有效切换, 根据指令类型对提示屏幕的背景进行颜色编码。每种任务类型的颜色如下:紫色表示简单观看, 蓝色表示认知重评, 红色表示表达抑制。在每张图片消失后, 被试对情绪效价和唤醒度进行1~9等级评分(Bradley & Lang, 1994)。实验过程中平均每20个trial被试休息一次, 准备好后继续进行实验。实验过程如图1所示。

图1

图1   情绪调节任务的实验过程(以表达抑制任务为例)


2.3 EEG记录和预处理

采用德国Brain-Product公司的ERP记录与分析系统, 按照10-20国际脑电记录系统的64导电极帽收集EEG信号。记录信号时, AFz电极点为接地电极, FCz为参考电极。右眼下方安置电极记录垂直眼电(VEOG), 滤波带宽为0.01~100 Hz, A/D采样频率为500 Hz/导, 每个电极点电阻低于10 KΩ。要求被试舒适地坐在椅子上, 闭眼保持清醒, 同时记录静息脑电波6分钟。用Brain Vision Analyzer 2.0软件离线分析EEG数据。数据重参考采用参考电极标准技术(REST)的无限零参考, 采样率降至256 Hz。使用0.1 Hz的高通滤波器和50 Hz的低通滤波器对EEG数据进行滤波, 采用独立成分分析剔除眼电成分, 然后将EEG数据按照每2秒为一段进行分段, 电压超过±150 μV的电极信号被排除在外以减少伪迹。选择无伪迹的EEG数据用于后续分析。

2.4 功能连接分析

为了进一步确定与情绪调节功能相关的连通性脑区, 使用标准化的低分辨率电子断层扫描软件(standardized Low Resolution Brain Electromagnetic Tomography, sLORETA) (Pascual-Marqui et al., 2011)对预处理后的头皮脑电波进行源定位和连通性分析, 具有较好的定位效果(Wagner, Fuchs, & Kastner, 2004)。本研究采用sLORETA进行源定位分析, 通过计算相位滞后指数(phase lag index, PLI)进行连通性分析, 在空间无偏的滞后相位同步分析中, 根据蒙特利尔神经学研究所(Montreal Neurological Institute, MNI)标准脑坐标系和布鲁德曼分区定位, 通过源定位将64个电极位置的MNI坐标定义成84个感兴趣区域作为节点。使用PLI可以解决容积传导和有源参考电极在功能连接性评估中的问题(Stam, Nolte, & Daffertshofer, 2007)。根据下面公式从时间序列的相位差Δφ(tk), k = 1…N中获得PLI:

PLI=|<sign[sin(Δϕ(tk))]>|

PLI的范围在0到1之间, 两个节点的PLI越高, 两个节点对应的大脑区域之间的相关性越强。PLI可有效检测功能网络的实际变化(Canuet et al., 2012; Fraga González et al., 2016; Pagani et al., 2012)。我们通过计算theta频带(4~8 Hz)的84个感兴趣区域之间的PLI值, 为每位被试构建了无向加权的脑网络。

2.5 图论分析

图论中, 图或网络是现实世界复杂系统的数学表达, 效率是图论分析中表示大脑区域之间信息交换效率的一种度量(Pan et al., 2018)。本研究为了获得两种情绪调节显著相关脑区的节点效率, 在MATLAB中采用GRETNA (graph theoretical network analysis)软件进行图论分析(Wang et al., 2015)。通过相位滞后同步得到84×84功能连接矩阵后, 应用阈值从相关矩阵计算邻接矩阵, 得到二进制无向图。由于不同的阈值会生成不同连接密度或稀疏度的图形, 因此一般建议在较宽的阈值范围内计算网络属性。本研究选择的阈值范围是0.15~0.85 (Arnold, Protzner, Bray, Levy, & Iaria, 2014; Langer et al., 2012)。

2.6 ERP数据处理

对任务脑电数据进行离线分析, 设置低通滤波为24 Hz, 采用独立成分分析去除眼电成分。选取波段为-200~2000 ms, 基线为刺激呈现前200 ms, 进行基线校正后去伪迹。波幅超过± 100 μV者视为伪迹自动剔除。对收集的脑电数据进行预处理后, 分别对每个条件下的ERP活动进行叠加平均。根据先前研究和本研究的目的, 选取额叶(F3, Fz, F4), 中央(C3, Cz, C4)和顶叶(P3, Pz, P4)的9个电极点进行统计分析(Amrhein, Mühlberger, Pauli, & Wiedemann, 2004; Langeslag, Jansma, Franken, & Strien, 2007; Langeslag & van Strien, 2017; 王一牛, 周立明, 曲琛, 罗跃嘉, 2007)。

2.7 相关分析

为了得到与认知重评和表达抑制相关的功能连接和网络效率, 并进一步探讨在两种情绪调节策略下的情绪调节效果与静息状态下脑网络自发活动的关系, 将每位被试theta频带下的84个布鲁德曼脑区的节点效率, 以及每个布鲁德曼脑区对之间的相位滞后同步值, 分别与情绪调节两个分量表评分以及LPP波幅做相关分析。为了消除性别和年龄的影响, 将性别和年龄作为协变量(Wang et al., 2017), 所有结果均进行多重检验Bonferroni校正。相关分析使用GRETNA完成。

3 结果

3.1 问卷结果

情绪调节问卷的两个分量表的平均得分为:CR为30.14 (SD = 4.20), ES为15.63 (SD = 3.24)。CR和ES的评分在统计学上相关不显著(r = -0.126, p = 0.47)。

3.2 功能连接

对每位被试构建一个84×84的功能连接矩阵, 共7056个相位滞后指数值与情绪调节的两个分量表得分作相关, 计算theta频带下与CR和ES相关脑区之间的功能连接, 并比较两种情绪调节策略之间的异同。我们将所有结果进行多重比较Bonferroni校正, 并且选择显著性p < 0.001的结果, 见表1。为了将与CR和ES相关的功能连接脑区更清晰地呈现出来, 使用BrainNet Viewer画出脑图, 如图2所示。

表1   theta频带下相位滞后同步与CR和ES评分相关显著的功能连接脑区

情绪调节策略脑区rp
CR顶叶(中央后回)-额叶(额下回/额中回) (1R-46R)0.63***0.00049
ES海马旁回-额下回/额中回/脑岛(35L-47R)0.61***0.00012
海马旁回-顶叶(顶下回) (37L-40L)0.60***0.00014
额下回/额中回/脑岛-海马旁回(47L-36R)0.56***0.00046
海马旁回-额下回/额中回/脑岛(28L-47R)0.55***0.00063
额下回/额中回/脑岛-海马旁回(47L-35R)0.54***0.00082

注:***p < 0.001, Bonferroni校正。

新窗口打开| 下载CSV


图2

图2   theta频带下相位滞后同步与CR和ES心理估计变量(即情绪调节两个分量表评分)相关显著的功能连接脑区(p < 0.001, Bonferroni校正)。左图表示在theta频带下EEG信号与被试的CR得分相关显著的功能连接脑区; 右图表示在该频带下EEG信号与被试的ES得分相关显著的功能连接脑区。红色线表示连接的两个布鲁德曼脑区之间功能上的同步性与情绪调节得分呈显著正相关。


3.3 节点效率

GRETNA相关性分析发现, theta频带的节点效率存在与情绪调节分量表评分显著相关的脑区, 结果见表2, 为了更清晰地呈现出与CR和ES显著相关的节点, 使用BrainNet Viewer画出脑图, 如图3所示。

表2   theta频带下节点效率与CR和ES评分相关显著的脑区

CRES
脑区rp脑区rp
前额中央后回(4R)-0.51**0.0016额下回/额中回/脑岛(47R)0.36*0.034
顶叶中央后回(3R)-0.43*0.010前额叶皮层(6L)-0.34*0.044
前扣带回(25L)-0.35*0.041内侧前额叶皮层(11R)-0.34*0.049

注: *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, Bonferroni校正。

新窗口打开| 下载CSV


图3

图3   theta频带下节点效率与CR和ES心理估计变量(即情绪调节两个分量表评分)相关显著的脑区(p < 0.05, Bonferroni校正)。左图表示theta频带下节点效率与被试的CR得分相关显著的脑区, 右图表示theta频带下节点效率与被试的ES得分相关显著的脑区。图中圆点位置表示对应的布鲁德曼脑区, 圆点大小表示相关的显著性程度, 越大表示两者相关越显著。


3.4 LPP

所有被试叠加平均后在观看中性、观看负性、重评负性和抑制负性条件下的LPP脑电如图4所示。根据以往研究, 我们最终选取400~800 ms的LPP脑电波幅进行分析(Gu, Chen, Xing, Zhao, & Li, 2019; Shigeto, Ishiguro, & Nittono, 2011; Varnum & Hampton, 2016), 4种条件下的LPP波幅进行单因素方差分析并未发现有显著差异, F (3, 140) = 0.897, p = 0.445。

图4

图4   观看中性、观看负性、重评负性和抑制负性条件下, 在F3、Fz、F4、C3、Cz、C4、P3、Pz和P4上诱发脑电的总平均图。


GRETNA相关性分析发现, 性别和年龄作为控制变量, 通过计算每位被试在情绪调节策略下调节负性-观看中性的LPP平均波幅差异值, 分别与theta频带的脑区之间的功能连接和网络效率进行相关分析, 发现其显著脑区, 结果见表3表4图5图6

表3   LPP波幅和theta频带下相位滞后同步相关显著的脑区

CRES
脑区rp脑区rp
颞下回(20L)-额上回(8R)0.66***0.000016
海马旁回(37L)-海马旁回(37R)-0.72***0.0000014海马旁回(35L)-海马旁回(36L)0.64***0.000036
顶上回(5L)-海马旁回(34L)0.63***0.000057
额内侧回\额上回\额下回(10L)-内侧
前额叶皮层(11R)
-0.60***0.00015海马旁回(36L)-额上回(8R)0.57***0.00034
中央后回(3R)-额上回(8R)0.57***0.00038
顶上回(5L)-海马旁回(28L)0.56***0.00043
额上回(8L)-颞下回(20L)0.55***0.00060

注:***p < 0.001, Bonferroni校正。

新窗口打开| 下载CSV


表4   LPP波幅和theta频带下节点效率相关显著的脑区

CRES
脑区rp脑区rp
顶叶中央后回(1L)0.44**0.0088额上回(8R)0.37*0.031
海马旁回(37L)-0.41*0.013
枕下回(17L)-0.35*0.040枕下回(17L)-0.34*0.043

注: *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, Bonferroni校正。

新窗口打开| 下载CSV


图5

图5   LPP波幅和theta频带下相位滞后同步相关显著的脑区(p < 0.001, Bonferroni校正)。左图表示在theta频带下EEG信号与被试的CR条件下LPP波幅相关显著的功能连接脑区; 右图表示在该频带下EEG信号与被试的ES条件下LPP波幅相关显著的功能连接脑区。红色线表示连接的两个布鲁德曼脑区之间功能上的同步性与情绪调节得分呈显著正相关。蓝色线表示连接的两个布鲁德曼脑区之间功能上的同步性与情绪调节得分呈显著负相关。


图6

图6   LPP波幅和theta频带下节点效率相关显著的脑区(p < 0.05, Bonferroni校正)。左图表示theta频带下节点效率与被试的CR条件下LPP波幅相关显著的脑区, 右图表示theta频带下节点效率与被试的ES条件下LPP波幅相关显著的脑区。图中圆点位置表示对应的布鲁德曼脑区, 圆点大小表示相关的显著性程度, 越大表示两者相关越显著。


4 讨论

4.1 功能连接

为了探索CR和ES两种常用情绪调节策略的自发脑网络特征, 及与两种情绪调节策略下调节效果之间的关系。使用基于EEG的连通性和图论, 对静息状态的CR和ES大脑连接的拓扑组织以及LPP波幅进行研究。

本研究从整体上对与情绪调节相关功能性脑区的同步活动进行探讨。如图2所示, 功能连接的结果发现, 在theta频带下与CR心理测量得分相关显著的脑区有顶叶中央后回和额中回/额下回, 而与ES心理测量得分相关显著的脑区有顶下回、脑岛、海马旁回和额中回/额下回。同时, 本研究在与CR的LPP波幅显著相关的功能脑区中发现了前额叶皮质和海马旁回, 而与ES的LPP波幅显著相关的功能脑区中发现了颞下回、海马旁回、顶上回和额上回, 如图5所示。这些结果对静息EEG结果进行了补充和验证。同时可以揭示大脑内在活动对反应输出的功能相关性(Karamacoska et al., 2017; Karamacoska et al., 2018)。这些相关脑区之间的功能连接强弱可能有助于揭示CR和ES情绪调节策略使用的效果。

综上所述, 本研究发现两种策略涉及的脑区有较多一致性, 包括顶叶、前额叶皮质和海马旁回。本研究结果与以往研究结果一致, 前额叶皮质、顶叶和海马旁回与CR有关(Deak et al., 2017; Duncan & Owen, 2000; Miller & Cohen, 2001; Nelson, Fitzgerald, Klumpp, Shankman, & Phan, 2015; Vanderhasselt, Kuhn, & De Raedt, 2013); 前额叶皮质、海马旁回和顶叶与ES有关(Hermann et al., 2014; Li et al., 2017; Vanderhasselt et al., 2013; Wang et al., 2017)。有研究发现前额叶皮质可以与顶叶(与注意力定向相关的皮层区域)一起传达和处理与动机优先或行为相关的信息输入, 编码情绪信息的个体差异与前额叶皮质和顶叶区域的调节显著相关(Viviani, 2014)。本研究结果表明, 如果个体习惯使用CR和ES情绪调节策略, 则其前额叶皮质和顶叶之间的功能连接可能会增强, 其编码情绪信息的能力可能也会提升。海马旁回主要功能与记忆处理有关, 特别是情景记忆的编码和检索(Frank et al., 2014; Hamann, 2001)。Deak等人(2017)的研究发现, 在CR过程中, 海马旁回的活动增强表明, 人们利用语义知觉过程来构建对同一情境的不同解释, 并增强对情感刺激的记忆。此外, 人们似乎拥有强烈的意识, 使他们把注意力从外部信息转移到内部信息, 以检测环境变化。这些认知过程可以使他们迅速调整自己的行为。在ES中, 个体更倾向于抑制自身的外在情感表达和回忆过去的经历, 有研究发现在表达抑制过程中, 额上回和海马旁回之间的功能连接增强, 当个体在压抑自己的情绪时, 会使用认知资源来控制负面经历的记忆(Wang et al., 2017)。也有研究发现前额叶皮质和海马旁回可能对陈述性记忆有独特的贡献(Krach et al., 2010; Samuelson, 2011)。这些研究结果表明, 顶叶、前额叶皮质和海马旁回之间的协同作用, 可能在情绪调节过程中扮演重要角色。

4.2 节点效率

效率是代表大脑区域之间信息交换效率的一种度量指标。本研究发现了CR和ES两种情绪调节策略的使用习惯与静息状态脑网络节点效率之间的关系。正相关表明越频繁使用CR或ES来调节个体的情绪, 该区域的节点效率即信息交换能力越强。

图3所示, 本研究在theta频带下发现CR得分与前额中央后回、顶叶中央后回和前扣带回的节点效率呈显著负相关。如图6所示, 本研究发现CR条件下的LPP波幅与海马旁回和枕下回呈显著负相关, 而与顶叶中央后回的节点效率呈显著正相关。这说明当海马旁回和枕叶的节点效率越高, 采用CR进行情绪调节时的效果可能越差; 而顶叶中央后回的节点效率越高, 采用CR进行情绪调节时的效果可能越好。

另外, 本研究在theta频带发现ES得分与额下回/额中回/脑岛的节点效率呈显著正相关, 而与前额叶皮质呈显著负相关, 如图3所示。本研究也发现ES条件下的LPP波幅与枕下回的节点效率呈显著负相关, 而与额上回的节点效率呈显著正相关, 如图6所示。这说明当枕叶的节点效率越高, 使用ES进行情绪调节时的效果可能越差; 而额上回的节点效率越高, 使用ES进行情绪调节时的效果可能越好。

本研究从整体上对情绪调节策略与网络效率相关的脑区进行探讨, 结果发现, 在theta频带下与CR和ES均相关显著的脑区是前额叶皮质和枕叶。编码情绪信息的能力与前额叶皮层显著相关(Viviani, 2014)。同时, 前额叶也是控制思维和行为的重要脑区(Egner & Hirsch, 2005), 并参与各种认知控制任务(Badre & Wagner, 2004), 有研究已发现内侧前额叶参与ES (Goldin et al., 2008; Li et al., 2017)。此外, 也有研究发现, 当个体调节负性情绪时会伴随枕叶的激活(Urbain, Sato, Pang, & Taylor, 2017)。由此可见, 前额叶皮质和枕叶可能在情绪调节过程中起到非常重要的作用。另外, 本研究发现两种情绪调节策略与对应节点效率相关不一致的脑区是海马旁回和顶叶中央后回, 这些脑区的节点效率只与CR的LPP波幅呈显著相关, 这与本研究在CR涉及的功能脑区中得到的结果一致。

4.3 综合启示

本文通过研究与情绪调节相关脑区之间的功能连接和网络效率, 发现两种情绪调节策略涉及的脑区有很多是一致的, 包括前额叶皮质、海马旁回、顶叶和枕叶。除此之外, 我们也发现两种情绪调节策略之间不一致的相关脑区, 主要表现在CR与前扣带回显著相关, ES与颞叶和脑岛显著相关。前扣带回是皮质边缘系统的一部分, 并且作为认知和情感网络的中心枢纽, 从广义上讲, 延髓前扣带回涉及情绪的隐性调控(Egner, Etkin, Gale, & Hirsch, 2008; Etkin, Egner, Peraza, Kandel, & Hirsch, 2006; Ochsner, Hughes, Robertson, Cooper, & Gabrieli, 2009)和评估功能(Bush, Luu, & Posner, 2000)。McRae等人(2010)发现在CR情绪调节过程中, 前额叶皮质和扣带回区域的活动增加。事实上, 与以往研究一致, 前扣带回区域激活在CR和情绪认知调节过程中对脑功能的检测起着重要作用(Giuliani et al., 2011b; Ohmatsu et al., 2014)。前扣带回的激活与检测反应冲突和注意力控制有关(Barch, Braver, Akbudak, & Ollinger, 2000; Braver, Barch, Gray, Molfese, & Snyder, 2001; Liu, Banich, Jacobson, & Tanabe, 2004), 可能反映了注意力向积极方向的转移(Zhao et al., 2012)。本研究结果表明, 在使用CR进行情绪调节时会激活前扣带回, 可能会更加有效地调节负性情绪, 更加有利于个体的身心健康(程利, 袁加锦, 何媛媛, 李红, 2009)。颞叶与情绪信息的加工处理有关(Batut et al., 2006; Szaflarski et al., 2014), 尤其是在情绪记忆方面(Buchanan, Tranel, & Adolphs, 2006; Dolcos, Labar, & Cabeza, 2004; Dougal, Phelps, & Davachi, 2007)。也有研究发现, 颞叶与ES有关(Goldin et al., 2008), 这可能说明在ES中也会涉及多种信息的记忆和整合。脑岛是非常稳定的脑结构, 并与总皮质体积高度相关(Makris et al., 2006)。ES是一种需要内在感受性和情感意识的情绪调节策略(Giuliani et al., 2011a), 脑岛是人们感知身体生理状态信息(即内在感受)与情绪体验整合的主要脑区(Cauda et al., 2012), 有研究发现ES可以增强脑岛的激活(Goldin et al., 2008; Pannu et al., 2010)。因此脑岛可能在ES中起重要作用(Giuliani et al., 2011a)。

另外, 我们也发现ES与额中回/额下回、顶上回、顶下回、颞下回、额上回、脑岛和海马旁回之间的功能连接都呈显著正相关。以往核磁研究发现抑郁症患者默认网络功能联结增强(Zhou et al., 2010), 默认网络包括后扣带皮层、外侧顶叶皮质/角回脾后皮质、内侧前额叶皮层、额上回、海马旁回和颞叶等(Alizadeh, Fatemizadeh, & Deevband, 2014), 可能和抑郁症状如认知控制能力、情绪调节能力下降等具有重要关系(Sheline, Price, Yan, & Mintun, 2010)。ES一般与消极的情感和社会后果有关(Butler et al., 2003; Goldin et al., 2008; Gross, 2002; Gross & John, 2003; Haga, Kraft, & Corby, 2009; Hofmann, Heering, Sawyer, & Asnaani, 2009; Langner, Epel, Matthews, Moskowitz, & Adler, 2012), Gross和John (2003)也发现, 较高的ES评分与较高的心理困扰相关。有研究发现ES与创伤后应激障碍、焦虑和抑郁症状相关(D’ Avanzato, Joormann, Siemer, & Gotlib, 2013; Dennis, 2007; Garnefski & Kraaij, 2007; Moore, Zoellner, & Mollenholt, 2008)。而本研究结果表明, 越习惯使用ES进行情绪调节的个体, 其前额叶皮质、颞叶、顶叶、脑岛和海马旁回脑区之间的功能连接可能会增强。这些结果表明, 长期使用ES进行情绪调节可能容易导致消极的情感后果, 从而对这些默认功能脑区的同步活动产生影响。这可能有助于解释习惯使用ES的个体在面对生活压力时容易产生持续的消极情绪。

尽管如此, 本研究仍是对情绪调节与复杂脑网络之间关系进行的初步探索, 仍存在一些局限性。如情绪调节中各个节点(区域)的角色可以采用更多的拓扑属性来评价, 并进一步探讨情绪调节与复杂脑网络之间的关系。未来可以进一步设计实验进行更具体的实证研究。

5 结论

本研究得到以下结论:(1)习惯使用认知重评的个体进行情绪调节时会激活前额叶皮质、前扣带回和顶叶等多个脑区, 而习惯使用表达抑制的个体进行情绪调节时会激活前额叶皮质、顶叶、脑岛和海马旁回等多个脑区。(2)与认知重评条件下的LPP波幅呈显著相关的脑区有顶叶、前额叶皮质、海马旁回和枕叶, 而与表达抑制条件下的LPP波幅呈显著相关的脑区有顶叶、前额叶皮质、海马旁回、枕叶和颞叶。这些脑区的节点效率和功能连接强度可能成为评估个体使用认知重评和表达抑制调节情绪效果的指标。

参考文献

Alizadeh A., Fatemizadeh E., & Deevband M. R . ( 2014, November)

Investigation of Brain Default Network's activation in autism spectrum disorders Using Group Independent Component Analysis

21st Iranian Conference on Biomedical Engineering (ICBME 2014), Biomedical Engineering Faculty, Amirkabir University of Technology (Tehran Polytechnic), Tehran, Iran.

[本文引用: 1]

Amrhein C., Mühlberger A., Pauli P., & Wiedemann G . ( 2004).

Modulation of event-related brain potentials during affective picture processing: A complement to startle reflex and skin conductance response?

International Journal of Psychophysiology, 54( 3), 231-240.

DOI:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2004.05.009      URL     PMID:15331214      [本文引用: 1]

The present study compared startle response, skin conductance response (SCR) and subjective variables (valence and arousal ratings, viewing time) assessed in an affective picture paradigm with simultaneously registered event-related brain potentials (ERPs) parameters such as P300 and positive slow waves (PSW). Pleasant, neutral and unpleasant pictures from the International Affective Picture System [Lang, P.J., Bradley, M.M., Cuthbert, B.N., 1999. International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical Report A-4, Center for Research in Psychophysiology. University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida] were presented for 8 s, and startle probes were delivered during picture presentation. Startle response was modulated by picture valence, and SCR by picture arousal. ERP positivity was greater for pleasant and unpleasant than for neutral pictures for the P300 amplitude and the positive slow wave (PSW). ERPs showed characteristic differences and a distinct time course for pictures of different valence categories and may deliver useful information not contained in startle response or SCR measures. The simultaneous registration of startle responses and ERPs in the affective picture paradigm seems valuable.

Arnold A. E. G. F., Protzner A. B., Bray S., Levy R. M., & Iaria G . ( 2014).

Neural network configuration and efficiency underlies individual differences in spatial orientation ability

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26( 2), 380-394.

DOI:10.1162/jocn_a_00491      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Spatial orientation is a complex cognitive process requiring the integration of information processed in a distributed system of brain regions. Current models on the neural basis of spatial orientation are based primarily on the functional role of single brain regions, with limited understanding of how interaction among these brain regions relates to behavior. In this study, we investigated two sources of variability in the neural networks that support spatial orientationnetwork configuration and efficiencyand assessed whether variability in these topological properties relates to individual differences in orientation accuracy. Participants with higher accuracy were shown to express greater activity in the right supramarginal gyrus, the right precentral cortex, and the left hippocampus, over and above a core network engaged by the whole group. Additionally, high-performing individuals had increased levels of global efficiency within a resting-state network composed of brain regions engaged during orientation and increased levels of node centrality in the right supramarginal gyrus, the right primary motor cortex, and the left hippocampus. These results indicate that individual differences in the configuration of task-related networks and their efficiency measured at rest relate to the ability to spatially orient. Our findings advance systems neuroscience models of orientation and navigation by providing insight into the role of functional integration in shaping orientation behavior.

Badre D., & Wagner A. D . ( 2004).

Selection, integration, and conflict monitoring: Assessing the nature and generality of prefrontal cognitive control mechanisms

Neuron, 41( 3), 473-487.

DOI:10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00851-1      URL     [本文引用: 1]

AbstractPrefrontal cortex (PFC) supports flexible behavior by mediating cognitive control, though the elemental forms of control supported by PFC remain a central debate. Dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC) is thought to guide response selection under conditions of response conflict or, alternatively, may refresh recently active representations within working memory. Lateral frontopolar cortex (FPC) may also adjudicate response conflict, though others propose that FPC supports higher order control processes such as subgoaling and integration. Anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is hypothesized to upregulate response selection by detecting response conflict; it remains unclear whether ACC functions generalize beyond monitoring response conflict. The present fMRI experiment directly tested these competing theories regarding the functional roles of DLPFC, FPC, and ACC. Results reveal dissociable control processes in PFC, with mid-DLPFC selectively mediating resolution of response conflict and FPC further mediating subgoaling/integration. ACC demonstrated a broad sensitivity to control demands, suggesting a generalized role in modulating cognitive control.]]>

Balconi M., Grippa E., & Vanutelli M. E . ( 2015).

What hemodynamic (FNIRs), electrophysiological (EEG) and autonomic integrated measures can tell us about emotional processing

Brain and Cognition, 95, 67-76.

DOI:10.1016/j.bandc.2015.02.001      URL     PMID:25721430      [本文引用: 2]

Due to its fast temporal evolution and its representation and integration among complex and widespread neural networks, the emotion perception process should preferably be examined by means of multimethodological approach. Indeed the indubitable vantage of acquiring both the autonomic (arousal-related) and the central (cortical-related) activities stands in the possibility to better elucidate the reciprocal interplay of the two compartments. In the present study EEG (frequency band analysis), systemic SCR and heart rate (HR) were all recorded simultaneously with hemodynamic (NIRS, Near-Infrared Spectroscopy) measurements as potential biological markers of emotions, related to both central and peripheral systems. These multiple measures were then related to the self-report correlates, that is the subjective appraisal in term of valence (positive vs. negative) and arousal (high vs. low) by using SAM rating. Twenty subjects were submitted to emotional cues processing (IAPS) when fNIRS, frequency bands (alpha, beta, delta, theta), SCR and HR were recorded. As shown by O2Hb increasing within the right hemisphere, the contribution of prefrontal cortex was elucidated, by pointing out a relevant lateralization effect (more right-PFC activity) induced by the specific valence (negative) of the emotional patterns. Secondly, EEG activity (mainly low-frequency theta and delta bands) was intrinsically associated with the cortical hemodynamic responsiveness to the negative emotional patterns, within the right side. Finally SCR increased mainly in response to negative patterns, and the autonomic behavior was related to explicit (SAM) and cortical (NIRS; EEG) activity. The intrinsic relationships between these three different levels are discussed.

Barch D., Braver T., Akbudak E., & Ollinger J . ( 2000).

Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: Effects of response modality and processing domain

Neuroimage, 11( 5), S104-S104.

DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0017635      URL     PMID:21408006      [本文引用: 1]

Neural systems underlying conflict processing have been well studied in the cognitive realm, but the extent to which these overlap with those underlying emotional conflict processing remains unclear. A novel adaptation of the AX Continuous Performance Task (AX-CPT), a stimulus-response incompatibility paradigm, was examined that permits close comparison of emotional and cognitive conflict conditions, through the use of affectively-valenced facial expressions as the response modality.

Batut A. C., Gounot D., Namer I. J., Hirsch E., Kehrli P., & Metz-Lutz M. N . ( 2006).

Neural responses associated with positive and negative emotion processing in patients with left versus right temporal lobe epilepsy

Epilepsy & Behavior, 9( 3), 415-423.

DOI:10.21101/cejph.a5765      URL     PMID:31901191      [本文引用: 1]

Alcohol use is one of the ten most common risk factors threatening global health that is avoidable (ranked fifth after smoking). It is involved in high rates of liver cirrhosis, epilepsy, hypertension, cerebrovascular and mental illness. Negative consequences of family and social drinking are also very significant. The aim of the study is to quantify the gender and age-differentiated disparities in alcohol-related mortality in the population over 16 years between the years 1996-2017 in Slovakia.

Bradley M. M., & Lang P. J . ( 1994).

Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential

Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25( 1), 49-59.

DOI:10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9      URL     PMID:7962581      [本文引用: 1]

The Self-Assessment Manikin (SAM) is a non-verbal pictorial assessment technique that directly measures the pleasure, arousal, and dominance associated with a person's affective reaction to a wide variety of stimuli. In this experiment, we compare reports of affective experience obtained using SAM, which requires only three simple judgments, to the Semantic Differential scale devised by Mehrabian and Russell (An approach to environmental psychology, 1974) which requires 18 different ratings. Subjective reports were measured to a series of pictures that varied in both affective valence and intensity. Correlations across the two rating methods were high both for reports of experienced pleasure and felt arousal. Differences obtained in the dominance dimension of the two instruments suggest that SAM may better track the personal response to an affective stimulus. SAM is an inexpensive, easy method for quickly assessing reports of affective response in many contexts.

Braver T. S., Barch D. M., Gray J. R., Molfese D. L., & Snyder A . ( 2001).

Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: Effects of frequency, inhibition and errors

Cerebral Cortex, 11( 9), 825-836.

DOI:10.1093/cercor/11.9.825      URL     PMID:11532888      [本文引用: 1]

Anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) may play a key role in cognitive control by monitoring for the occurrence of response conflict (i.e. simultaneous activation of incompatible response tendencies). Low-frequency responding might provide a minimal condition for eliciting such conflict, as a result of the need to overcome a prepotent response tendency. We predicted that ACC would be selectively engaged during low-frequency responding, irrespective of the specific task situation. To test this hypothesis, we examined ACC activity during the performance of simple choice-discrimination tasks, using rapid event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. Subjects were scanned while performing three tasks thought to tap different cognitive processes: 'Go/No-go' (response inhibition), 'oddball' (target detection), and two-alternative forced- choice (response selection). Separate conditions manipulated the frequency of relevant task events. Consistent with our hypothesis, the same ACC region was equally responsive to low-frequency events across all three tasks, but did not show differential responding when events occurred with equal frequency. Subregions of the ACC were also identified that showed heightened activity during the response inhibition condition, and on trials in which errors were committed. Task-sensitive activity was also found in right prefrontal and parietal cortex (response inhibition), left superior temporal and tempoparietal cortex (target detection), and supplementary motor area (response selection). Taken together, the results are consistent with the hypothesis that the ACC serves as a generic detector of processing conflict arising when low-frequency responses must be executed, but also leave open the possibility that further functional specialization may occur within ACC subregions.

Buchanan T. W., Tranel D., & Adolphs R . ( 2006).

Memories for emotional autobiographical events following unilateral damage to medial temporal lobe

Brain, 129( 1), 115-127.

DOI:10.1093/brain/awh672      URL     PMID:16291807      [本文引用: 1]

Abnormalities of both memory and emotion have been reported in patients with unilateral damage to the anteromedial temporal lobe, probably reflecting the functions of the amygdala and hippocampus in these processes. Emotion and memory are also known to interact: emotional experiences often leave remarkably durable autobiographical memories. To explore this interaction, and to extend prior studies to the domain of autobiographical memory, we investigated the recollection of real-life emotional events in patients with unilateral damage to the anteromedial temporal lobe. Twenty-three patients who had undergone unilateral temporal lobectomy for the treatment of epilepsy (12 left, 11 right) and 20 healthy comparison participants completed an emotional autobiographical memory test. Participants were asked to recollect their five most emotional memories from any time in their lives and then they completed a word-cued autobiographical memory task. Participants dated each memory and gave ratings on scales of pleasantness, intensity, significance, novelty, vividness and frequency of rehearsal. Left temporal lobectomy (LTL) and healthy comparison groups generated similar numbers of pleasant and unpleasant memories, whereas the right temporal lobectomy (RTL) group produced significantly fewer memories of unpleasant events (P &amp;lt; 0.01). When memories were further categorized according to pleasantness and intensity, the RTL group produced significantly fewer unpleasant/high intensity memories than the other groups (P &amp;lt; 0.01). All groups reported more memories from between the ages of 10 and 30 (the so-called autobiographical memory 'bump'). The results demonstrate a positive bias in the recollection of autobiographical memory following right-sided anteromedial temporal damage. This finding is consistent with the notion that the right, but not the left, anteromedial temporal lobe is involved in the retrieval of negatively valenced, high-intensity memories.

Buhle J. T., Silvers J. A., Wager T. D., Lopez R., Onyemekwu C., Kober H., … Ochsner. K. N . ( 2014).

Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: A meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies

Cerebral Cortex, 24( 11), 2981-2990.

DOI:10.1093/cercor/bht154      URL     [本文引用: 1]

In recent years, an explosion of neuroimaging studies has examined cognitive reappraisal, an emotion regulation strategy that involves changing the way one thinks about a stimulus in order to change its affective impact. Existing models broadly agree that reappraisal recruits frontal and parietal control regions to modulate emotional responding in the amygdala, but they offer competing visions of how this is accomplished. One view holds that control regions engage ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), an area associated with fear extinction, that in turn modulates amygdala responses. An alternative view is that control regions modulate semantic representations in lateral temporal cortex that indirectly influence emotion-related responses in the amygdala. Furthermore, while previous work has emphasized the amygdala, whether reappraisal influences other regions implicated in emotional responding remains unknown. To resolve these questions, we performed a meta-analysis of 48 neuroimaging studies of reappraisal, most involving downregulation of negative affect. Reappraisal consistently 1) activated cognitive control regions and lateral temporal cortex, but not vmPFC, and 2) modulated the bilateral amygdala, but no other brain regions. This suggests that reappraisal involves the use of cognitive control to modulate semantic representations of an emotional stimulus, and these altered representations in turn attenuate activity in the amygdala.

Bush G., Luu P., & Posner M. I . ( 2000).

Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex

Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4( 6), 215-222.

DOI:10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01483-2      URL     PMID:10827444      [本文引用: 1]

Anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is a part of the brain's limbic system. Classically, this region has been related to affect, on the basis of lesion studies in humans and in animals. In the late 1980s, neuroimaging research indicated that ACC was active in many studies of cognition. The findings from EEG studies of a focal area of negativity in scalp electrodes following an error response led to the idea that ACC might be the brain's error detection and correction device. In this article, these various findings are reviewed in relation to the idea that ACC is a part of a circuit involved in a form of attention that serves to regulate both cognitive and emotional processing. Neuroimaging studies showing that separate areas of ACC are involved in cognition and emotion are discussed and related to results showing that the error negativity is influenced by affect and motivation. In addition, the development of the emotional and cognitive roles of ACC are discussed, and how the success of this regulation in controlling responses might be correlated with cingulate size. Finally, some theories are considered about how the different subdivisions of ACC might interact with other cortical structures as a part of the circuits involved in the regulation of mental and emotional activity.

Butler E. A., Egloff B., Wlhelm F. H., Smith N. C., Erickson E. A., & Gross J. J . ( 2003).

The social consequences of expressive suppression

Emotion, 3( 1), 48-67.

DOI:10.1037/1528-3542.3.1.48      URL     PMID:12899316      [本文引用: 1]

At times, people keep their emotions from showing during social interactions. The authors' analysis suggests that such expressive suppression should disrupt communication and increase stress levels. To test this hypothesis, the authors conducted 2 studies in which unacquainted pairs of women discussed an upsetting topic. In Study 1, one member of each pair was randomly assigned to (a) suppress her emotional behavior, (b) respond naturally, or (c) cognitively reappraise in a way that reduced emotional responding. Suppression alone disrupted communication and magnified blood pressure responses in the suppressors' partners. In Study 2, suppression had a negative impact on the regulators' emotional experience and increased blood pressure in both regulators and their partners. Suppression also reduced rapport and inhibited relationship formation.

Canuet L., Ishii R., Pascual-Marqui R. D., Iwase M., Kurimoto R., Aoki Y., … Takeda M . ( 2011).

Resting-state EEG source localization and functional connectivity in schizophrenia-like psychosis of epilepsy

PLOS ONE, 6( 11), e27863.

DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0027863      URL     PMID:22125634      [本文引用: 1]

It is unclear whether, like in schizophrenia, psychosis-related disruption in connectivity between certain regions, as an index of intrinsic functional disintegration, occurs in schizophrenia-like psychosis of epilepsy (SLPE). In this study, we sought to determine abnormal patterns of resting-state EEG oscillations and functional connectivity in patients with SLPE, compared with nonpsychotic epilepsy patients, and to assess correlations with psychopathological deficits.

Canuet L., Tellado I., Couceiro V., Fraile C., Fernandez- Novoa L., Ishii R., … Cacabelos R . ( 2012).

Resting-state network disruption and APOE genotype in Alzheimer’s disease: A lagged functional connectivity study

PLOS ONE, 7( 9), e46289.

DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0046289      URL     PMID:23050006      [本文引用: 1]

The apolipoprotein E epsilon 4 (APOE-4) is associated with a genetic vulnerability to Alzheimer's disease (AD) and with AD-related abnormalities in cortical rhythms. However, it is unclear whether APOE-4 is linked to a specific pattern of intrinsic functional disintegration of the brain after the development of the disease or during its different stages. This study aimed at identifying spatial patterns and effects of APOE genotype on resting-state oscillations and functional connectivity in patients with AD, using a physiological connectivity index called &amp;quot;lagged phase synchronization&amp;quot;.

Cauda F., Costa T., Torta D. M., Sacco K., D'Agata F., & Duca S., … Vercelli A . ( 2012).

Meta-analytic clustering of the insular cortex: Characterizing the meta-analytic connectivity of the insula when involved in active tasks

Neuroimage, 62( 1), 343-355.

DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.012      URL     [本文引用: 1]

The human insula has been parcellated on the basis of resting state functional connectivity and diffusion tensor imaging. Little is known about the organization of the insula when involved in active tasks. We explored this issue using a novel meta-analytic clustering approach. We queried the BrainMap database asking for papers involving normal subjects that recorded activations in the insular cortex, retrieving 1305 papers, involving 22,872 subjects and a total of 2957 foci. Data were analyzed with several different methodologies, some of which expressly designed for this work. We used meta-analytic connectivity modeling and meta-analytic clustering of data obtained from the BrainMap database. We performed cluster analysis to subdivide the insula in areas with homogeneous connectivity, and density analysis of the activated foci using Voronoi tessellation. Our results confirm and extend previous findings obtained investigating the resting state connectivity of the anterior posterior and left right insulae. They indicate, for the first time, that some blocks of the anterior insula play the role of hubs between the anterior and the posterior insulae, as confirmed by their activation in several different paradigms. This finding supports the view that the network to which the anterior insula belongs is related to saliency detection. The insulae of both sides can be parcellated in two clusters, the anterior and the posterior: the anterior is characterized by an attentional pattern of connectivity with frontal, cingulate, parietal, cerebellar and anterior insular highly connected areas, whereas the posterior is characterized by a more local connectivity pattern with connections to sensorimotor, temporal and posterior cingulate areas. This antero-posterior subdivision, better characterized on the right side, results sharper with the connectivity based clusterization than with the behavioral based clusterization. The circuits belonging to the anterior insula are very homogeneous and their blocks in multidimensional scaling of MACM-based profiles are in central position, whereas those belonging to the posterior insula, especially on the left, are located at the periphery and sparse, thus suggesting that the posterior circuits bear a more heterogeneous connectivity. The anterior cluster is mostly activated by cognition, whereas the posterior is mostly activated by interoception, perception and emotion. (C) 2012 Elsevier Inc.

Cheng L., Yuan J. J., He Y. Y., & Li H . ( 2009).

Emotion regulation strategies: Cognitive reappraisal is more effective than expressive suppression

Advances in Psychological Science, 17( 4), 730-735.

URL     [本文引用: 1]

Gross suggested that emotion regulation is generated in emotion process. There are two commonly used emotion regulation strategies: cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression. This review compares the two strategies with respect to their effects on emotional responses and their neural underpinnings. Moreover, the impacts of the two strategies on other cognition processes are discussed. Cognitive reappraisal not only reduces subjective emotion experience but also decreases the sympathetic responses and deactivates the emotion-related brain, while it will not impair other cognition process. Despite its usefulness in reducing subjective emotional experience, expressive suppression leads to sympathetic and amygdala activations. In particular, it will disrupt other cognitive processes. Therefore, cognitive reappraisal is more effective than expressive suppression, and is better for people’s physical and mental health.

[ 程利, 袁加锦, 何媛媛, 李红 . ( 2009).

情绪调节策略: 认知重评优于表达抑制

心理科学进展, 17( 4), 730-735.]

[本文引用: 1]

Cui X. J., Lu C. J., Guo Y. F., & Shi H. M .( 2012).

The relationship between emotion regulation and depression of college students

China Journal of Health Psychology, 20( 3), 431-433.

[本文引用: 1]

[ 崔向军, 逯春洁, 郭永芳, 石贺敏 . ( 2012).

大学生情绪调节与抑郁的相关研究

中国健康心理学杂志, 20( 3), 431-433.]

[本文引用: 1]

D’ Avanzato C., Joormann J., Siemer M., & Gotlib I. H . ( 2013).

Emotion regulation in depression and anxiety: Examining diagnostic specificity and stability of strategy use

Cognitive Therapy and Research, 37( 5), 968-980.

DOI:10.1007/s10608-013-9537-0      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Many psychological disorders are characterized by difficulties in emotion regulation. It is unclear, however, whether different disorders are associated with the use of specific emotion regulation strategies, and whether these difficulties are stable characteristics that are evident even after recovery. It is also unclear whether the use of specific strategies is problematic across all disorders or whether disorders differ in how strongly strategy use is associated with symptom severity. This study investigated (1) the specificity of use of emotion regulation strategies in individuals diagnosed with current major depressive disorder (MDD), with social anxiety disorder (SAD), and in never-disordered controls (CTL); and (2) the stability of strategy use in formerly depressed participants (i.e., remitted; RMD). Path analysis was conducted to examine the relation between strategy use and symptom severity across diagnostic groups. Compared to the CTL group, participants in both clinical groups endorsed more frequent use of rumination and expressive suppression, and less frequent use of reappraisal. Specific to SAD were even higher levels of expressive suppression relative to MDD, as well as a stronger relation between rumination and anxiety levels. In contrast, specific to MDD were even higher levels of rumination and lower levels of reappraisal. Interestingly, elevated rumination, but not decreased reappraisal, was found to be a stable feature characterizing remitted depressed individuals. These results may provide insight into ways in which emotion regulation strategy use maintains psychological disorders.

Deak A., Bodrogi B., Biro B., Perlaki G., Orsi G., & Bereczkei T . ( 2017).

Machiavellian emotion regulation in a cognitive reappraisal task: An fMRI study

Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 17( 3), 528-541.

[本文引用: 2]

Dennis T. A . ( 2007).

Interactions between emotion regulation strategies and affective style: Implications for trait anxiety versus depressed mood

Motivation and Emotion, 31( 3), 200-207.

DOI:10.1007/s11031-007-9069-6      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Affective style reflecting approach and inhibition is thought to be associated in distinct ways with anxious versus depressed mood; relatively few studies, however, consider how the interaction between affective style and the strategies individuals use to regulate mood and emotion might influence these associations. Sixty-seven non-disordered adults self-reported on their use of two emotion regulation strategies (cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression), behavioral approach (BAS) and behavioral inhibition sensitivity (BIS) dimensions of affective style, and anxious and depressed mood (trait anxiety and symptoms of depression). Trait anxiety versus depressed mood was associated with unique interactive patterns of emotion regulation and affective style: enhanced use of reappraisal was linked to less depressed mood in those reporting low BAS, whereas high suppression was linked to greater trait anxiety in those reporting low BIS. The implications of findings for typical emotional processes and for clinical disorders and interventions are discussed.]]>

Dennis T. A., & Hajcak G . ( 2009).

The late positive potential: A neurophysiological marker for emotion regulation in children

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50( 11), 1373-1383.

DOI:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02168.x      URL     PMID:19754501      [本文引用: 1]

The ability to modulate emotional responses, or emotion regulation, is a key mechanism in the development of mood disruptions. Detection of a neural marker for emotion regulation thus has the potential to inform early detection and intervention for mood problems. One such neural marker may be the late positive potential (LPP), which is a scalp-recorded event-related potential reflecting facilitated attention to emotional stimuli. In adults, the LPP is reduced following use of cognitive emotion regulation strategies such as reappraisal. No studies to date have examined the LPP in relation to cognitive emotion regulation in children, and whether the LPP is related to parent-report measures of emotion regulation and mood disruptions.

Dolcos F., Labar K. S., & Cabeza R . ( 2004).

Interaction between the amygdala and the medial temporal lobe memory system predicts better memory for emotional events

Neuron, 42( 5), 855-863.

DOI:10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00289-2      URL     [本文引用: 1]

AbstractEmotional events are remembered better than neutral events possibly because the amygdala enhances the function of medial temporal lobe (MTL) memory system (modulation hypothesis). Although this hypothesis has been supported by much animal research, evidence from humans has been scarce and indirect. We investigated this issue using event-related fMRI during encoding of emotional and neutral pictures. Memory performance after scanning showed a retention advantage for emotional pictures. Successful encoding activity in the amygdala and MTL memory structures was greater and more strongly correlated for emotional than for neutral pictures. Moreover, a double dissociation was found along the longitudinal axis of the MTL memory system: activity in anterior regions predicted memory for emotional items, whereas activity in posterior regions predicted memory for neutral items. These results provide direct evidence for the modulation hypothesis in humans and reveal a functional specialization within the MTL regarding the effects of emotion on memory formation.]]>

Dougal S., Phelps E. A., & Davachi L . ( 2007).

The role of medial temporal lobe in item recognition and source recollection of emotional stimuli

Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7( 3), 233-242.

DOI:10.1002/mgg3.1050      URL     PMID:31899609      [本文引用: 1]

Fragile X mental retardation 1 (FMR1) premutation can cause developmental problems including autism spectrum disorder (ASD), social anxiety, depression, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). These problems fall under an umbrella term of Fragile X-associated Neuropsychiatric Disorders (FXAND) and is separate from Fragile X-associated Tremor/Ataxia syndrome (FXTAS), a neurodegenerative disorder.

Drabant E. M., Mcrae K., Manuck S. B., Hariri A. R., & Gross J. J . ( 2008).

Individual differences in typical reappraisal use predict amygdala and prefrontal responses

Biological Psychiatry, 65( 5), 367-373.

DOI:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.09.007      URL     PMID:18930182      [本文引用: 1]

Participants who are instructed to use reappraisal to downregulate negative emotion show decreased amygdala responses and increased prefrontal responses. However, it is not known whether individual differences in the tendency to use reappraisal manifests in similar neural responses when individuals are spontaneously confronted with negative situations. Such spontaneous emotion regulation might play an important role in normal and pathological responses to the emotional challenges of everyday life.

Duncan J., & Owen A. M ., ( 2000).

Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands

Trends in Neurosciences, 23( 10), 475-483.

DOI:10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01633-7      URL     PMID:11006464      [本文引用: 1]

Though many neuroscientific methods have been brought to bear in the search for functional specializations within prefrontal cortex, little consensus has emerged. To assess the contribution of functional neuroimaging, this article reviews patterns of frontal-lobe activation associated with a broad range of different cognitive demands, including aspects of perception, response selection, executive control, working memory, episodic memory and problem solving. The results show a striking regularity: for many demands, there is a similar recruitment of mid-dorsolateral, mid-ventrolateral and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex. Much of the remainder of frontal cortex, including most of the medial and orbital surfaces, is largely insensitive to these demands. Undoubtedly, these results provide strong evidence for regional specialization of function within prefrontal cortex. This specialization, however, takes an unexpected form: a specific frontal-lobe network that is consistently recruited for solution of diverse cognitive problems.

Egner T., Etkin A., Gale S., & Hirsch J . ( 2008).

Dissociable neural systems resolve conflict from emotional versus nonemotional distracters

Cerebral Cortex, 18( 6), 1475-1484.

DOI:10.1093/cercor/bhm179      URL     PMID:17940084      [本文引用: 1]

The human brain protects the processing of task-relevant stimuli from interference (&amp;quot;conflict&amp;quot;) by task-irrelevant stimuli via attentional biasing mechanisms. The lateral prefrontal cortex has been implicated in resolving conflict between competing stimuli by selectively enhancing task-relevant stimulus representations in sensory cortices. Conversely, recent data suggest that conflict from emotional distracters may be resolved by an alternative route, wherein the rostral anterior cingulate cortex inhibits amygdalar responsiveness to task-irrelevant emotional stimuli. Here we tested the proposal of 2 dissociable, distracter-specific conflict resolution mechanisms, by acquiring functional magnetic resonance imaging data during resolution of conflict from either nonemotional or emotional distracters. The results revealed 2 distinct circuits: a lateral prefrontal &amp;quot;cognitive control&amp;quot; system that resolved nonemotional conflict and was associated with enhanced processing of task-relevant stimuli in sensory cortices, and a rostral anterior cingulate &amp;quot;emotional control&amp;quot; system that resolved emotional conflict and was associated with decreased amygdalar responses to emotional distracters. By contrast, activations related to both emotional and nonemotional conflict monitoring were observed in a common region of the dorsal anterior cingulate. These data suggest that the neuroanatomical networks recruited to overcome conflict vary systematically with the nature of the conflict, but that they may share a common conflict-detection mechanism.

Egner T., & Hirsch J . ( 2005).

Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task- relevant information

Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1784-1790.

DOI:10.1038/nn1594      URL     PMID:16286928      [本文引用: 1]

A prominent model of how the brain regulates attention proposes that the anterior cingulate cortex monitors the occurrence of conflict between incompatible response tendencies and signals this information to a cognitive control system in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Cognitive control is thought to resolve conflict through the attentional biasing of perceptual processing, emphasizing task-relevant stimulus information. It is not known, however, whether conflict resolution is mediated by amplifying neural representations of task-relevant information, inhibiting representations of task-irrelevant information, or both. Here we manipulated trial-by-trial levels of conflict and control during a Stroop task using face stimuli, while recording hemodynamic responses from human visual cortex specialized for face processing. We show that, in response to high conflict, cognitive control mechanisms enhance performance by transiently amplifying cortical responses to task-relevant information rather than by inhibiting responses to task-irrelevant information. These results implicate attentional target-feature amplification as the primary mechanism for conflict resolution through cognitive control.

Ertl M., Hildebrandt M., Ourina K., Leicht G., & Mulert C . ( 2013).

Emotion regulation by cognitive reappraisal - The role of frontal theta oscillations

NeuroImage, 81( 11), 412-421.

DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.044      URL     PMID:23689018      [本文引用: 1]

The regulation of emotion by cognitive reappraisal has attracted a lot of attention over the last decade. Studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) revealed a wide-spread network of multiple prefrontal and sub-cortical brain regions involved in the successful decrease of negative emotions. However, less is known about the temporal dynamics and the physiological mechanisms underlying these regulation processes. Synchronization of neural oscillations in specific frequency bands plays a key-role in the long-range interaction of different brain regions and oscillatory coupling in the theta frequency range was recently identified to play an important role in the interaction of prefrontal structures, the amygdala and the hippocampus in animal models. Accordingly, we investigated the role of prefrontal theta oscillations during the cognitive reappraisal of aversive pictures in humans. We hypothesized an increase in frontal theta oscillations during emotion regulation and a relationship between frontal theta power and the subjective success of emotion regulation. EEG from 30 healthy participants was recorded while they were asked to passively watch or reappraise the content of pictures with negative content. As expected, we found a significant increase in frequencies around 4Hz at electrode Fz during the regulation condition 'decrease' compared to the 'maintain' condition (p=.006) as well as for the regulation condition 'increase' compared to the 'maintain' condition (p=.017). Additionally, the strength of theta power was positively correlated with the regulation success as reported by the participants (r=0.463, p&amp;lt;.05). The estimation of possible generators of the theta oscillations was done using standardized low resolution electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA). Results suggested the left middle/inferior frontal gyrus as a possible generator. The results of the present study are in line with previous findings of fMRI studies suggesting the same regions as part of the regulation network and provide a first direct link between the imaging based knowledge of emotion regulation and a possible physiological mechanism.

Etkin A., Egner T., Peraza D. M., Kandel E. R., & Hirsch J . ( 2006).

Resolving emotional conflict: A role for the rostral anterior cingulate cortex in modulating activity in the amygdala

Neuron, 51( 6), 871-882.

DOI:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.07.029      URL     PMID:16982430      [本文引用: 1]

Effective mental functioning requires that cognition be protected from emotional conflict due to interference by task-irrelevant emotionally salient stimuli. The neural mechanisms by which the brain detects and resolves emotional conflict are still largely unknown, however. Drawing on the classic Stroop conflict task, we developed a protocol that allowed us to dissociate the generation and monitoring of emotional conflict from its resolution. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we find that activity in the amygdala and dorsomedial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices reflects the amount of emotional conflict. By contrast, the resolution of emotional conflict is associated with activation of the rostral anterior cingulate cortex. Activation of the rostral cingulate is predicted by the amount of previous-trial conflict-related neural activity and is accompanied by a simultaneous and correlated reduction of amygdalar activity. These data suggest that emotional conflict is resolved through top-down inhibition of amygdalar activity by the rostral cingulate cortex.

Foti D., & Hajcak G . ( 2008).

Deconstructing reappraisal: Descriptions preceding arousing pictures modulate the subsequent neural response

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20 ( 6), 977-988.

DOI:10.1162/jocn.2008.20066      URL     PMID:18211235      [本文引用: 1]

The late positive potential (LPP) is a sustained positive deflection in the event-related potential that is larger following the presentation of emotional compared to neutral visual stimuli. Recent studies have indicated that the magnitude of the LPP is sensitive to emotion regulation strategies such as reappraisal, which involves generating an alternate interpretation of emotional stimuli so that they are less negative. It is unclear, however, whether reappraisal-related reductions in the LPP reflect reduced emotional processing or increased cognitive demands following reappraisal instructions. In the present study, we sought to examine whether a more or less negative description preceding the presentation of unpleasant images would similarly modulate the LPP. The LPP was recorded from 26 subjects as they viewed unpleasant and neutral International Affective Picture System images. All participants heard a brief description of the upcoming picture; prior to unpleasant images, this description was either more neutral or more negative. Following the more neutral description, the magnitude of the LPP, unpleasant ratings, and arousal ratings were all reliably reduced. These results indicate that changes in narrative are sufficient to modulate the electrocortical response to the initial viewing of emotional pictures, and are discussed in terms of recent studies on reappraisal and emotion regulation.

Fraga González G., Van der Molen M., Žarić G., Bonte M., Tijms J., Blomert L., … Van der Molen M. W . ( 2016).

Graph analysis of EEG resting state functional networks in dyslexic readers

Clinical Neurophysiology, 127( 9), 3165-3175.

DOI:10.1016/j.clinph.2016.06.023      URL     PMID:27476025      [本文引用: 1]

Neuroimaging research suggested a mixed pattern of functional connectivity abnormalities in developmental dyslexia. We examined differences in the topological properties of functional networks between 29 dyslexics and 15 typically reading controls in 3rd grade using graph analysis. Graph metrics characterize brain networks in terms of integration and segregation.

Frank D. W., Dewitt M., Hudgens-Haney M., Schaeffer D. J., Ball B. H., Schwarz N. F., … Sabatinelli D . ( 2014).

Emotion regulation: Quantitative meta-analysis of functional activation and deactivation

Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 45, 202-211.

DOI:10.1111/ejn.14671      URL     PMID:31901174      [本文引用: 1]

Maternal immune activation during pregnancy is associated with increased risk of development of schizophrenia in later life. There are sex differences in schizophrenia, particularly in terms of age of onset, course of illness and severity of symptoms. However, there is limited and inconsistent literature on sex differences in the effects of maternal immune activation on behaviour with relevance to schizophrenia. The aim of this study was therefore to investigate sex differences in the effects of maternal immune activation by treating Long Evans rats with poly(I:C) on gestational day 15. We compared adult male and female offspring on spatial working memory in the touchscreen trial-unique nonmatching-to-location task, pairwise discrimination and reversal learning, as well as on prepulse inhibition and psychotropic drug-induced locomotor hyperactivity. Male, but not female poly(I:C) offspring displayed a deficit in spatial working memory, particularly at the longer delay. Neither pairwise discrimination nor reversal learning showed an effect of poly(I:C), but female controls outperformed male controls in the reversal learning task. Significant reduction of prepulse inhibition and enhancement of acute methamphetamine-induced locomotor hyperactivity was found similarly in male and female poly(I:C) offspring. These results show that maternal immune activation induces a range of behavioural effects in the offspring, with sex-specificity in the effects of maternal immune activation on some aspects of cognition, but not psychosis-like behaviour.

Gan T., Luo Y. J., & Zhang Z. J . ( 2009).

The Influence of Emotion on Time Perception

Journal of Psychological Science, 32( 4), 836-839.

[本文引用: 1]

[ 甘甜, 罗跃嘉, 张志杰 . ( 2009).

情绪对时间知觉的影响

心理科学, 32( 4), 836-839.]

[本文引用: 1]

Garnefski N., & Kraaij V . ( 2007).

The Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire: Psychometric features and prospective relationships with depression and anxiety in adults

European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 23, 141-149.

DOI:10.1027/1015-5759.23.3.141      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Giuliani N. R., Drabant E. M., Bhatnagar R., & Gross J. J . ( 2011a).

Emotion regulation and brain plasticity: Expressive suppression use predicts anterior insula volume

Neuroimage, 58( 1), 10-15.

DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.028      URL     PMID:21704173      [本文引用: 4]

Expressive suppression is an emotion regulation strategy that requires interoceptive and emotional awareness. These processes both recruit the anterior insula. It is not known, however, whether increased use of expressive suppression is associated with increased anterior insula volume. In the present study, high-resolution anatomical MRI images were used to calculate insula volumes in a set of 50 healthy female subjects (mean 21.9 years) using both region of interest (ROI) and voxel-based morphometry (VBM) approaches. Participants also completed trait measures of expressive suppression usage, cognitive reappraisal usage, and negative emotional reactivity (the latter two served as control measures). As predicted, both ROI and VBM methods found that expressive suppression usage, but not negative affect and cognitive reappraisal, was positively related to anterior insula volume. These findings are consistent with the idea that trait patterns of emotion processing are related to brain structure.

Giuliani N. R., Drabant E. M., & Gross J. J . ( 2011b).

Anterior cingulate cortex volume and emotion regulation: Is bigger better?

Biological Psychology, 86( 3), 379-382.

DOI:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.11.010      URL     PMID:21138751      [本文引用: 4]

Emotion dysregulation is a key feature of mood and anxiety disorders. Many of these disorders also involve volumetric reductions in brain regions implicated in emotion regulation, including the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC). Investigating this relationship in healthy individuals may clarify the link between emotion regulation and volumetric reductions in this key brain region. High-resolution anatomical MRI images were used to calculate dACC volumes in 50 healthy female subjects. Trait measures of emotion regulation (cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression) and negative affect were also obtained. As predicted, cognitive reappraisal was positively related to dACC volume, but not the volume of a control region, the ventral ACC. Expressive suppression, negative affect, and age were not related to dACC volume. These findings indicate that individual differences in cognitive reappraisal are related to individual differences in dACC volume in healthy participants.

Goldin P. R., McRae K., Ramel W., & Gross J. J . ( 2008).

The neural bases of emotion regulation: Reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion

Biological Psychiatry, 63( 6), 577-586.

DOI:10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.031      URL     PMID:17888411      [本文引用: 6]

Emotion regulation strategies are thought to differ in when and how they influence the emotion-generative process. However, no study to date has directly probed the neural bases of two contrasting (e.g., cognitive versus behavioral) emotion regulation strategies. This study used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine cognitive reappraisal (a cognitive strategy thought to have its impact early in the emotion-generative process) and expressive suppression (a behavioral strategy thought to have its impact later in the emotion-generative process).

Gross J. J . ( 1998).

The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review

Review of General Psychology, 2( 3), 271-299.

DOI:10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00093      URL     PMID:26191007      [本文引用: 3]

Emerging lines of research suggest that both testosterone and maladaptive reward processing can modulate behavioral dysregulation. Yet, to date, no integrative account has been provided that systematically explains neuroendocrine function, dysregulation of reward, and behavioral dysregulation in a unified perspective. This is particularly important given specific neuroendocrine systems are potential mechanisms underlying and giving rise to reward-relevant behaviors. In this review, we propose a forward-thinking approach to study the mechanisms of reward and behavioral dysregulation from a positive affective neuroendocrinology (PANE) perspective. This approach holds that testosterone increases reward processing and motivation, which increase the likelihood of behavioral dysregulation. Additionally, the PANE framework holds that reward processing mediates the effects of testosterone on behavioral dysregulation. We also explore sources of potential sex differences and the roles of age, cortisol, and individual differences within the PANE framework. Finally, we discuss future prospects for research questions and methodology in the emerging field of affective neuroendocrinology.

Gross J. J . ( 2002).

Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences

Psychophysiology, 39( 3), 281-291.

DOI:10.1017/s0048577201393198      URL     PMID:12212647      [本文引用: 1]

One of life's great challenges is successfully regulating emotions. Do some emotion regulation strategies have more to recommend them than others? According to Gross's (1998, Review of General Psychology, 2, 271-299) process model of emotion regulation, strategies that act early in the emotion-generative process should have a different profile of consequences than strategies that act later on. This review focuses on two commonly used strategies for down-regulating emotion. The first, reappraisal, comes early in the emotion-generative process. It consists of changing the way a situation is construed so as to decrease its emotional impact. The second, suppression, comes later in the emotion-generative process. It consists of inhibiting the outward signs of inner feelings. Experimental and individual-difference studies find reappraisal is often more effective than suppression. Reappraisal decreases emotion experience and behavioral expression, and has no impact on memory. By contrast, suppression decreases behavioral expression, but fails to decrease emotion experience, and actually impairs memory. Suppression also increases physiological responding for suppressors and their social partners. This review concludes with a consideration of five important directions for future research on emotion regulation processes.

Gross J. J . ( 2015).

Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects

Psychological Inquiry, 26( 1), 1-26.

DOI:10.1080/1047840X.2014.940781      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Gross J. J., & John O.P . ( 2003).

Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85( 2), 348-362.

DOI:10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348      URL     PMID:12916575      [本文引用: 2]

Five studies tested two general hypotheses: Individuals differ in their use of emotion regulation strategies such as reappraisal and suppression, and these individual differences have implications for affect, well-being, and social relationships. Study 1 presents new measures of the habitual use of reappraisal and suppression. Study 2 examines convergent and discriminant validity. Study 3 shows that reappraisers experience and express greater positive emotion and lesser negative emotion, whereas suppressors experience and express lesser positive emotion, yet experience greater negative emotion. Study 4 indicates that using reappraisal is associated with better interpersonal functioning, whereas using suppression is associated with worse interpersonal functioning. Study 5 shows that using reappraisal is related positively to well-being, whereas using suppression is related negatively.

Gu H., Chen Q., Xing X., Zhao J., & Li X . ( 2019).

Facial emotion recognition in deaf children: Evidence from event-related potentials and event-related spectral perturbation analysis

Neuroscience Letters, 703, 198-204.

DOI:10.1016/j.neulet.2019.01.032      URL     PMID:30677434      [本文引用: 1]

Impairment of facial emotion recognition ability has been reported in deaf children, but few researches have examined the neural mechanism of this impairment. This study applied the electroencephalogram (EEG) technique to investigate the emotion recognition ability in 31 deaf children and 30 hearing controls with facial emotion recognition task. Results from behavioral task showed that deaf children exhibited lower accuracy compared to hearing controls. As for EEG analysis, results showed that deaf children showed diminished activation in the early (N1、P1), middle (N170) and late stages (late positive potential, LPP) compared to hearing controls. In addition, for brain oscillation, alpha band (400-800 ms) also revealed a reduced desynchronization in deaf children. The present findings seem to indicate that the deficit during facial emotion recognition ability among deaf children might be due to the impairments in visual processing, attention and emotional semantic distribution ability during facial emotion recognition processing.

Haga S. M., Kraft P., & Corby E. K . ( 2009).

Emotion regulation: Antecedents and well-being outcomes of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression in cross-cultural samples

Journal of Happiness Studies, 10( 3), 271-291.

DOI:10.1007/s10902-007-9080-3      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Habitual emotional state is a predictor of long-term health and life expectancy and successful emotion regulation is necessary for adaptive functioning. However, people are often unsuccessful in regulating their emotions. We investigated the use of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression in 489 university students in Norway, Australia, and the United States and how these strategies related to measures of well-being (affect, life satisfaction, and depressed mood). Data was collected by means of selfadministered questionnaires. The major aims of the study were to begin to explore the prevalence of use of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression across gender, age and culture, possible antecedents of emotion regulation strategies, and the influence of emotion regulation upon well-being. Results showed that the use of emotion regulation strategies varied across age, gender and culture. Private self-consciousness (self-reflection and insight) was found to be a central antecedent for the use of cognitive reappraisal. Use of emotion regulation strategies predicted well-being outcomes, also after the effect of extraversion and neuroticism had been controlled for. Generally, increased use of cognitive reappraisal predicted increased levels of positive well-being outcomes, while increased use of expressive suppression predicted increased levels of negative well-being outcomes.]]>

Hajcak G., & Nieuwenhuis S . ( 2006).

Reappraisal modulates the electrocortical response to unpleasant pictures

Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 6( 4), 291-297.

DOI:10.3758/cabn.6.4.291      URL     PMID:17458444      [本文引用: 1]

Cognitive strategies such as reappraisal reduce the intensity of negative experience and brain activity that is sensitive to emotional salience. The time course of reappraisal-related neural modulation remains unclear, and it is unknown whether the electrocortical response to emotional stimuli is sensitive to reappraisal. Event-related brain potentials were recorded first while participants passively viewed pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral pictures, and then during an emotion regulation block in which participants were instructed to attend to or reappraise unpleasant pictures. The late positive potential (LPP) was enhanced for pleasant and unpleasant pictures in the passive viewing block, and reappraisal resulted in a reliably reduced LPP--a protracted modulation that began 200 msec after stimulus onset. The degree of LPP modulation was positively related to reductions in the self-reported emotional intensity that followed emotion regulation instructions. These results indicate that reappraisal modulates early electrocortical activity that is related to emotional salience, and that the LPP is a useful tool for studying emotion regulation.

Hamann S., . ( 2001).

Cognitive and neural mechanisms of emotional memory

Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5( 9), 394-400.

DOI:10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01707-1      URL     PMID:11520704      [本文引用: 1]

A highly adaptive aspect of human memory is the enhancement of explicit, consciously accessible memory for emotional stimuli. Recent findings from neuroimaging, neuropsychological, drug and neural stimulation studies indicate that emotional stimuli engage specific cognitive and neural mechanisms that enhance explicit memory. Emotional arousal influences memory via factors that act during memory encoding (attention and elaboration) and factors that modulate memory consolidation. Across studies, the amygdala has been consistently implicated as playing a key role in enhancing explicit memory for both pleasant and unpleasant emotional stimuli through modulation of encoding and consolidation processes.

Hermann A., Bieber A., Keck T., Vaitl D., & Stark R . ( 2014).

Brain structural basis of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression

Social Cognitive & Affective Neuroscience, 9( 9), 1435-1442.

DOI:10.1007/s10802-019-00607-5      URL     PMID:31900836      [本文引用: 6]

There is accumulating evidence of a prospective relation between early language problems and ADHD, a disorder associated with deficits in executive functioning. However, little is known regarding this link among bilingual children. Here, we investigate whether (i) the prediction from language to ADHD may be lower among bilinguals, and (ii) explore if this moderation can be explained by differential executive functioning ability. Utilising a prospective sample of 408 South-East Asian toddlers, bilingual exposure as a moderator of the link between language delay at 24 months to ADHD intermediate diagnosis at 54 months was first examined with an interaction model. Next, structural equation mediated moderation models examined if the proposed moderation could be explained by executive function measures of Snack Delay and Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS) task, when children were 41 months. Results indicate that higher levels of bilingual exposure moderated the prospective risk of language delay to ADHD diagnosis (Predominantly single-language exposed OR = 6.37; p = .011; Predominantly dual-language exposed OR = 0.30, p = .156). Thus, language delay associated with ADHD among toddlers predominantly exposed to one but not two languages. However, this could not be explained by differential executive functioning, as this moderation was not mediated by performance on Snack Delay or DCCS. Unexpectedly, bilingual exposure associated with ADHD among toddlers of typical language development. Possible explanations, including variation in the degree of social stigma and persistence of language delay between bilingual and monolingual children, and bilingualism as an additional cognitive load for ADHD, are discussed.

Hermann A., Leutgeb V., Scharmüller W., Vaitl D., & Stark R . ( 2013).

Individual differences in cognitive reappraisal usage modulate the time course of brain activation during symptom provocation in specific phobia

Biology of Mood and Anxiety Disorders, 3( 1), 16.

DOI:10.1186/2045-5380-3-20      URL     PMID:24517388      [本文引用: 1]

Social anxiety disorder (SAD) is thought to involve deficits in emotion regulation, and more specifically, deficits in cognitive reappraisal. However, evidence for such deficits is mixed.

Hofmann S. G., Heering S., Sawyer A. T., & Asnaani A . ( 2009).

How to handle anxiety: The effects of reappraisal, acceptance, and suppression strategies on anxious arousal

Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47( 5), 389-394.

DOI:10.1016/j.brat.2009.02.010      URL     [本文引用: 1]

AbstractIt has been suggested that reappraisal strategies are more effective than suppression strategies for regulating emotions. Recently, proponents of the acceptance-based behavior therapy movement have further emphasized the importance of acceptance-based emotion regulation techniques. In order to directly compare these different emotion regulation strategies, 202 volunteers were asked to give an impromptu speech in front of a video camera. Participants were randomly assigned to one of three groups. The Reappraisal group was instructed to regulate their anxious arousal by reappraising the situation; the Suppression group was asked to suppress their anxious behaviors; and the Acceptance group was instructed to accept their anxiety. As expected, the Suppression group showed a greater increase in heart rate from baseline than the Reappraisal and Acceptance groups. Moreover, the Suppression group reported more anxiety than the Reappraisal group. However, the Acceptance and Suppression groups did not differ in their subjective anxiety response. These results suggest that both reappraising and accepting anxiety is more effective for moderating the physiological arousal than suppressing anxiety. However, reappraising is more effective for moderating the subjective feeling of anxiety than attempts to suppress or accept it.]]>

Karamacoska D., Barry R. J., & Steiner G. Z . ( 2017).

Resting state intrinsic EEG impacts on go stimulus-response processes

Psychophysiology, 54( 6), 894-903.

DOI:10.1111/psyp.12851      URL     PMID:28258583      [本文引用: 2]

Neuropsychological research and practice rely on cognitive task performance measures as indicators of brain functioning. The neural activity underlying stimulus-response processes can be assessed with ERPs, but the relations between these cognitive processes and the brain's intrinsic resting state EEG activity are less understood. This study focused on the neurocognitive functioning of 20 healthy young adults in an equiprobable go/no-go task to map the ERP correlates of behavioral responses and examine contributions of the resting state intrinsic EEG to task-related outcomes. Continuous EEG was recorded during pretask eyes-closed (EC) and eyes-open (EO) conditions, and in the subsequent task. Delta, theta, alpha, and beta band amplitudes were assessed for the EC state and also for the reactive change to EO. Go/no-go ERPs were submitted to temporal principal components analysis, where the P2, N2, P3, and slow wave components of interest were extracted. The performance measure of reaction time (RT) variability was positively correlated with no-go and go errors, and also with go P2 amplitude, linking these to stimulus discrimination efforts involved in appropriate response selection. An N2c-P3b pairing was enhanced for shorter mean RTs, supporting their involvement in the decision to execute a response. A stepwise regression model identified EC midline delta as a predictor of P3b positivity, highlighting the relevance of delta in the neural mechanisms of attentional processes. These findings clarify the electrophysiology underlying decision-making processes in executive function, and provide a platform for further research assessing performance outcomes in larger samples and in developmental/clinical contexts.

Karamacoska D., Barry R. J., Steiner G. Z., Coleman E. P., & Wilson E. J . ( 2018).

Intrinsic EEG and task-related changes in EEG affect go/nogo task performance

International Journal of Psychophysiology, S0167876017306864.

DOI:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.01.015      URL     PMID:29409782      [本文引用: 2]

Substantial research into the brain dynamics underlying cognitive functioning during tasks links the brain's EEG activity to the stimulus-evoked ERP activity. This study focused on examining how the resting state intrinsic EEG, and the change from rest to the task, affect these stimulus-response processes. Forty young adults (aged 20.3 ± 2.3 years) had EEG recorded during eyes-closed (EC) and eyes-open (EO) resting states, and then during an auditory Go/NoGo task. Amplitude in the delta to beta bands was analyzed for the overall resting state EEG, the reactive change from EC to EO, and for the change from EO to the task (termed task-related change here). The relationships between these EEG measures and Go/NoGo behavioral outcomes and ERPs were assessed. Greater resting state delta and theta amplitudes were linked to Go N1-1 enhancements, but only resting state delta correlated with the NoGo N1-1. These relationships replicate previous data and highlight the functional relevance of low frequency intrinsic activity in attentional processes. However, delta increases from EO to the task predicted poorer Go response accuracy and variability, and enhanced Go Slow Wave (SW) positivity. This increase in delta, and smaller alpha-1 increments, were associated with longer mean RTs. Theta increases predicted larger Go N1-1 amplitudes, but lower NoGo accuracy rates, while beta-1 increments were predictive of NoGo SW negativity. These novel effects suggest that task-related EEG changes impact decision-making and cognitive control processes, and subsequent behavioral performance.

Krach S., Jansen A., Krug A., Markov V., Thimm M., Sheldrick A. J., … Kircher T . ( 2010).

Comt genotype and its role on hippocampal-prefrontal regions in declarative memory

Neuroimage, 53( 3), 978-984.

DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.090      URL     PMID:20060911      [本文引用: 1]

Memory dysfunction is a prominent feature in schizophrenia. Impairments of declarative memory have been consistently linked to alterations especially within hippocampal-prefrontal regions. Due to the high heritability of schizophrenia, susceptibility genes and their modulatory impact on the neural correlates on memory are of major relevance. In the present study the influence of the COMT val(158)met status on the neural correlates of declarative memory was investigated in healthy subjects.

Samuelson K. W . ( 2011).

Post-traumatic stress disorder and declarative memory functioning: A review

Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 13( 3), 346-351.

URL     PMID:22033732      [本文引用: 1]

Declarative memory dysfunction is associated with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). This paper reviews this literature and presents two frameworks to explain the nature of this dysfunction: that memory deficits are a product of neurobiological abnormalities caused by PTSD and/or that pre-existing memory deficits serve as a risk factor for the development of PTSD following trauma exposure. Brain regions implicated in declarative memory deficits include the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex, and imaging and biochemistry studies as they relate to memory dysfunction are described. Prospective and twin studies provide support for a risk factor model.

Langer N., Pedroni A., Gianotti L. R. R., Hänggi J., Knoch D., & Jäncke L . ( 2012).

Functional brain network efficiency predicts intelligence

Human Brain Mapping, 33( 6), 1393-1406.

DOI:10.1002/hbm.21297      URL     [本文引用: 1]

The neuronal causes of individual differences in mental abilities such as intelligence are complex and profoundly important. Understanding these abilities has the potential to facilitate their enhancement. The purpose of this study was to identify the functional brain network characteristics and their relation to psychometric intelligence. In particular, we examined whether the functional network exhibits efficient small-world network attributes (high clustering and short path length) and whether these small-world network parameters are associated with intellectual performance. High-density resting state electroencephalography (EEG) was recorded in 74 healthy subjects to analyze graph-theoretical functional network characteristics at an intracortical level. Ravens advanced progressive matrices were used to assess intelligence. We found that the clustering coefficient and path length of the functional network are strongly related to intelligence. Thus, the more intelligent the subjects are the more the functional brain network resembles a small-world network. We further identified the parietal cortex as a main hub of this resting state network as indicated by increased degree centrality that is associated with higher intelligence. Taken together, this is the first study that substantiates the neural efficiency hypothesis as well as the Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence in the context of functional brain network characteristics. These theories are currently the most established intelligence theories in neuroscience. Our findings revealed robust evidence of an efficiently organized resting state functional brain network for highly productive cognitions. Hum Brain Mapp, 2011. (c) 2011 Wiley-Liss, Inc.

Langner C. A., Epel E., Matthews K., Moskowitz J. T., & Adler N . ( 2012).

Social hierarchy and depression: The role of emotion suppression

Journal of Psychology, 146( 4), 417-436.

DOI:10.1080/00223980.2011.652234      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Position in the social hierarchy is a major determinant of health outcomes. We examined the associations between aspects of social hierarchy and depressive symptoms with a specific focus on one potential psychological mechanism: emotion suppression. Suppressing negative emotion has mental health costs, but individuals with low social power and low social status may use these strategies to avoid conflict. Study 1 assessed perceived social power, tendency to suppress negative emotion, and depressive symptoms in a community sample of women. Low social power was related to greater depressive symptoms, and this relationship was partially mediated by emotion suppression. Study 2 examined education as a proxy for social hierarchy position, anger suppression, and depressive symptoms in a national, longitudinal cohort study (The Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults [CARDIA] study; Cutter et al., 1991). Much as in Study 1, low education levels were correlated with greater depressive symptoms, and this relationship was partially mediated by anger suppression. Further, suppression mediated the relationship between low education and subsequent depression up to 15 years later. These findings support the theory that social hierarchy affects mental health in part through a process of emotion suppression.

Langeslag S. J. E., Jansma B. M., Franken I. H. A., & Strien J. W. V . ( 2007).

Event-related potential responses to love-related facial stimuli

Biological Psychology, 76( 1-2), 109-115.

DOI:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.06.007      URL     PMID:17681417      [本文引用: 1]

In event-related potential (ERPs) studies, emotional stimuli usually elicit an enhanced late positive potential (LPP), which is assumed to reflect motivated attention. However, whether a stimulus elicits emotional responses may depend on the individual's state, such as experiencing romantic love. It has been suggested that stimuli that are related to someone's beloved will elicit increased attention in that infatuated individual. In this study, participants who were in love viewed faces of their beloved, their friend, and of an unknown, beautiful person. The friend was included to control for familiarity, and the unknown person for perceived beauty. As expected, the LPP was larger in response to the face of the beloved than to the other two emotionally significant faces. Interpreting the LPP as reflecting motivated attention, this implies that romantic love is accompanied by increased attention for the face of one's beloved.

Langeslag S. J. E., & van Strien J. W . ( 2017).

Preferential processing of task-irrelevant beloved-related information and task performance: Two event-related potential studies

Neuropsychologia, S002839321730341X.

DOI:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107328      URL     PMID:31887313      [本文引用: 1]

Why do we sometimes easily retrieve memories, but other times appear to forget them? We often look to our external environment for retrieval cues, but another way to optimize memory retrieval is to be in a mental state, or mode, that prioritizes access to our internal representation of the world. Such a 'retrieval mode' was proposed by Endel Tulving (1983), who considered it a neurocognitive state in which one keeps the goal of memory retrieval in mind. Building on Tulving's proposal, we review converging evidence from multiple lines of research that emphasize the importance of internal states in the instantiation of retrieval modes that optimize successful remembering. We identify three key factors that contribute to a retrieval mode by modulating either the likelihood or the content of retrieval: (1) an intention to remember or forget (either in the present or the future), (2) attentional selection of goal-relevant memories and suppression of distractors, and (3) fluctuating levels of acetylcholine in the hippocampus. We discuss empirical evidence that these internal states individually influence memory retrieval and propose how they may interact synergistically. Characterizing these dynamic internal factors is an important key for unlocking our understanding of the organization and accessibility of our memories.

Lee T. W., Dolan R. J., & Critchley H. D . ( 2008).

Controlling emotional expression: Behavioral and neural correlates of nonimitative emotional responses

Cerebral Cortex, 18( 1), 104-113.

DOI:10.1093/cercor/bhm035      URL     PMID:17483530      [本文引用: 1]

Emotional facial expressions can engender similar expressions in others. However, adaptive social and motivational behavior can require individuals to suppress, conceal, or override prepotent imitative responses. We predicted, in line with a theory of &amp;quot;emotion contagion,&amp;quot; that when viewing a facial expression, expressing a different emotion would manifest as behavioral conflict and interference. We employed facial electromyography (EMG) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate brain activity related to this emotion expression interference (EEI) effect, where the expressed response was either concordant or discordant with the observed emotion. The Simon task was included as a nonemotional comparison for the fMRI study. Facilitation and interference effects were observed in the latency of facial EMG responses. Neuroimaging revealed activation of distributed brain regions including anterior right inferior frontal gyrus (brain area [BA] 47), supplementary motor area (facial area), posterior superior temporal sulcus (STS), and right anterior insula during emotion expression-associated interference. In contrast, nonemotional response conflict (Simon task) engaged a distinct frontostriatal network. Individual differences in empathy and emotion regulatory tendency predicted the magnitude of EEI-evoked regional activity with BA 47 and STS. Our findings point to these regions as providing a putative neural substrate underpinning a crucial adaptive aspect of social/emotional behavior.

Li X., Lu J., Li B., Li H., Jin L., & Qiu J . ( 2017).

The role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex volume in the association of expressive suppression and externally oriented thinking

Journal of Affective Disorders, 222, 112-119.

DOI:10.1016/j.jad.2017.06.054      URL     PMID:28688264      [本文引用: 3]

Studies have suggested that expressive suppression (ES) is linked to externally oriented thinking (EOT) through the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), and there are gender differences in their association. The present structural magnetic resonance imaging study was to investigate the neural bases of ES and EOT and their association in females versus males in a Chinese college sample.

Li Z. Q., Wang L., Zhang H. C., & Liu H. C . ( 2010).

Personality traits and subjective well-being: The mediating role of emotion regulation

Journal of Psychological Science, 33( 1), 165-167.

[本文引用: 1]

[ 李中权, 王力, 张厚粲, 柳恒超 . ( 2010).

人格特质与主观幸福感: 情绪调节的中介作用

心理科学, 33( 1), 165-167.]

[本文引用: 1]

Liu X., Banich M. T., Jacobson B. L., & Tanabe J. L . ( 2004).

Common and distinct neural substrates of attentional control in an integrated Simon and spatial Stroop task as assessed by event-related fMRI

Neuroimage, 22( 3), 1097-1106.

DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.02.033      URL     [本文引用: 1]

AbstractThe purpose of this experiment was to directly examine the neural mechanisms of attentional control involved in the Simon task as compared to a spatial Stroop task using event-related fMRI. The Simon effect typically refers to the interference people experience when there is a stimulus–response conflict. The Stroop effect refers to the interference people experience when two attributes of the same stimulus conflict with each other. Although previous imaging studies have compared the brain activation for each of these tasks performed separately, none had done so in an integrated task that incorporates both types of interference, as was done in the current experiment. Both tasks activated brain regions that serve as a source of attentional control (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) and posterior regions that are sites of attentional control (the visual processing stream–middle occipital and inferior temporal cortices). In addition, there were also specific brain regions activated to a significantly greater degree by one task and/or only by a single task. The brain regions significantly more activated by the Simon task were those sensitive to detection of response conflict, response selection, and planning (anterior cingulate cortex, supplementary motor areas, and precuneus), and visuospatial–motor association areas. In contrast, the regions significantly more activated by the Stroop task were those involved in biasing the processing toward the task-relevant attribute (inferior parietal cortex). These findings suggest that the interference effects of these two tasks are caused by different types of conflict (stimulus–response conflict for the Simon effect and stimulus–stimulus conflict for the Stroop effect) but both invoke similar sources of top-down modulation.]]>

Lou Y. X., Cai A. Y., Yang J. M., & Yuan J. J . ( 2014).

The impact of introversion-extraversion on emotion regulations and the neurophysiological underpinnings

Advances in Psychological Science, 22( 12), 1855-1866.

DOI:10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.01855      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Extraversion is closely linked with emotional activity: it not only affects people&rsquo;s adaptive capacity and subjective well-being, but it&rsquo;s also closely associated with emotional and behavioral disorders such as anxiety and impulsiveness. Prior studies focused on the influences of extraversion in sensitivity to emotional stimuli. Participants&rsquo; positive emotional responses to reward stimuli increase significantly with higher extraversion scoring. However, we hypothesize that the links between extraversion and emotion may also be manifested by the impacts of extraversion on the behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of emotion regulation. Therefore, we are going to manipulate two variables &ndash; extraversion and emotion regulation strategies &ndash; to test these hypotheses. Accordingly, it is theoretically and practically meaningful to address the following three scientific questions using behavioral/physiological, EEG, and fMRI measures: 1), developing an emotion regulation questionnaire inclusive of most of the common strategies, and comparing emotion regulation characteristics between extroverts and introverts; 2), exploring the impacts of extroversion on cognitive emotion regulation and the neurophysiologic underpinnings; 3) exploring brain mechanisms underlying interactive influences of emotional stability and extraversion. The ultimate goal of this series of studies is to unravel the brain mechanisms of emotion regulation differences in people with varying extraversion, to better understand the relationship between emotion and personality, and to lay theoretic and practical bases for clinical treatment of affective disturbances.

[ 娄熠雪, 蔡阿燕, 杨洁敏, 袁加锦 . ( 2014).

内-外倾人格对情绪调节的影响及神经机制

心理科学进展, 22(12), 1855-1866.]

[本文引用: 1]

Makris N., Goldstein J. M., Kennedy D., Hodge S. M., Caviness V. S., & Faraone S. V., … Seidmancdfi L. J . ( 2006).

Decreased volume of left and total anterior insular lobule in schizophrenia

Schizophrenia Research, 83( 2-3), 155-171.

DOI:10.1016/j.schres.2005.11.020      URL     PMID:16448806      [本文引用: 1]

The insula is anatomically situated to be critically involved in many bio-behavioral functions impaired in schizophrenia. Furthermore, its total volume has been shown to be reduced in schizophrenia. In the present study, we tested the hypothesis that in schizophrenia it is the anterior insular lobule (aINS(lbl)) rather than the posterior insular lobule (pINS(lbl)) that is smaller, given that limbic system abnormalities are central in schizophrenia and that the affiliations of the limbic system are principally with the anterior insular lobule. We used T1-weighted high resolution magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) to measure the cortical volume of the left and right anterior and posterior insular subdivisions. The subjects included a sample of healthy community controls (N=40) and chronic patients with DSM-III-R schizophrenia (N=41). We correlated insula volumes with positive and negative symptoms. We found that the total aINS(lbl), and the left aINS(lbl) in particular, were significantly volumetrically smaller in schizophrenia compared to controls, and significantly correlated with bizarre behavior. Given that the anterior insular lobule offers anatomic features that allow for MRI-based morphometric analysis, namely its central and circular sulci, this brain structure provides a useful model to test hypotheses regarding genotype-phenotype relationships in schizophrenia using the anterior insular lobule as a candidate endophenotype.

Mcrae K., Hughes B., Chopra S., Gabrieli J. D. E., Gross J. J., & Ochsner K. N . ( 2010).

The neural bases of distraction and reappraisal

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22( 2), 248-262.

DOI:10.1162/jocn.2009.21243      URL     PMID:19400679      [本文引用: 1]

Distraction and reappraisal are two commonly used forms of cognitive emotion regulation. Functional neuroimaging studies have shown that each one depends upon interactions between pFC, interpreted as implementing cognitive control, and limbic regions, interpreted as mediating emotional responses. However, no study has directly compared distraction with reappraisal, and it remains unclear whether they draw upon different neural mechanisms and have different emotional consequences. The present fMRI study compared distraction and reappraisal and found both similarities and differences between the two forms of emotion regulation. Both resulted in decreased negative affect, decreased activation in the amygdala, and increased activation in prefrontal and cingulate regions. Relative to distraction, reappraisal led to greater decreases in negative affect and to greater increases in a network of regions associated with processing affective meaning (medial prefrontal and anterior temporal cortices). Relative to reappraisal, distraction led to greater decreases in amygdala activation and to greater increases in activation in prefrontal and parietal regions. Taken together, these data suggest that distraction and reappraisal differentially engage neural systems involved in attentional deployment and cognitive reframing and have different emotional consequences.

Miller E. K., & Cohen J. D . ( 2001).

An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function

Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167-202.

DOI:10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167      URL     PMID:11283309      [本文引用: 1]

The prefrontal cortex has long been suspected to play an important role in cognitive control, in the ability to orchestrate thought and action in accordance with internal goals. Its neural basis, however, has remained a mystery. Here, we propose that cognitive control stems from the active maintenance of patterns of activity in the prefrontal cortex that represent goals and the means to achieve them. They provide bias signals to other brain structures whose net effect is to guide the flow of activity along neural pathways that establish the proper mappings between inputs, internal states, and outputs needed to perform a given task. We review neurophysiological, neurobiological, neuroimaging, and computational studies that support this theory and discuss its implications as well as further issues to be addressed

Moore S. A., Zoellner L. A., & Mollenholt N . ( 2008).

Are expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal associated with stress-related symptoms?

Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46( 9), 993-1000.

DOI:10.1016/j.brat.2008.05.001      URL     [本文引用: 1]

AbstractEmotion dysregulation is thought to be critical to the development of negative psychological outcomes. Gross (1998b) conceptualized the timing of regulation strategies as key to this relationship, with response-focused strategies, such as expressive suppression, as less effective and more detrimental compared to antecedent-focused ones, such as cognitive reappraisal. In the current study, we examined the relationship between reappraisal and expressive suppression and measures of psychopathology, particularly for stress-related reactions, in both undergraduate and trauma-exposed community samples of women. Generally, expressive suppression was associated with higher, and reappraisal with lower, self-reported stress-related symptoms. In particular, expressive suppression was associated with PTSD, anxiety, and depression symptoms in the trauma-exposed community sample, with rumination partially mediating this association. Finally, based on factor analysis, expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal appear to be independent constructs. Overall, expressive suppression, much more so than cognitive reappraisal, may play an important role in the experience of stress-related symptoms. Further, given their independence, there are potentially relevant clinical implications, as interventions that shift one of these emotion regulation strategies may not lead to changes in the other.]]>

Moser J. S., Hajcak G., Bukay E., & Simons R. F . ( 2006).

Intentional modulation of emotional responding to unpleasant pictures: An ERP study

Psychophysiology, 43( 3), 292-296.

DOI:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00402.x      URL     PMID:16805868      [本文引用: 1]

Intentionally altering responses to unpleasant stimuli affects physiological and hemodynamic activity associated with emotional and cognitive processing. In the present experiment, we measured the late-positive potential (LPP) of the visually evoked event-related brain potential to examine the effects of intentional emotion modulation on electrophysiological correlates of emotional and cognitive processing. Seventeen participants received instructions to view, suppress, and enhance emotional responses to unpleasant stimuli. Results revealed significantly decreased electrophysiological activity during suppression of emotional responses beginning around 250 ms poststimulus and lasting several hundred milliseconds. These data suggest that ERPs are sensitive to emotion modulation/regulation processes.

Nallasamy N., & Tsao D. Y . ( 2011).

Functional connectivity in the brain: Effects of anesthesia

The Neuroscientist, 17( 1), 94-106.

DOI:10.1177/1073858410374126      URL     PMID:21343409      [本文引用: 1]

Functional connectivity has been defined as &amp;quot;the temporal correlation of a neurophysiological index measured in different brain areas.&amp;quot; Since its definition, functional connectivity analysis has been used to describe temporal correlations across multiple spatial scales in PET imaging, single-unit and local field potential recordings, electroencephalography (EEG) and magnetoencephalography (MEG), optical imaging, and fMRI. These findings have been used to identify coactivating brain regions as functional networks. In some instances, as in the case of the default mode network (DMN), functional connectivity has been used to describe &amp;quot;modes&amp;quot; of brain function. The opportunity to probe the anesthetized state using functional connectivity analysis has given rise to a diverse literature over the past two decades. The examination of functional connectivity in the anesthetized state is of relevance to both anesthesiologists and neuroscientists, as it has the potential to elucidate still unclear mechanisms of anesthesia while offering insight into intrinsic functional activity in the brain. Complications have arisen, however, in the form of a lack of standardization of anesthetics, dosages, depths of anesthesia, and methods of functional connectivity analysis across studies. The present work attempts to examine, elucidate, and integrate the insight that functional connectivity analysis of the anesthetized state has generated thus far.

Nelson B. D., Fitzgerald D. A., Klumpp H., Shankman S. A., & Phan K. L . ( 2015).

Prefrontal engagement by cognitive reappraisal of negative faces

Behavioural Brain Research, 279, 218-225.

DOI:10.1016/j.bbr.2014.11.034      URL     PMID:25433095      [本文引用: 1]

Cognitive reappraisal has been associated with increased activation in prefrontal cortex (PFC) and cingulate regions implicated in cognitive control and affect regulation. To date, neuroimaging studies of reappraisal have primarily used emotionally evocative scenes, and it remains unclear whether the same cognitive strategy applied to emotional facial expressions would involve similar or different neural underpinnings. The present study used fMRI to examine brain activation during cognitive reappraisal of negatively valenced facial expressions relative to passive viewing of negative and neutral facial expressions. Twenty-two healthy adults completed a cognitive reappraisal task comprised of three different conditions (Look-Neutral, Maintain-Negative, Reappraise-Negative). Results indicated that reappraisal was associated with a decrease in negative affect and engagement of PFC brain regions implicated in cognitive control and affect regulation (DLPFC, mPFC, and VLPFC). Furthermore, individual differences in habitual reappraisal use were associated with greater DLPFC and mPFC activation, while suppression use was associated with greater amygdala activation. The present study provides preliminary evidence that facial expressions are effective alternative 'targets' of prefrontal engagement during cognitive reappraisal. These findings are particularly relevant for future research probing the neural bases of emotion regulation in populations for whom aversive scenes may be less appropriate (e.g., children) and illnesses in which aberrant responses to social signals of threat and negative feedback are cardinal phenotypes.

Ochsner K. N., Bunge S. A., Gross J. J., & Gabrieli J. D. E . ( 2002).

Rethinking feelings: An FMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14( 8), 1215-1229.

DOI:10.1162/089892902760807212      URL     PMID:12495527      [本文引用: 1]

The ability to cognitively regulate emotional responses to aversive events is important for mental and physical health. Little is known, however, about neural bases of the cognitive control of emotion. The present study employed functional magnetic resonance imaging to examine the neural systems used to reappraise highly negative scenes in unemotional terms. Reappraisal of highly negative scenes reduced subjective experience of negative affect. Neural correlates of reappraisal were increased activation of the lateral and medial prefrontal regions and decreased activation of the amygdala and medial orbito-frontal cortex. These findings support the hypothesis that prefrontal cortex is involved in constructing reappraisal strategies that can modulate activity in multiple emotion-processing systems.

Ochsner K. N., Hughes B., Robertson E. R., Cooper J. C., & Gabrieli J. D. E . ( 2009).

Neural systems supporting the control of affective and cognitive conflicts

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21( 9), 1841-1854.

DOI:10.1162/jocn.2009.21129      URL     PMID:18823233      [本文引用: 1]

Although many studies have examined the neural bases of controlling cognitive responses, the neural systems for controlling conflicts between competing affective responses remain unclear. To address the neural correlates of affective conflict and their relationship to cognitive conflict, the present study collected whole-brain fMRI data during two versions of the Eriksen flanker task. For these tasks, participants indicated either the valence (affective task) or the semantic category (cognitive task) of a central target word while ignoring flanking words that mapped onto either the same (congruent) or a different (incongruent) response as the target. Overall, contrasts of incongruent &amp;gt; congruent trials showed that bilateral dorsal ACC, posterior medial frontal cortex, and dorsolateral pFC were active during both kinds of conflict, whereas rostral medial pFC and left ventrolateral pFC were differentially active during affective or cognitive conflict, respectively. Individual difference analyses showed that separate regions of rostral cingulate/ventromedial pFC and left ventrolateral pFC were positively correlated with the magnitude of response time interference. Taken together, the findings that controlling affective and cognitive conflicts depends upon both common and distinct systems have important implications for understanding the organization of control systems in general and their potential dysfunction in clinical disorders.

Ohira H., Nomura M., Ichikawa N., Isowa T., Iidaka T., Sato A., … Yamada J .( 2006).

Association of neural and physiological responses during voluntary emotion suppression

NeuroImage, 29( 3), 721-733.

DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.048      URL     PMID:17239620      [本文引用: 1]

In this study, we examined the impact of goal-directed processing on the response to emotional pictures and the impact of emotional pictures on goal-directed processing. Subjects (N=22) viewed neutral or emotional pictures in the presence or absence of a demanding cognitive task. Goal-directed processing disrupted the BOLD response to emotional pictures. In particular, the BOLD response within bilateral amygdala and inferior frontal gyrus decreased during concurrent task performance. Moreover, the presence of both positive and negative distractors disrupted task performance, with reaction times increasing for emotional relative to neutral distractors. Moreover, in line with the suggestion of the importance of lateral frontal regions in emotional regulation [Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R. D., Cooper, J. C., Robertson, E. R., Chopra, S., Gabrieli, J. D., et al. (2004). For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down-and up-regulation of negative emotion. NeuroImage, 23(2), 483-499], connectivity analysis revealed positive connectivity between lateral superior frontal cortex and regions of middle frontal cortex previously implicated in emotional suppression [Beauregard, M., Levesque, J., and Bourgouin, P. (2001). Neural correlates of conscious self-regulation of emotion. J. Neurosci., 21 (18), RC165.; Levesque, J., Eugene, F., Joanette, Y., Paquette, V., Mensour, B., Beaudoin, G., et al. (2003). Neural circuitry underlying voluntary suppression of sadness. Biol. Psychiatry, 53 (6), 502-510.; Ohira, H., Nomura, M., Ichikawa, N., Isowa, T., Iidaka, T., Sato, A., et al. (2006). Association of neural and physiological responses during voluntary emotion suppression. NeuroImage, 29 (3), 721-733] and negative connectivity with bilateral amygdala. These data suggest that processes involved in emotional regulation are recruited during task performance in the context of emotional distractors.

Ohmatsu S., Nakano H., Tominaga T., Terakawa Y., Murata T., & Morioka S . ( 2014).

Activation of the serotonergic system by pedaling exercise changes anterior cingulate cortex activity and improves negative emotion

Behavioural Brain Research, 270, 112-117.

DOI:10.1016/j.bbr.2014.04.017      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Pedaling exercise (PE) of moderate intensity has been shown to ease anxiety and discomfort; however, little is known of the changes that occur in brain activities and in the serotonergic (5-HT) system after PE. Therefore, this study was conducted for the following reasons: (1) to localize the changes in the brain activities induced by PE using a distributed source localization algorithm, (2) to examine the changes in frontal asymmetry, as used in the Davidson model, with electroencephalography (EEG) activity, and (3) to examine the effect of PE on the 5-HT system. A 32-channel EEG was used to record before and after PE. Profile of Mood States tests indicated that there was a significant decrease in tension-anxiety and a significant increase in vigor after PE. A standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography analysis showed a significant decrease in brain activities after PE in the alpha-2 band (10-12.5 Hz) in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). Moreover, a significant increase in frontal EEG asymmetry was observed after PE in the alpha-1 band (7.5-10 Hz). Urine 5-HT levels significantly increased after PE. Urine 5-HT levels positively correlated with the degree of frontal EEG asymmetry in the alpha-1 band and negatively correlated with brain activity in ACC. Our results suggested that PE activates the 5-HT system and consequently induces increases in frontal EEG asymmetry in the alpha-1 band and reductions of brain activity in the alpha-2 band in the ACC region. (C) 2014 Elsevier B.V.

Pagani M., Di Lorenzo G., Verardo A. R., Nicolais G., Monaco L., & Lauretti G., … Siracusano A . ( 2012).

Neurobiological correlates of EMDR monitoring - An EEG study

PLOS ONE, 7( 9), e45753.

DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0045753      URL     PMID:23049852      [本文引用: 1]

Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing (EMDR) is a recognized first-line treatment for psychological trauma. However its neurobiological bases have yet to be fully disclosed.

Pan J., Zhan L., Hu C. L., Yang J., Wang C., Gu L., … Wu X . ( 2018).

Emotion regulation and complex brain networks: Association between expressive suppression and efficiency in the fronto-parietal network and default-mode network

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 70.

DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2018.00070      URL     PMID:29662443      [本文引用: 4]

Emotion regulation (ER) refers to the &amp;quot;implementation of a conscious or non-conscious goal to start, stop or otherwise modulate the trajectory of an emotion&amp;quot; (Etkin et al., 2015). Whereas multiple brain areas have been found to be involved in ER, relatively little is known about whether and how ER is associated with the global functioning of brain networks. Recent advances in brain connectivity research using graph-theory based analysis have shown that the brain can be organized into complex networks composed of functionally or structurally connected brain areas. Global efficiency is one graphic metric indicating the efficiency of information exchange among brain areas and is utilized to measure global functioning of brain networks. The present study examined the relationship between trait measures of ER (expressive suppression (ES) and cognitive reappraisal (CR)) and global efficiency in resting-state functional brain networks (the whole brain network and ten predefined networks) using structural equation modeling (SEM). The results showed that ES was reliably associated with efficiency in the fronto-parietal network and default-mode network. The finding advances the understanding of neural substrates of ER, revealing the relationship between ES and efficient organization of brain networks.

Pannu H. J., Morey R. A., Petty C. M., Srishti S., Smoski M. J., Gregory M. C., & Labar K. S . ( 2010).

Staying cool when things get hot: Emotion regulation modulates neural mechanisms of memory encoding

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 230.

DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2010.00230      URL     PMID:21212840      [本文引用: 1]

During times of emotional stress, individuals often engage in emotion regulation to reduce the experiential and physiological impact of negative emotions. Interestingly, emotion regulation strategies also influence memory encoding of the event. Cognitive reappraisal is associated with enhanced memory while expressive suppression is associated with impaired explicit memory of the emotional event. However, the mechanism by which these emotion regulation strategies affect memory is unclear. We used event-related fMRI to investigate the neural mechanisms that give rise to memory formation during emotion regulation. Twenty-five participants viewed negative pictures while alternately engaging in cognitive reappraisal, expressive suppression, or passive viewing. As part of the subsequent memory design, participants returned to the laboratory two weeks later for a surprise memory test. Behavioral results showed a reduction in negative affect and a retention advantage for reappraised stimuli relative to the other conditions. Imaging results showed that successful encoding during reappraisal was uniquely associated with greater co-activation of the left inferior frontal gyrus, amygdala, and hippocampus, suggesting a possible role for elaborative encoding of negative memories. This study provides neurobehavioral evidence that engaging in cognitive reappraisal is advantageous to both affective and mnemonic processes.

Pascual-Marqui R. D., Lehmann D., Koukkou M., Kochi K., Anderer P., Saletu B., … Kinoshita T . ( 2011)

Assessing interactions in the brain with exact low resolution electromagnetic tomography

Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences, 369, 3768-3784.

DOI:10.1098/rsta.2011.0081      URL     PMID:21893527      [本文引用: 1]

Scalp electric potentials (electroencephalogram; EEG) are contingent to the impressed current density unleashed by cortical pyramidal neurons undergoing post-synaptic processes. EEG neuroimaging consists of estimating the cortical current density from scalp recordings. We report a solution to this inverse problem that attains exact localization: exact low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (eLORETA). This non-invasive method yields high time-resolution intracranial signals that can be used for assessing functional dynamic connectivity in the brain, quantified by coherence and phase synchronization. However, these measures are non-physiologically high because of volume conduction and low spatial resolution. We present a new method to solve this problem by decomposing them into instantaneous and lagged components, with the lagged part having almost pure physiological origin.

Pei X., Wang J., Deng B., Wei X., & Yu H . ( 2014).

Wlpvg approach to the analysis of eeg-based functional brain network under manual acupuncture

Cognitive Neurodynamics, 8( 5), 417-428.

DOI:10.1007/s11571-014-9297-x      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Functional brain network, one of the main methods for brain functional studies, can provide the connectivity information among brain regions. In this research, EEG-based functional brain network is built and analyzed through a new wavelet limited penetrable visibility graph (WLPVG) approach. This approach first decompose EEG into delta, theta, alpha, beta sub-bands, then extracting nonlinear features from single channel signal, in addition forming a functional brain network for each sub-band. Manual acupuncture (MA) as a stimulation to the human nerve system, may evoke varied modulating effects in brain activities. To investigating whether and how this happens, WLPVG approach is used to analyze the EEGs of 15 healthy subjects with MA at acupoint ST36 on the right leg. It is found that MA can influence the complexity of EEG sub-bands in different ways and lead the functional brain networks to obtain higher efficiency and stronger small-world property compared with pre-acupuncture control state.

Phan K. L., Fitzgerald D. A., Nathan P. J., Moore G. J., Uhde T. W., & Tancer M. E . ( 2005).

Neural substrates for voluntary suppression of negative affect: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study

Biological Psychiatry, 57( 3), 210-219.

DOI:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.030      URL     PMID:15691521      [本文引用: 1]

Successful control of affect partly depends on the capacity to modulate negative emotional responses through the use of cognitive strategies. Although the capacity to regulate emotions is critical to mental well-being, its neural substrates remain unclear.

Qian L., Xi C., Tom H., Duo X., Frederick C., & James B. R . ( 2014).

Theta band activity in response to emotional expressions and its relationship with gamma band activity as revealed by MEG and advanced beamformer source imaging

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 940.

DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00940      URL     PMID:24550804      [本文引用: 1]

Neuronal oscillations in the theta and gamma bands have been shown to be important for cognition. Here we examined the temporal and spatial relationship between the two frequency bands in emotional processing using magnetoencephalography and an advanced dynamic beamformer source imaging method called synthetic aperture magnetometry. We found that areas including the amygdala, visual and frontal cortex showed significant event-related synchronization in both bands, suggesting a functional association of neuronal oscillations in the same areas in the two bands. However, while the temporal profile in both bands was similar in the amygdala, the peak in gamma band power was much earlier within both visual and frontal areas. Our results do not support a traditional view that the localizations of lower and higher frequencies are spatially distinct. Instead, they suggest that in emotional processing, neuronal oscillations in the gamma and theta bands may reflect, at least in visual and frontal cortex either different but related functional processes or, perhaps more probably, different computational components of the same functional process.

Scult M. A., Knodt A. R., Swartz J. R., Brigidi B. D., & Hariri A. R . ( 2017).

Thinking and feeling: Individual differences in habitual emotion regulation and stress- related mood are associated with prefrontal executive control

Clinical Psychological Science, 5( 1), 150-157.

DOI:10.1177/2167702616654688      URL     PMID:28191365      [本文引用: 1]

Calculating math problems from memory may seem unrelated to everyday processing of emotions, but they have more in common than one might think. Prior research highlights the importance of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) in executive control, intentional emotion regulation, and experience of dysfunctional mood and anxiety. While it has been hypothesized that emotion regulation may be related to 'cold' (ie. not emotion-related) executive control, this assertion has not been tested. We address this gap by providing evidence that greater dlPFC activity during 'cold' executive control is associated with increased use of cognitive reappraisal to regulate emotions in everyday life. We then demonstrate that in the presence of increased life stress, increased dlPFC activity is associated with lower mood and anxiety symptoms and clinical diagnoses. Collectively, our results encourage ongoing efforts to understand prefrontal executive control as a possible intervention target for improving emotion regulation in mood and anxiety disorders.

Sheline Y. I., Price J. L., Yan Z., & Mintun M. A . ( 2010).

Resting-state functional MRI in depression unmasks increased connectivity between networks via the dorsal nexus

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107( 24), 11020-11025.

DOI:10.1073/pnas.1000446107      URL     PMID:20534464      [本文引用: 1]

To better understand intrinsic brain connections in major depression, we used a neuroimaging technique that measures resting state functional connectivity using functional MRI (fMRI). Three different brain networks--the cognitive control network, default mode network, and affective network--were investigated. Compared with controls, in depressed subjects each of these three networks had increased connectivity to the same bilateral dorsal medial prefrontal cortex region, an area that we term the dorsal nexus. The dorsal nexus demonstrated dramatically increased depression-associated fMRI connectivity with large portions of each of the three networks. The discovery that these regions are linked together through the dorsal nexus provides a potential mechanism to explain how symptoms of major depression thought to arise in distinct networks--decreased ability to focus on cognitive tasks, rumination, excessive self-focus, increased vigilance, and emotional, visceral, and autonomic dysregulation--could occur concurrently and behave synergistically. It suggests that the newly identified dorsal nexus plays a critical role in depressive symptomatology, in effect &amp;quot;hot wiring&amp;quot; networks together; it further suggests that reducing increased connectivity of the dorsal nexus presents a potential therapeutic target.

Shigeto H., Ishiguro J., & Nittono H . ( 2011).

Effects of visual stimulus complexity on event-related brain potentials and viewing duration in a free-viewing task

Neuroscience Letters, 497( 2), 85-89.

DOI:10.1016/j.neulet.2011.04.035      URL     PMID:21540078      [本文引用: 1]

The anterior N2 is a component of the event-related brain potential (ERP) elicited by visual novel stimuli. Previous studies have reported that the stimuli that were viewed for longer periods of time elicited a larger anterior N2 than the stimuli viewed for shorter periods of time. To scrutinize this relationship between the ERP and viewing duration in response to visual materials, 18 university students were asked to look at various random polygons one-by-one for as long as they wished. ERPs time-locked to stimulus onset were averaged separately for three levels of complexity (12-, 24-, and 48-sided polygons). We found that the more complex the stimulus, the larger the anterior negativity (N2, 200-300 ms) and the posterior positivity (late positive potential [LPP], 400-800 ms), and the longer the viewing duration. However, when ERPs were calculated separately for the stimuli viewed for longer or shorter than the median viewing time of each participant at each complexity level, no amplitude differences were found in either component. These results suggest that the previously reported correlation between the anterior N2 and visual duration is spurious and produced by a third variable, namely, the perceptual demand of the eliciting stimulus such as complexity.

Stam C. J., Nolte G., & Daffertshofer A . ( 2007).

Phase lag index: Assessment of functional connectivity from multichannel EEG and MEG with diminished bias from common sources

Human Brain Mapping, 28( 11), 1178-1193.

DOI:10.1002/hbm.20346      URL     PMID:17266107      [本文引用: 1]

To address the problem of volume conduction and active reference electrodes in the assessment of functional connectivity, we propose a novel measure to quantify phase synchronization, the phase lag index (PLI), and compare its performance to the well-known phase coherence (PC), and to the imaginary component of coherency (IC).

Szaflarski J. P., Allendorfer J. B., Heyse H., Mendoza L., Szaflarski B. A., & Cohen N . ( 2014).

Functional mri of facial emotion processing in left temporal lobe epilepsy

Epilepsy & Behavior, 32, 92-99.

DOI:10.21101/cejph.a5765      URL     PMID:31901191      [本文引用: 1]

Alcohol use is one of the ten most common risk factors threatening global health that is avoidable (ranked fifth after smoking). It is involved in high rates of liver cirrhosis, epilepsy, hypertension, cerebrovascular and mental illness. Negative consequences of family and social drinking are also very significant. The aim of the study is to quantify the gender and age-differentiated disparities in alcohol-related mortality in the population over 16 years between the years 1996-2017 in Slovakia.

Urbain C., Sato J., Pang E. W., & Taylor M. J . ( 2017).

The temporal and spatial brain dynamics of automatic emotion regulation in children

Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 26, 62-68.

DOI:10.1016/j.dcn.2017.05.004      URL     PMID:28527986      [本文引用: 1]

corr&lt;0.05), and finally, the left anterior temporal lobe (ATL) from 325 to 425ms. Our results suggest a specific involvement of these regions in the automatic regulation of negative emotional stimuli in children. In the future, this knowledge may help understand developmental conditions where inhibition impairments are exacerbated by an emotional context.]]>

van den Heuvel M. P., Stam C. J., Kahn R. S., & Hulshoff Pol H. E . ( 2009).

Efficiency of functional brain networks and intellectual performance

Journal of Neuroscience, 29( 23), 7619-7624.

DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1443-09.2009      URL     PMID:19515930      [本文引用: 1]

Our brain is a complex network in which information is continuously processed and transported between spatially distributed but functionally linked regions. Recent studies have shown that the functional connections of the brain network are organized in a highly efficient small-world manner, indicating a high level of local neighborhood clustering, together with the existence of more long-distance connections that ensure a high level of global communication efficiency within the overall network. Such an efficient network architecture of our functional brain raises the question of a possible association between how efficiently the regions of our brain are functionally connected and our level of intelligence. Examining the overall organization of the brain network using graph analysis, we show a strong negative association between the normalized characteristic path length lambda of the resting-state brain network and intelligence quotient (IQ). This suggests that human intellectual performance is likely to be related to how efficiently our brain integrates information between multiple brain regions. Most pronounced effects between normalized path length and IQ were found in frontal and parietal regions. Our findings indicate a strong positive association between the global efficiency of functional brain networks and intellectual performance.

van den Heuvel, M. P., & Hulshoff Pol, H. E . ( 2010).

Exploring the brain network: A review on resting-state fMRI functional connectivity

European Neuropsychopharmacology, 20( 8), 519-534.

DOI:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010.03.008      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Our brain is a network. It consists of spatially distributed, but functionally linked regions that continuously share information with each other. Interestingly, recent advances in the acquisition and analysis of functional neuroimaging data have catalyzed the exploration of functional connectivity in the human brain. Functional connectivity is defined as the temporal dependency of neuronal activation patterns of anatomically separated brain regions and in the past years an increasing body of neuroimaging studies has started to explore functional connectivity by measuring the level of co-activation of resting-state fMRI time-series between brain regions. These studies have revealed interesting new findings about the functional connections of specific brain regions and local networks, as well as important new insights in the overall organization of functional communication in the brain network. Here we present an overview of these new methods and discuss how they have led to new insights in core aspects of the human brain, providing an overview of these novel imaging techniques and their implication to neuroscience. We discuss the use of spontaneous resting-state fMRI in determining functional connectivity, discuss suggested origins of these signals, how functional connections tend to be related to structural connections in the brain network and how functional brain communication may form a key role in cognitive performance. Furthermore, we will discuss the upcoming field of examining functional connectivity patterns using graph theory, focusing on the overall organization of the functional brain network. Specifically, we will discuss the value of these new functional connectivity tools in examining believed connectivity diseases, like Alzheimer's disease, dementia, schizophrenia and multiple sclerosis.

Vanderhasselt M. A., Kuhn S., & De Raedt R . ( 2013).

"Put on your poker face": Neural systems supporting the anticipation for expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal

Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8( 8), 903-910.

DOI:10.1093/scan/nss090      URL     PMID:22956675      [本文引用: 2]

It is a unique human ability to regulate negative thoughts and feelings. Two well-investigated emotion-regulation strategies (ERSs), cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression, are associated with overlapping prefrontal neural correlates, but differ temporally during the emotion-generation process. Although functional imaging studies have mainly investigated these ERS as a reaction to an emotion-inducing event, the intention to regulate upcoming negative emotions might already be associated with differences in neural activity. Hence, event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging was recorded in 42 participants while they completed an emotion-regulation paradigm. During this task, participants were instructed to proactively prepare to use a specific ERS knowing that a negative, high-arousing image would appear after the preparation period. As expected, the results demonstrated prefrontal and parietal activation while participants were suppressing or reappraising their emotions (family-wise error (FWE)-corrected). The intention to suppress emotions was associated with increased activation in the right inferior frontal gyrus, bilateral putamen, pre-supplementary motor area and right supramarginal gyrus (FWE-corrected). This enhanced proactive inhibitory control: (i) predicted decreased motoric activity during the actual suppression of emotional expressions and (2) trended toward a significant association with how successfully participants suppressed their emotions. However, neural correlates of preparatory control for cognitive reappraisal were not observed, possibly because contextual cues about the upcoming emotional stimulus are necessary to proactively start to cognitively reinterpret the situation.

van Diessen E., Numan T., van Dellen E., van der Kooi A.W., Boersma M., Hofman D., … Stam C. J . ( 2015).

Opportunities and methodological challenges in EEG and MEG resting state functional brain network research

Clinical Neurophysiology, 126( 8), 1468-1481.

DOI:10.1016/j.clinph.2014.11.018      URL     PMID:25511636      [本文引用: 1]

Electroencephalogram (EEG) and magnetoencephalogram (MEG) recordings during resting state are increasingly used to study functional connectivity and network topology. Moreover, the number of different analysis approaches is expanding along with the rising interest in this research area. The comparison between studies can therefore be challenging and discussion is needed to underscore methodological opportunities and pitfalls in functional connectivity and network studies. In this overview we discuss methodological considerations throughout the analysis pipeline of recording and analyzing resting state EEG and MEG data, with a focus on functional connectivity and network analysis. We summarize current common practices with their advantages and disadvantages; provide practical tips, and suggestions for future research. Finally, we discuss how methodological choices in resting state research can affect the construction of functional networks. When taking advantage of current best practices and avoid the most obvious pitfalls, functional connectivity and network studies can be improved and enable a more accurate interpretation and comparison between studies.

Varnum M. E. W., & Hampton R. S . ( 2016).

Cultures differ in the ability to enhance affective neural responses

Social Neuroscience, 12( 5), 1-10.

DOI:10.1080/17470919.2016.1209239      URL     PMID:27420406      [本文引用: 1]

The present study (N = 55) used an event-related potential paradigm to investigate whether cultures differ in the ability to upregulate affective responses. Using stimuli selected from the International Affective Picture System, we found that European-Americans (N = 29) enhanced central-parietal late positive potential (LPP) (400-800 ms post-stimulus) responses to affective stimuli when instructed to do so, whereas East Asians (N = 26) did not. We observed cultural differences in the ability to enhance central-parietal LPP responses for both positively and negativelyvalenced stimuli, and the ability to enhance these two types of responses was positively correlated for Americans but negatively for East Asians. These results are consistent with the notion that cultural variations in norms and values regarding affective expression and experiences shape how the brain regulates emotions.

Viviani R . ( 2014).

Neural correlates of emotion regulation in the ventral prefrontal cortex and the encoding of subjective value and economic utility

Frontiers in Psychiatry, 5.

DOI:10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00187      URL     PMID:25691873      [本文引用: 2]

In movies, alcohol-related cues are frequently depicted and there is evidence for a link between movie alcohol cues and immediate alcohol consumption. Less is known about factors influencing immediate effects movie alcohol exposure on drinking. The exertion of self-control is thought to be important in avoiding or resisting certain temptations.

Wagner M., Fuchs M., & Kastner J . ( 2004).

Evaluation of sLORETA in the presence of noise and multiple sources

Brain Topography, 16( 4), 277-280.

DOI:10.1023/B:BRAT.0000032865.58382.62      URL     [本文引用: 1]

The standardized Low Resolution Brain Electromagnetic Tomography method (sLORETA) can be used to compute statistical maps from EEG and MEG data that indicate the locations of the underlying source processes with low error. These maps are derived by performing a location-wise inverse weighting of the results of a Minimum Norm Least Squares (MNLS) analysis with their estimated variances. In this contribution, we evaluate the performance of the method under the presence of noise and with multiple, simultaneously active sources. It is shown that the sLORETA method localizes well, as compared to other linear approaches such as MNLS and LORETA. However, simultaneously active sources can only be separated if their fields are distinct enough and of similar strength. In the context of a strong or superficial source, weak or deep sources remain invisible, and nearby sources of similar orientation tend not to be separated but interpreted as one source located roughly in between.]]>

Wang J. X., Wang C. M., Xie F., Chang M., & Zhang K . ( 2015).

The effect of cognitive reappraisal and distraction in regulating negative emotion: ERPs study

Journal of Psychological Science, 38( 5), 1039-1044.

[本文引用: 2]

[ 王敬欣, 王春梅, 谢芳, 常敏, 张阔 . ( 2015).

负性情绪调节中认知重评和分心策略的作用: ERPs研究

心理科学, 38( 5), 1039-1044.]

[本文引用: 2]

Wang J., Wang X., Xia M., Liao X., Evans A., & He Y . ( 2015).

Gretna: A graph theoretical network analysis toolbox for imaging connectomics

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9.

DOI:10.2147/NDT.S135426      URL     PMID:28652747     

Obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) is a common sleep-related breathing disorder that can damage cognitive function. However, the functional network organization remains poorly understood. The aim of this study was to investigate the topological properties of OSA patients using a graph theoretical analysis.

Wang J., Zuo X., & He Y . ( 2010).

Graph-based network analysis of resting-state functional MRI

Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 4.

DOI:10.3389/fnsys.2010.00162      URL     PMID:21283555      [本文引用: 1]

The putative excitatory and inhibitory cell classes within the mouse primary visual cortex V1 have different functional properties as studied using recording microelectrode. Excitatory neurons show high selectivity for the orientation angle of moving gratings while the putative inhibitory neurons show poor selectivity. However, the study of selectivity of the genetically identified interneurons and their subtypes remain controversial. Here we use novel Cre-driver and reporter mice to identify genetic subpopulations in vivo for two-photon calcium dye imaging: Wfs1(+)/Gad1(-) mice that labels layer 2/3 excitatory cell population and Pvalb(+)/Gad1(+) mice that labels a genetic subpopulation of inhibitory neurons. The cells in both mice were identically labeled with a tdTomato protein, visible in vivo, using a Cre-reporter line. We found that the Wfs1(+) cells exhibited visual tuning properties comparable to the excitatory population, i.e., high selectivity and tuning to the angle, direction, and spatial frequency of oriented moving gratings. The functional tuning of Pvalb(+) neurons was consistent with previously reported narrow-spiking interneurons in microelectrode studies, exhibiting poorer selectivity than the excitatory neurons. This study demonstrates the utility of Cre-transgenic mouse technology in selective targeting of subpopulations of neurons and makes them amenable to structural, functional, and connectivity studies.

Wang K., Huang H., Chen L., Hou X., Zhang Y., Yang J., … Qiu J . ( 2017).

MRI correlates of interaction between gender and expressive suppression among the Chinese population

Neuroscience, 347, 76-84.

DOI:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.01.042      URL     PMID:28188856      [本文引用: 5]

Expressive suppression is a kind of emotion regulation strategies by suppressing behaviors related to emotional responding. Despite the amount of behavioral research on expressive suppression, the structural and functional mechanisms underlying the interaction between gender and expressive suppression in Chinese healthy subjects have remained unknown. In the current study, we assessed the levels of expressive suppression and acquired the structural and functional imaging data from 273 Chinese individuals. A nearly automatic cortical processing technique was used to calculate cortical thickness for each subject. The results from cortical thickness analyses revealed a significant interaction between gender and expressive suppression in the superior frontal gyrus. Then, we conducted the whole-brain functional connectivity analysis with the seed of the superior frontal gyrus to explore the functionally related regions of brain. Subsequent analysis of the interaction between gender and expressive suppression indicated a significant functional connectivity between the superior frontal gyrus and default mode network (DMN) core regions, including the medial prefrontal cortex, precuneus and parahippocampal gyrus. Our results provided the robust empirical evidence illustrating the role of the superior frontal gyrus and DMN in gender difference of expressive suppression among the Chinese population. These findings might have implications for understanding gender difference in emotion processing and regulation.

Wang L., Liu H. C., Li Z. Q., & Du W .( 2007).

Reliability and validity of emotion regulation questionnaire Chinese revised version

China Journal of Health Psychology, 15( 6), 503-505.

[本文引用: 1]

[ 王力, 柳恒超, 李中权, 杜卫 . ( 2007).

情绪调节问卷中文版的信效度研究

中国健康心理学杂志, 15( 6), 503-505.]

[本文引用: 1]

Wang Y. N., Zhou L. M., Qu C., & Luo Y. J . ( 2007).

Implicitly processing of affective connotation of chinese words evidence from event-related brain potential

Journal of Beijing Normal University (Natural Science), 43( 4), 466-470.

[本文引用: 1]

[ 王一牛, 周立明, 曲琛, 罗跃嘉 . ( 2007).

感情色彩双字词内隐加工的ERP研究

北京师范大学学报(自然科学版), 43( 4), 466-470.]

[本文引用: 1]

Xing M., Tadayonnejad R., MacNamara A., Ajilore O., Phan K. L., Klumpp H., & Leow A . ( 2016).

EEG based functional connectivity reflects cognitive load during emotion regulation

IEEE International Symposium on Biomedical Imaging. IEEE.

[本文引用: 1]

Zhang W., Li X., Liu X., Duan X., Wang D., & Shen J . ( 2013).

Distraction reduces theta synchronization in emotion regulation during adolescence

Neuroscience Letters, 550, 81-86.

DOI:10.1016/j.neulet.2013.05.070      URL     PMID:23827226      [本文引用: 2]

We sought to determine if attentional distraction in adolescents can modulate event-related desynchronization or synchronization (ERD or ERS) of the theta band during emotion regulation. Event-related theta oscillations were collected from 48 adolescents and young adults as they performed a distraction (counting) task while viewing affective pictures. Consistent with data from adult participants, positive and negative pictures elicited a larger theta ERS than did neutral pictures within a 100-400 ms window, indicating that early theta ERS is indicative of motivated attention to biologically salient stimuli. Counting as a distraction strategy attenuated early affective modulation of theta ERS. Moreover, theta ERS increased with age in the anterior regions of the brain regardless of valence; however, no age differences were found in the posterior regions. These results suggest that distraction depends on a top-down attentional mechanism that disrupts theta ERS for affective pictures at an early stage. Furthermore, adolescents undergo a developmental increase in oscillatory brain reorganization.

Zhao L.Y., Tian J., Wang W., Qin W., Shi J., Li Q., … Lu L . ( 2012).

The role of dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in the regulation of craving by reappraisal in smokers

PLOS ONE, 7( 8), e43598.

DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0043598      URL     PMID:22928000      [本文引用: 1]

Drug cues can induce craving for drugs of abuse. Dysfunctional regulation of emotion and motivation regarding rewarding objects appears to be an integral part of addiction. It has been found that cognitive strategies decreased the intensity of craving in addicts. Reappraisal strategy is a type of cognitive strategy that requires participants to reinterpret the meaning of an emotional situation. In addition, studies have found that activation of the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) is associated with the selection and application of cognitive reappraisal. In present study, we sought to determine whether such cognitive regulation engages the dACC and improves inhibition of craving in smokers.

Zhou Y., Yu C., Zheng H., Liu Y., Song M., Qin W., … Jiang T . ( 2010).

Increased neural resources recruitment in the intrinsic organization in major depression

Journal of Affective Disorders, 121( 3), 220-230.

DOI:10.1016/j.jad.2009.05.029      URL     PMID:19541369      [本文引用: 1]

To investigate the functional connectivity (FC) pattern within an intrinsic functional organization, including both task-positive (TPN) and task-negative (TNN) networks, in major depressive disorder (MDD), and to examine relationships between the involved FCs and clinical variables.

/


版权所有 © 《心理学报》编辑部
地址:北京市朝阳区林萃路16号院 
邮编:100101 
电话:010-64850861 
E-mail:xuebao@psych.ac.cn
备案编号:京ICP备10049795号-1 京公网安备110402500018号

本系统由北京玛格泰克科技发展有限公司设计开发