ISSN 0439-755X
CN 11-1911/B
主办:中国心理学会
   中国科学院心理研究所
出版:科学出版社

心理学报, 2018, 50(12): 1413-1427 doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.01413

研究报告

正念维持适应的机制:来自心率变异性自相似的初步证据

孙莎莎1, 李小兵2, 李宝山1, 刘承宜,3, 黄敏儿,1

1 中山大学心理学系, 广州 510006

2 中山大学体育部, 广州 510275

3 华南师范大学体育科学学院, 广州 510006

Physiological mechanisms of mindfulness: Preliminary evidence from self-similarity of heart rate variability

SUN Shasha1, LI Xiaobing2, LI Baoshan1, LIU Chengyi,3, HUANG Miner,1

1 Department of Psychology, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou 510006, China

2 Department of Physical Education, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou 510275, China

3 School of Physical Education and Sports Science, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510006, China

通讯作者: 刘承宜, E-mail:liutcy@scnu.edu.cn黄敏儿, E-mail:edshme@mail.sysu.edu.cn

收稿日期: 2017-09-11   网络出版日期: 2018-11-30

基金资助: * 国家自然科学基金项目.  61575065
教育部人文社会科学基金项目.  17YJA190008, 14YJC630226
《广州大典》与广州历史文化研究重点课题.  2018GZZ07

Received: 2017-09-11   Online: 2018-11-30

摘要

引入自相似的算法表征功能内稳态的过程, 从功能内稳态的角度探讨特质正念与不同过程中心率变异性(Heart rate variability, HRV)自相似的关系, 以揭示正念起作用的可能机制。功能内稳态是机体面对内外环境的变化保持自身功能相对稳定的一种能力或素质。研究设置静息态、冷加压任务、冷加压后恢复和正念练习4种不同的条件, 并记录被试在这4个环节的HRV指标。结果显示, HRV自相似与特质正念之间存在显著相关, 高HRV自相似组在冷加压环节的HRV各指标都比低HRV自相似组高。结果表明, 正念维持适应的生理机制可能与功能内稳态有关。

关键词: 特质正念 ; 自相似 ; 功能内稳态 ; 心率变异性

Abstract

Mindfulness has a considerable contribution to one’s physical and psychological health and his/her ability to adapt to an endlessly changing environment. Researchers have proposed different perspectives in studying the mechanism of mindfulness. However, few of the previous theoretical frameworks are based on the ongoing changes in a dynamic process. In this study, we investigate the relationship between trait mindfulness and the functions of the autonomic nervous system from a “self-similarity” perspective, which represents function- specific homeostasis. The concept of self-similarity refers to the ability of the human body to remain stable when experiencing changes from the internal and external environmental factors In the current study, four conditions (5 min resting, 3 min stressful, 5 min post-stress states, and 5.4 min mindfulness practice) were established to induce different physical and psychological status and to examine the self-similarity levels of the participants. The cold-pressor task, a safe and effective paradigm to induce pain in laboratory settings, was used in this study, which involved 56 undergraduate students. Heart rate variability (HRV) with its time- and frequency-domain measures (SDNN, RMSSD, TP, LF, and HF), which represent the functions of the autonomic nervous system, were employed to calculate self-similarity. A biofeedback system (emWave Pro Plus) with an ear sensor was used to record the HRV measures consecutively during the four phases.

The three main results of the study are as follows. First, one-way repeated analyses of the variance test on HRV measures (SDNN, RMSSD, TP, LF, and HF) yielded significant effects for the conditions. The post hoc test indicated that the HRV under stressful condition was higher than those under conditions of resting state, post-stress state, and mindfulness practice. Hence, the whole function and balance of the autonomic nervous system, HRV measure, and coherence were significantly higher in mindfulness practice than in the resting, stressful, and post-stress states. Second, correlation analysis revealed that the self-similarity level of HRV significantly correlated with trait mindfulness measured by the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS). Third, the 56 participants were divided into two groups according to mean self-similarity level. Measures of HRV (SDNN, RMSSD, TP, LF, and HF) of the high self-similarity group (n = 25) were significantly higher than those of the low self-similarity group (n = 31) only under stressful conditions. However, this was not the case under the other three conditions.

The above results indicated that HRV changes according to the changing conditions; moreover, high trait mindfulness and self-similarity can protect organisms from poor adaptation, especially when they encounter stressful conditions. The correlation between mindfulness and self-similarity of HRV measures implies that self-similarity may play a core role in how mindfulness works. The current study is a pilot study conducted with only college students, thus limiting the generalization of our conclusion. Moreover, the participants in our study had no previous experience of mindfulness practice, as their mindfulness level was represented with the MAAS measurement. In the future, we seek to recruit people with mindfulness or meditation practice experiences to explore whether meditation experts would better keep their function-specific homeostasis in different processes than meditation novices would.

Keywords: trait mindfulness ; self-similarity ; function-specific homeostasis ; heart rate variability (HRV)

PDF (911KB) 元数据 多维度评价 相关文章 导出 EndNote| Ris| Bibtex  收藏本文

本文引用格式

孙莎莎, 李小兵, 李宝山, 刘承宜, 黄敏儿. 正念维持适应的机制:来自心率变异性自相似的初步证据 . 心理学报[J], 2018, 50(12): 1413-1427 doi:10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.01413

SUN Shasha, LI Xiaobing, LI Baoshan, LIU Chengyi, HUANG Miner. Physiological mechanisms of mindfulness: Preliminary evidence from self-similarity of heart rate variability. Acta Psychologica Sinica[J], 2018, 50(12): 1413-1427 doi:10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.01413

1 引言

“正念” (mindfulness)这个来源于佛学的概念, 近些年来得到了越来越多的关注。从研究层面, 近20年来关于正念的论文数量呈指数级增长; 从应用层面, 正念已被越来越多地应用到临床、教育、管理及体育竞技等各个领域。

那么, 正念究竟指的是什么?有哪些作用?其作用机制有哪些?下文将从正念对健康和适应的促进作用及其生理机制方面做一些梳理, 并指出以往研究的不足, 在此基础上提出本文的研究。

1.1 正念的概念与作用

正念是指有意识地对当下非评判的觉察(Kabat-Zinn, 1994)。正念是一个过程, 着重对当下体验的开放与接纳(Baer, Smith, & Allen, 2004; Bishop et al., 2004; Brown & Ryan, 2003; Chambers, Gullone, & Allen, 2009)。正念也是觉知当下的一系列技术方法(Didonna, 2009)。作为一种维护健康与适应的自我调节的方法, 正念得到了越来越多的关注。自Kabat-Zinn (1982)报告关于正念缓解慢性疼痛的研究后, 后续大量研究支持正念对健康及适应的促进效应(Eberth, & Sedlmeier, 2012; Grossman, Niemann, Schmidt, & Walach, 2004; Keng, Smoski, & Robins, 2011; Sedlmeier et al., 2012)。作为干预方法, 正念可以减少抑郁的复发(Godfrin & van Heeringen, 2010), 提高社交焦虑者的情绪调节能力(Goldin & Gross, 2010); 减少酒精依赖者复饮(Vieten, Astin, Buscemi, & Galloway, 2010), 改善糖尿病患者的身体健康状况(Hartmann et al., 2012), 促进癌症患者心理社会方面的调整(Henderson et al., 2012), 维持HIV阳性者的免疫系统功能 (Creswell, Myers, Cole, & Irwin, 2009)。作为一种心理特质, 正念预测更高的关系满意度(Barnes, Brown, Krusemark, Cambell, & Rogge, 2007), 更强的幸福感及工作卷入(Malinowski & Hui, 2015)。

特质正念反映个体聚焦当下的注意与自我调节的适应倾向(Weinstein, Brown, & Ryan, 2009)。高特质正念的个体对自己的体验采取较为接纳、非评判的态度, 更多地采用去中心化视角, 能够更好地将自己的想法和情绪等视为心理历程, 而不是视为对现实的精准表征(Shapiro, Carlson, Astin, & Freedman, 2006)。因此, 相对于特质正念较低个体, 特质正念较高个体能够根据环境的变化做出更为灵活有效的调整。有研究者将特质正念定义为注意觉知相关的单维度结构(Brown & Ryan, 2003), 也有研究者认为特质正念是多维度结构, 涵盖注意觉知及非评判接纳自身内部体验等(Sauer et al., 2013)。尽管不同研究者对于特质正念的操作性定义存在一定差异, 大量研究结果还是一致表明了高特质正念在自我调节时更具适应性(Siegling & Petrides, 2014)。

1.2 正念维持健康和适应的作用机制

正念如何维持和促进适应?其内在生理机制有哪些?有研究者指出, 正念增强前额皮层的调节功能, 通过HPA (hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical)轴和SAM (sympathetic-adrenomedullary)轴减少皮质醇、肾上腺素及去甲肾上腺素的分泌, 减弱应激反应, 降低应激相关疾病风险(Creswell & Lindsay, 2014; Creswell, 2015)。自主神经系统是机体自动化的应激反应调节系统(Thayer & Lane, 2000)。正念有可能通过调节心血管系统的应激反应来影响自主神经系统的调节功能(Demarzo et al., 2014)。

心率变异性(Heart rate variability, HRV)标志自主神经系统的调节功能, 反映大脑对行为与外周生理的控制(Thayer, Åhs, Fredrikson, Sollers, & Wager, 2012), 是个体适应情况的良好生物标记物(Force, 1996; Thayer et al., 2012; Thayer & Lane, 2000,2009; Thayer & Brosschot, 2005)。较强的HRV表明心率变异范围较大, 心血管系统有较强的应变能力。较弱的HRV则表明自主神经系统对变化环境缺少足够的应变能力, 将增加相关疾病的患病风险(Thayer & Lane, 2000)。研究表明, 正念练习可增强HRV, 增强副交感神经系统的活动(Libby, Worhunsky, Pilver, & Brewer, 2012; Krygier et al., 2013)。可见, HRV的改善可能是正念促进适应的一种重要生理机制。目前HRV线性量化方法大致可以分为时域分析和频域分析(Berntson et al., 1997)。其中, 常用的HRV时域分析指标主要包括以下两个:连续正常RR间期的标准差(the standard deviation of the normal (NN) sinus, SDNN), 反映影响HRV的所有因素的变化情况(Shaffer, McCraty, & Zerr, 2014); 相邻RR间期之差的均方根(the root mean square of successive differences between normal heartbeats, RMSSD), 反映逐次心跳的变异情况, 主要用来评估副交感神经系统的活动(Kleiger, Stein, & Bigger, 2005)。HRV频域分析指标是将总的频谱功率(total power, TP)根据功率频谱密度(power spectral density, PSD)分离出低频(low frequency, LF, 0.04~0.15 Hz) HRV和高频(high frequency, HF, 0.15~0.40 Hz) HRV。一般认为, LF-HRV反映交感神经系统的活动, HF-HRV反映副交感神经系统的活动(Berntson et al., 1997; Billman, 2011)。研究者用LF/HF ratio来量化交感神经活动和副交感神经活动的变化关系(Pagani et al., 1984)。和谐性(coherence)是近年来研究者提出的HRV频域分析的重要指标, 该指标可以反映呼吸、血压及心脏节律间的相互影响, 也可以反映系统活动间(如EEG节律与心跳周期)的同步性或系统共振(Shaffer et al., 2014; McCraty & Childre, 2010)。

有研究者从内稳态(homeostasis)的角度探讨正念维持机体适应的机制。研究者指出, 面对压力时, 前扣带回(Anterior cingulate gyrus, ACC, Tirch, 2010)、自主神经系统(autonomic nervous system, ANS, Hosemans, 2015)、皮质醇分泌(Matousek, Dobkin, & Pruessner, 2010)等功能的失调, 使机体无法维持自身内稳态。通过全然地觉知(保持正念), 个体能够实时地监控内外刺激带来的身体感觉, 更好地评估环境及所面临的问题, 实时调整自身状态以应对环境变化, 重新建立内稳态并适应新的环境, 而不是处于持续的应激状态中。也就是说, 正念能够让个体在面对复杂的环境时保持自身的稳态应变, 使机体各项功能(诸如上述所提到的ACC、ANS和皮质醇分泌等)得以充分稳定地发挥, 从而做出更具适应性的行为反应(Fletcher, Schoendorff, & Hayes, 2010; Matousek et al., 2010; Fries, 2007)。愤怒作为一种情绪, 在调动心理资源、维护自尊等方面有着重要作用。然而, 一直以来愤怒也被看作是一种最具破坏性的情绪, 对愤怒者本身及他人都会带来危害(Wright, Day & Howells, 2009)。Novaco (2007)指出, 愤怒是一种习得的自动化的应对方式, 愤怒调节的核心是自我监控, “个体只有觉察到偏离内稳态的信号, 才有可能修正这种偏差”。而正念可以让个体及时探测到与愤怒有关的线索, 更为恰当地做出反应, 从而避免自动化反应带来的一系列问题(Wright et al., 2009)。Chrisman, Christopher和Lichtenstein (2009)的一项质性研究以八段锦(气功的一种)作为正念练习的一种方法, 对心理咨询专业的研究生进行了为期15周的气功训练, 结果发现, 被试在身体、情绪及心理方面都有改善, 比如平衡感更好、呼吸更深、情绪更平 静、注意力更集中、意识更清晰等。作者从中医“气”的角度解释气功的作用机制, 认为疾病是由体内“气”的失衡、不通, 机体内稳态遭到破坏导致的; 而长期的气功练习可以消除阻塞, 使全身的“气”流更为通畅, 恢复机体固有的内稳态, 最终得以实现身心健康。还有研究者认为, 积极重评的能力弱会导致个体在面对挑战时认为自己没有足够的资源应对挑战, 这种压力评价(stress appraisals)的方式将会导致HPA轴的延长、过度激活, 破坏机体整个系统功能的内稳态, 最终导致疾病的发生(Rosmond, 2005)。正念能够增强积极重评(positive reappraisal)的能力, 正念在重评过程中发挥核心作用, 从而降低应激相关疾病的患病风险(Garland, Gaylord, & Park, 2009)。从以上内容可以看出, 目前研究者多用“内稳态”的概念来解释正念在维持机体适应过程中的作用, 还未见有效的方法用以量化“内稳态”的过程, 因此, 本研究将尝试引入一个新的算法表征这个过程。这一新的算法将在后文详细介绍。

以往研究发现, 特质正念与情绪调节过程中HRV变化有密切关系。Braeken, Otte, Nyklicek和Van den Bergh (2012)设置5种条件:静息态-心算任务-图片放松-心算任务-音乐放松, 考察150名孕期妇女的特质正念与HRV (RMSSD、HF) 的关系, 结果显示, 高特质正念孕期妇女的HRV在平静休息状态和心算任务状态下都比低特质正念孕期妇女高。Garland (2011)检测了58名酒精依赖住院患者的特质正念、酒精注意偏好及HRV (HF)的关系, 结果发现高特质正念的酒精依赖者在酒精注意偏好任务中表现出对酒精更少的注意偏好, 任务结束后恢复阶段的HRV更高。Fogarty等人(2015)的研究通过书写任务诱发被试的负性情绪, 发现高特质正念个体在情绪恢复阶段有较高的HRV (HF), 结果表明特质正念预测负性情绪反应之后较好的恢复。Mankus, Aldao, Kerns, Mayville和Mennin (2013)的研究探讨高低广泛性焦虑患者在问卷填写时特质正念与HRV的关系, 发现较高广泛性焦虑患者的特质正念与HRV (MSD, 连续相邻心跳间差异绝对值的平均值, 是HRV的一个时域指标)显著相关, 而低广泛性焦虑患者的特质正念和HRV相关并不显著。

然而, 也有研究并没有发现特质正念与HRV之间存在显著相关。Jäger (2016)以106名大学生为被试, 发现特质正念与静息态的HRV (SDNN、LF、VLF)相关不显著。Soer, Jong, Hofstra, Preuper和Reneman (2015)对10名慢性肌肉骨骼痛患者和15名健康被试进行3次HRV协调性(HRV coherence)的训练, 训练前后的HRV协调性及特质正念都有提升, 但HRV协调性的变化值与特质正念的变化值之间的相关并不显著。

1.3 问题提出

HRV的指标有很多, 但研究者在探讨特质正念与HRV的关系时, 所选用的HRV指标只局限在某一两个(例如, Braeken et al., 2012; Jäger, 2016), 而且不同的研究所选用的指标并不一致。不仅如此, 以往研究在探讨HRV的指标在不同环节(静息态、任务态及任务态后的恢复)的变化时, 并没有考虑指标之间的联动关系。例如, Braeken等人(2012)的研究只分析HRV (RMSSD和HF)与特质正念的相关, 但是没有考察HRV多项指标协同、系统变化的模式。因此, 以往研究还不能全面系统地反映自主神经系统的活动与特质正念的关系。本研究将引入较为全面的HRV指标, 而且将引入“自相似” (self-similarity)的概念和方法, 考察HRV指标在变化过程中保持自相似的程度与个体特质正念之间的关系, 尝试揭示正念起作用的生理机制。

自相似是指一个模式不随空间尺度或时间尺度的变化而变化(Muzzio, Swanson, & Ottino, 1992), 表现为一个集合(set)以相同的模式在不同的空间尺度或时间尺度上重复它自己(Galatzer-Levy, 1995)。自相似广泛存在于自然现象中, 例如植物的生长、大小瓷器碎片的关系等。自相似也存在于精神分析的文本材料中。Galatzer-Levy (1995)从分钟、小时、月和年4个时间尺度展示一个强迫症患者的文本材料, 发现患者在这4个尺度上表现出极为相似的强迫模式。将HRV各指标在特定状态下的变化理解为某种分布或某种集合, 如果HRV各指标之间的联动关系在不同状态下表现出时间不变性或者重复了它自己, 那么就说明HRV指标之间是自相似的。

一个系统的自相似表征的是系统参数(也称指标)之间的联动关系(West, 2017)。如式(1)所示, 一个变化过程中两个参数的自相似联动关系用幂函数表示(West, 2017)。

\[{{y}_{2}}/{{y}_{1}}={{({{x}_{2}}/{{x}_{1}})}^{SSE(1,2)}} (1)\]

其中的指数称为自相似指数(self-similar exponent, SSE)。引入两个参数在不同状态下以黄金分割常数 \({\text{τ}} = (\sqrt{5}-1)/2≈0.618\)为底的过程对数(刘承宜等, 2016, 2017; Liu et al., 2017)。

\[{{l}_{y}}(1,2)=\text{lo}{{\text{g}}_{\tau }}({{y}_{2}}/{{y}_{1}}),{{l}_{x}}(1,2)=\text{lo}{{\text{g}}_{\tau }}({{x}_{2}}/{{x}_{1}}) (2)\]

从式(1)可得,

\[SSE(1,2)={{l}_{y}}(1,2)/{{l}_{x}}(1,2) (3)\]

过程对数的绝对值称为定量差异(quantitative diff.erence, QD, 刘承宜等, 2016, 2017; Liu et al., 2017)。(3)式表明, SSE确实可以表征两个参数之间的联动关系。

系统参数之间的联动关系可以表现为系统整体功能的稳定性。QD和自相似的生物学意义可以用功能内稳态(function-specific homeostasis, FSH)和稳态应变(allostasis)来阐述, 是指机体为适应内外环境的变化而作出的主动偏离内稳态的调整(Liu et al., 2012, 2014; Sterling & Eyer, 1988), 是一种在变化过程中维持的稳态(Sterling, 2012)。功能内稳态和稳态应变是维持功能充分稳定发挥的负反馈机制, 保证机体适应内外环境的变化。功能内稳态与内稳态概念的区别在于, 内稳态强调某项参数的稳定性, 而功能内稳态强调功能的稳定性。某项参数的内稳态可以针对该参数设计研究。机体某项功能的内稳态由大量参数之间的互动来表征, 目前来看只有利用自相似的方法才能揭示这种互动关系。可以假设, 正念调整的就是HRV参数之间的互动关系。

每个功能的QD显著性阈值用(α, β)表示, QD大于α或β称为显著性或非常显著性差异。研究者(刘承宜等人, 2016, 2017; Liu et al., 2017)拟合流行病学研究数据发现, 心理神经水平的QD显著性阈值为(0.80, 1.22)。本研究采用的HRV指标为心理神经水平的功能, 因此, HRV指标之间在不同变化过程中的QD < 0.80时, 即为HRV指标之间在不同的过程维持了自相似。

由以上分析可以看出, 自相似的算法能够呈现动态变化过程中HRV各指标之间的联动关系, 表征机体的功能内稳态。通过全然的觉知(正念), 个体能够使自身功能得到充分稳定的发挥。因此, 本研究借助自相似的算法, 将HRV代表的自主神经系统功能内稳态的概念和理论进行量化, 探讨特质正念与HRV自相似之间的关系, 尝试揭示正念维持适应的作用机制。研究假设, 特质正念与HRV自相似正相关。研究以健康大学生为被试, 设置4种不同的条件:静息态-冷加压-冷加压后恢复-正念练习, 记录被试在这4种不同条件下的HRV, 考察特质正念与不同条件下HRV的关系。具体的研究目的如下:1)呈现不同状态下健康大学生HRV的变化情况; 2)探讨不同状态下特质正念与HRV的关系; 3)考察特质正念与变化过程中HRV自相似的关系。

2 方法

2.1 被试

通过微信群招募广东某高校56名在校大学生(41名女生), 年龄18~20岁, M = 19.14, SD = 0.59。全部没有正念练习或禅修经验。

2.2 测量工具、实验材料与仪器

2.2.1 测量工具

正念注意觉知量表(Mindfulness Attention Awareness Scale, MAAS)用以测量个体的特质正念(Brown, Ryan, Loverich, Biegel, & West, 2011; Quaglia et al., 2016), 该量表由Brown和Ryan (2003)编制, 中文版由Deng等(2012)修订; 单维度结构, 15个条目, 6点评分, “1”到“6”表示“几乎总是”到“几乎从不”; 分数越高越正念。条目举例:“我发现自己沉浸在对未来的幻想或对过去的回忆中。”及“我发现静下心来关注当前发生的事情有些困难。”在众多测量特质正念的工具中, MAAS的应用最为广泛(Park, Reilly-Spong, & Gross, 2013)。大量研究表明, MAAS在不同文化、不同正念冥想经验的人群有良好的信效度(Brown & Ryan, 2003; Carlson & Brown, 2005; Deng et al., 2012; Mac Killop & Anderson, 2007)。本研究中, 该量表56名被试的内部一致性系数α为0.89。

2.2.2 实验材料

正念音频为安卓版手机灵犀App童慧琦呼吸空间练习, 时长5.4 min。呼吸空间练习是正念认知疗法(mindfulness-based cognitive therapy, MBCT; Segal, Teasdale, Williams, & Gemar, 2002)的一个核心练习, 分为三步:觉察-集中-扩展(Segal, Williams, & Teasdale, 2012)。第一步, 觉察当下的体验, 包括想法、情绪和感觉; 第二步, 将注意力集中到呼吸的感觉上, 特别是腹部的感觉; 第三步, 将对呼吸的觉知扩展到整个身体。

2.2.3 实验仪器

HeartMath公司开发生产的emWave pro plus系统, 硬件主要是一个夹耳朵的传感器, 连接PC的USB接口; 软件版本号:3.6.0.9625, 用来记录并转换心脏节律(heart rhythms)数据。系统采样率为370 Hz, 自动进行脉搏波的探测与校准。研究用到的指标有:每分钟平均心率(heart rate, HR), 常用的HRV时域分析指标SDNN和RMSSD, 常用的HRV频域分析指标TP、LF-HRV、HF-HRV、LF/HF ratio和coherence (HRV指标具体含义见引言部分)。由于HR与HRV都可表征自主神经系统的活动, 因此放在一起进行统计分析。

冷加压实验是实验室常用的安全有效的痛觉诱发方法(Zeltzer, Fanurik, & Lebaron, 1989)。本研究所用仪器与工具包括塑料桶(直径22 mm, 高 23 mm)、电子测温仪和制冰机。实验前将水温调至4~6℃ (Liu, Wang, Chang, Chen, & Si, 2013)。冷加压实验的具体流程见2.3研究程序的指导语。

2.3 研究程序

被试进入接待室, 首先填写知情同意书, 研究协议由学校伦理审查委员会审批通过。之后被试需要用微信扫描二维码链接到问卷星, 填写性别、年龄等个人资料, 完成MAAS。

接下来被试被带入实验室, 首先了解实验的4个环节:静息5 min, 冷加压实验3 min, 冷加压实验后恢复5 min, 正念练习5.4 min。主试确认被试清楚实验任务之后, 打开emWave Pro Plus的软件, 在被试的右耳耳垂上夹好传感器, 然后开始正式实验。具体流程(见图1)及实验指导语如下:

环节1:“安静时5分钟心率记录。请舒适地坐在椅子上, 尽量保持头部和身体不动, 放松, 清醒, 但也不要思考。”

环节2:“冷加压实验。这个实验测试的是忍受疼痛的程度, 以手放在冷水中的时间长度为衡量指标。这是实验室常用的安全有效的痛觉诱发方法, 需要你把左手放进水里, 五指分开, 水要没过手腕。当你把手放到水里之后, 可能会感到不舒服, 但我们希望你能尽可能长时间地把手放在水里。一般来说, 以1分钟为下限, 3分钟为上限。但当你感觉非常难以忍受或非常疼痛的时候, 也可以随时点

“stop”, 把手拿出水中, 结束实验。实验结束后, 请用纸巾将手擦干。当你准备好了, 请先把手放入水中, 然后点“start”开始实验。”

环节3:“实验后的5分钟心率记录。请保持平静、放松, 自然地呼吸。”

环节4:“请跟着录音的指示做练习。”

整个研究过程持续35~45分钟, 室温维持在19~22℃; 被试获得课外体育积分或15元报酬。

图1

图1   研究流程图


图2

图2   自相似计算示意图(以HF-HRV和LF-HRV为例)


2.4 数据处理——自相似的计算

本研究设置了4种条件:静息态(环节1)、冷加压(环节2)、冷加压后恢复(环节3)和正念练习(环节4)。环节2是冷加压状态, 其HR和HRV各指标均为最高点(见图2), 选为计算的起点。

具体来讲, HRV自相似的计算步骤如下:

\[{{l}_{x}}(2,i)=\text{lt}({{x}_{i}}/{{x}_{2}})={{\log }_{\tau }}({{x}_{i}}/{{x}_{2}}),i=3,4. (4)\]

\[{{l}_{y}}(2,i)=\text{lt(}{{y}_{i}}/{{y}_{2}}\text{)}={{\log }_{\tau }}({{y}_{i}}/{{y}_{2}}),i=3,4. (5)\]

步骤1:根据式(4)和(5), 分别计算LF-HRV和HF-HRV环节3之于环节2以及环节4之于环节2的过程对数, 共计得到4个过程对数。

\[SSE(2,i)={{l}_{y}}(2,i)/{{l}_{x}}(2,i),i=3,\text{ }4. (6)\]

步骤2:根据式(6), 计算LF-HRV和HF-HRV在环节2~3与环节2~4相应过程对数的自相似指数SSE, 共计得到2个SSE。

\[QD=\left| \frac{SSE(2,3)+SSE(2,4)}{2}\ln \frac{SSE(2,3)}{SSE(2,4)} \right| (7)\]

步骤3:根据式(7), 计算2个SSE之间的定量差异QD, 共计得到1个QD。为了使QD尽可能大, (6)式中所得到的过程对数相除方法尽可能让SSE大于1。至此, 完成了LF-HRV和HF-HRV这一对参数在环节2~3与环节2~4的自相似计算。

步骤4:重复步骤1~3, 完成所有HR和HRV两两参数对之间的过程对数、SSE以及QD计算, 这样一个被试的HRV自相似计算完成。

步骤5:所有被试重复步骤1~4, 完成所有被试HRV自相似的计算。

HRV的活动属于心理神经水平的功能, 因此当QD < 0.80时, 就说明参数对在不同过程中保持了自相似; 反之, 当QD > 0.80时, 说明参数对在不同过程中没有保持自相似。本研究采用HR和HRV指标(SDNN、RMSSD、TP、LF-HRV、HF-HRV、LF/HF ratio和coherence)共计8个参数。经过计算, 每个被试可以得到C82 = (8×7)/2 = 28个QD。每个被试的HRV自相似程度最终以QD < 0.80的参数对的数量来表示。因此最后一个步骤如下:步骤6:确定每个被试QD < 0.80的HRV参数对数量。

自相似的计算用Excel 2010完成。

由于HR和HRV都可表征自主神经系统的活动, HR又是计算HRV的基础和依据, 因此本文所提到的“HRV自相似”指的是将HR的数据纳入计算的HRV自相似。

3 结果

3.1 不同环节自主神经系统各指标的平均数、标准差

4个环节被试自主神经系统各指标的平均数、标准差见表1

表1   不同环节自主神经系统各指标的平均数和标准差(n = 56)

指标环节1: 静息态环节2: 冷加压环节3: 冷加压后恢复环节4: 正念练习
HR78.12 ± 12.0982.70 ± 11.8174.82 ± 11.8875.50 ± 12.11
SDNN82.87 ± 41.72108.96 ± 55.9282.42 ± 33.2979.03 ± 25.61
RMSSD84.51 ± 57.66120.79 ± 76.5283.51 ± 47.5271.61 ± 33.95
TP2233.91 ± 4210.024497.88 ± 6378.482088.89 ± 2448.301901.19 ± 1268.06
LF-HRV1004.59 ± 1985.802521.39 ± 4099.93803.93 ± 1102.43867.86 ± 782.57
HF-HRV531.60 ± 681.381079.55 ± 1411.91503.92 ± 494.46493.10 ± 485.91
LF/HF ratio1.74 ± 1.331.92 ± 1.131.87 ± 2.312.94 ± 3.67
coherence38.23 ± 8.8336.15 ± 8.8336.29 ± 9.1941.48 ± 9.56

注: HR = heart rate, 每分钟平均心率; SDNN = the standard deviation of the normal (NN) sinus, 连续正常RR间期的标准差; RMSSD = the root mean square of successive differences between normal heartbeats, 相邻RR间期之差的均方根; TP = total power, 总功率; LF = low frequency, 低频; HRV = heart rate variability, 心率变异性; HF = high frequency, 高频。下同。

新窗口打开| 下载CSV


重复测量方差分析结果显示, HR在不同环节差异显著, F(3,165) = 28.14, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.34。事后检验表明, 静息态的HR高于冷加压后恢复和正念练习环节; 冷加压环节的HR高于静息态、冷加压后恢复和正念练习环节 (图3a)。

图3

图3   HRV各指标在不同环节的重复测量方差分析结果
注:*表示p < 0.05, **表示p < 0.01, ***表示p < 0.001, 下同


SDNN在不同环节差异显著, F(3,165) = 11.02, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.17。事后检验表明, 冷加压环节SDNN高于静息态、冷加压后恢复和正念练习环节 (图3b)。

RMSSD在不同环节差异显著, F(3,165) = 15.56, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.22。事后检验表明, 冷加压环节的RMSSD高于静息态、冷加压后恢复和正念练习环节; 正念练习环节的RMSSD低于静息态和冷加压后恢复环节 (图3c)。

TP在不同环节差异显著, F(3,165) = 6.45, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.11。事后检验表明, 冷加压环节的TP高于静息态、冷加压后恢复和正念练习环节 (图3d)。

LF-HRV在不同环节差异显著, F(3,165) = 7.61, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.12。事后检验表明, 冷加压环节的LF-HRV高于静息态、冷加压后恢复和正念练习环节 (图3e)。

HF-HRV在不同环节差异显著, F(3,165) = 7.99, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.13。事后检验表明, 冷加压环节的HF-HRV高于静息态、冷加压后恢复和正念练习环节 (图3f)。

LF/HF ratio在不同环节差异显著, F(3,165) = 3.88, p = 0.010, ηp2 = 0.07。事后检验表明, 正念练习环节的LF/HF ratio高于静息态、冷加压和冷加压恢复环节 (图3g)。

coherence在不同环节差异显著, F(3,165) = 6.04, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.10。正念练习时的coherence高于静息态、冷加压和冷加压后恢复环节 (图3h)。

3.2 特质正念与4个环节自主神经系统各指标的相关

56名被试的MAAS总分为51.64 ± 12.14。Pearson相关结果(见表2)显示, MAAS总分与静息态的自主神经系统各指标相关不显著, 与正念练习各指标的相关也不显著; MAAS总分与冷加压环节的TP和HF-HRV显著正相关, 与RMSSD和LF-HRV正相关边缘显著; MAAS总分与冷加压后恢复环节的HF-HRV显著正相关, 与LF/HF ratio与coherence显著负相关。

表2   MAAS总分与4个环节自主神经系统各指标的相关(n = 56)

指标MAAS总分与自主神经系统各指标的皮尔逊相关系数r
环节1: 静息态环节2: 冷加压环节3: 冷加压后恢复环节4: 正念练习
HR0.017-0.0750.0400.076
SDNN0.1820.2160.1440.007
RMSSD0.1920.2530.2160.091
TP0.2300.329*0.191-0.054
LF-HRV0.2260.2490.103-0.136
HF-HRV0.1770.307*0.276*0.022
LH/HF ratio-0.0030.010-0.278*-0.116
Coherence-0.123-0.077-0.266*-0.106

注:†表示p < 0.08, *表示p < 0.05,**表示p < 0.01,***表示p < 0.001。

新窗口打开| 下载CSV


表3   高低自相似组HRV各指标在不同环节的平均数和标准差(M ± SD)

环节指标
SDNNRMSSDTPLF-HRVHF-HRV
环节1: 静息态
低自相似组(n=31)81.80 ± 38.8878.68 ± 50.942178.93 ± 3955.731107.73 ± 2080.27494.98 ± 552.94
高自相似组(n=25)84.20 ± 45.7991.74 ± 65.392302.07 ± 4587.96876.69 ± 1896.51577.00 ± 823.34
环节2: 冷加压
低自相似组(n=31)93.41 ± 42.0798.08 ± 60.112618.21 ± 3296.851542.05 ± 2395.99676.75 ± 945.63
高自相似组(n=25)128.23 ± 65.23149.00 ± 86.086828.65 ± 8332.313735.76 ± 5345.151579.01 ± 1726.61
环节3: 冷加压后恢复
低自相似组(n=31)81.64 ± 33.8978.78 ± 44.842403.22 ± 3037.06974.99 ± 1389.88480.25 ± 562.25
高自相似组(n=25)83.41 ± 35.6089.36 ± 50.971699.11 ± 1386.02591.80 ± 534.58533.25 ± 404.34
环节4: 正念练习
低自相似组(n=31)81.91 ± 26.3270.61 ± 33.092131.67 ± 1294.551026.77 ± 924.48489.30 ± 544.08
高自相似组(n=25)75.47 ± 24.7772.86 ± 35.631615.38 ± 1198.50670.80 ± 511.91497.81 ± 413.52

新窗口打开| 下载CSV


3.3 特质正念与HRV自相似的关系

本研究中, 神经心理水平功能的QD阈值α为0.80, 参数对之间的QD小于0.80表示满足自相似。小于QD阈值α的参数对的平均数量为5.18 ± 4.30, 取值范围为0~18。MAAS总分与满足自相似的参数对的数量相关显著, 斯皮尔曼相关系数ρ = 0.27, p = 0.047。

为了进一步分析HRV自相似与特质正念的关系以及HRV自相似在不同环节中对HRV各指标的影响, 以HRV自相似为依据进行分组。将自相似参数对数量小于或等于5和大于5分别定义为低自相似和高自相似。低自相似组31人, MAAS总分为48.64 ± 9.91; 高自相似组25人, MAAS总分为55.36 ± 13.75。独立样本t检验结果表明, MAAS总分在高低自相似组差异显著, t(54) = 2.12, p = 0.038, Cohen°s d = 0.57 (图4)。

图4

图4   高、低自相似组在MAAS总分上的差异


3.4 高低自相似分组在不同环节HRV各指标上的差异

以下为高、低自相似组HRV各指标在不同环节中的差异。

重复测量方差分析结果显示, 环节对SDNN主效应显著, F(3,162) = 13.40, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.20; 高低自相似组与不同环节对SDNN交互作用显著, F(3,162) = 5.06, p = 0.002, η2 = 0.09。简单效应分析结果显示, 冷加压环节高自相似组的SDNN高于低自相似组, F(1,54) = 6.84, p = 0.019, ηp2 =.0.10; 其他环节高低自相似组的SDNN没有显著差异 (图5a)。

环节对RMSSD主效应显著, F(3,162) = 18.09, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.25; 高低自相似组与不同环节对RMSSD交互作用显著, F(3,162) = 4.20, p = 0.007, ηp2 = 0.07。简单效应分析结果显示, 冷加压环节高自相似组的RMSSD高于低自相似组, F(1,54) = 6.76, p = 0.012, ηp2 = 0.11; 其他环节高低自相似组的RMSSD没有显著差异 (图5b)。

环节对TP主效应显著, F(3,162) = 8.47, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.14; 高低自相似组与不同环节对TP交互作用显著, F(3,162) = 6.30, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.10。简单效应分析结果显示, 冷加压环节高自相似组的TP高于低自相似组, F(1,54) = 6.65, p = 0.01, ηp2 = 0.11; 其他环节高低自相似组的TP没有显著差异 (图5c)。

环节对LF-HRV主效应显著, F(3,162) = 9.47, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.15; 高低自相似组与不同环节对LF-HRV交互作用显著, F(3,162) = 4.76, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.08。简单效应分析结果显示, 冷加压环节高自相似组的LF-HRV高于低自相似组, F(1,54) = 4.19, p = 0.045, ηp2 = 0.20; 其他环节高低自相似组的LF-HRV没有显著差异 (图5d)。

环节对HF-HRV主效应显著, F(3,162) = 9.84, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.15; 高低自相似组与不同环节对HF-HRV交互作用显著, F(3,162) = 4.74, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.08。简单效应分析结果显示, 冷加压环节高自相似组的HF-HRV高于低自相似组, F(1,54) = 6.18, p = 0.016, ηp2 = 0.10; 其他环节高低自相似组的HF-HRV没有显著差异 (图5e)。

4 讨论

正念在维持适应方面的作用已被大量研究所证明, 也有不少研究尝试探讨正念在适应过程中的作用机制。本研究聚焦在特质正念与自主神经系统活动的关系方面, 用HRV表征自主神经系统的活动。综合以往研究可以看到, 前人在探讨特质正念与HRV的关系时, 所用的HRV指标仅局限于其中的几个(例如Braeken et al., 2012; Jäger, 2016), 且没有考虑不同状态下HRV指标之间的联动关 系。本研究认为, 机体作为一个系统, 在面对不同的刺激时, 多个不同指标之间的联动作用更能真 实、全面、系统地反映机体的适应过程。另外, 前人尝试用“内稳态”的概念解释正念在维持机体适应方面的作用机制(例如Chrisman et al., 2009; Hosemans, 2015; Wright et al., 2009), 却无法将“内稳态”的过程进行量化。因此, 本研究引入了自相似的算法和功能内稳态的概念, 通过计算HR和HRV各指标在不同环节的联动关系来表征不同状态下自主神经系统活动的功能内稳态, 将功能内稳态的概念和过程进行量化并首次应用于心理学的研究, 用以探讨正念维持适应的机制, 推进了以往研究。

研究有以下几个发现。最重要的结果在于MAAS总分与冷加压-冷加压后恢复和冷加压-正念练习这两个过程HRV自相似之间显著正相关, 支持了研究假设。结果说明, 特质正念越高, 个体在面对不同刺激时自主神经系统的功能越能保持自相似。自相似的生物学意义在于面对环境变化时保持稳态应变, 也就意味着高自相似的个体, 能够实时调整自己的身心状态及身体机能去适应当前环境的变化, 而这种调整又是基于自身的功能内稳态, 不至于使个体机能过度调高或调低, 超过某种限度。以MAAS总分表征的特质正念测量的是个体的注意觉知能力, 全然的觉知让个体能够探测到身心信号及环境刺激的细微变化, 以及时地作出最为恰当的反应。高自相似组的MAAS总分显著高于低自相似组(见图4); 高自相似组的HRV各指标(SDNN、RMSSD、TP、LF和HF)在冷加压环节皆高于低自相似组(见图5), 而高的HRV表明大脑对外周生理控制更好, 个体适应更好(Force, 1996; Thayer & Lane, 2000, 2009; Thayer et al., 2012; Thayer & Brosschot, 2005)。综合图4图5的结果, 说明高HRV自相似的个体特质正念更高, 在面对不同变化时能够保持功能内稳态, 反应更具适应 性。这同时也说明, 依据HRV自相似对被试进行分组是有效的而且必要的。如图5所示, 冷加压环节, 高HRV自相似组的HRV各指标显著高于低HRV自相似组; 而在冷加压后恢复环节, 高、低HRV自相似组的HRV各指标之间没有显著差异。这个结果说明, 低HRV自相似组对冷加压没有反应, 而高HRV自相似组对冷加压产生了较为有效的反应; 在冷加压后恢复过程, 高HRV自相似组能够更为迅速地恢复。高HRV自相似组在冷加压时HRV各指标升高, 冷加压后恢复环节迅速恢复, 这个过程从另一方面体现了功能内稳态, 而这种稳态体现的又是一种自限性(self-limited)过程, 指机体在应对环境刺激的过程中本身有一种自愈力, 并不需要额外的干预, 就能自动恢复到原来的水平(Bigelow, 1835)。以往研究表明, 正念并不会降低人们对刺激的反应性, 而是让人在反应之后能更快地恢复到初始水平(Greenberg & Meiran, 2014)。这意味着特质正念高的个体, 通过对内外环境实时敏锐的觉察, 不仅可以更有效地对环境刺激做出恰当的反应, 还可以在反应过后更为迅速恢复到初始水平。研究结果更为直接地表明高HRV自相似的个体对冷加压的反应更为强烈、恢复更为迅速。虽然结果3.3显示MAAS总分与HRV自相似之间显著正相关, 但正念在维持功能内稳态的过程中究竟是如何展现自限性过程(自愈力)的, 还需要进一步的研究去揭示。

图5

图5   HRV高低自相似组与不同环节对HRV各指标的交互作用


本研究4个环节的设计能够反映不同状态下自主神经系统的活动。相比于静息态、冷加压后恢复和正念练习环节, HR和HRV的指标(SDNN、RMSSD、TP、LF和HF)在冷加压时最高, 这看起来与以往研究结果有所不同。以往研究发现, 相比于应激状态, 静息态和自我调节的状态下的HRV更高(Grossman & Taylor, 2007), HRV在正念练习时更高(Ditto, Eclache, & Goldman, 2006; Takahashi et al., 2005; Tang et al., 2009)。Burg等人(2012)指出, 正念练习时更高的HRV可能是由于深度放松引起的, 这种深度放松与副交感神经的激活有关。这也从侧面解释了为什么本研究中正念练习阶段的HRV没有升高, 因为被试完全没有正念冥想经验, 面对一个新异刺激无法达到深度放松。Moses, Luecken和Eason (2007)研究3种不同压力任务 下——注意(模式匹配)、冷加压和演讲——HRV从静息态、任务态到恢复过程的变化情况, HRV的指标为HF-HRV和LF-HRV。结果发现, 注意任务条件下, 任务态的HF-HRV和LF-HRV均低于静息态; 而冷加压和演讲条件下, 相比于静息态, 任务态的HF-HRV和LF-HRV的升降变化并不一致。具体表现为, 冷加压条件下, 相比于静息态, 任务态的LF-HRV下降, HF-HRV不变; 演讲条件下, 相比于静息态, 任务态的LF-HRV不变, HF-HRV上升。此研究说明, 不同的压力任务引发的HRV指标变化并不相同。本研究的结果与Moses等人(2007)的研究结果并不一致, Moses等人(2007)发现相比于静息态, 冷加压条件下LF-HRV下降, HF-HRV不变, 而本研究发现冷加压条件下LF-HRV和HF-HRV皆升高。Moses等人(2007)的冷加压设置是将冰袋贴在被试前臂皮肤2分钟, 没有报告诸如室温等其他信息。本研究的设置是让被试将右手(没过手腕)放入4~6℃的冷水中3分钟, 室温19~22℃, 而实验时间为12月至次年1月份, 正值南方的冬季, 天气较为寒冷。是不是这些具体的操作与设置导致了两个研究HRV指标的变化不一致, 需要进一步的研究去验证。本研究中冷加压任务下HRV各指标更高, 说明个体在协调自主神经系统的活动进行自我调节。

研究探讨的另一个问题是特质正念与不同状态下HR和HRV各指标之间的关系。与Jäger (2016)的结果一致, 本研究发现特质正念与静息态的HRV各指标相关不显著。本研究还发现, 特质正念与冷加压后恢复环节的HRV各指标相关不显著, 与正念练习时的HRV各指标相关也不显著, 而coherence这个反映心脏功能和谐稳定的指标在正念练习时最高, 说明正念练习确实能让心脏处于良好的状态。为什么特质正念与正念练习时的HRV之间没有显著相关呢?可能的一个原因在于, 被试皆为从未接触过正念训练、没有任何冥想经验的大学生, 虽然正念练习能让他们的心脏功能处于良好的状态, 但正念练习对他们来说毕竟是一个新异刺激。Evans, Eisenlohr-Moul, Button, Baer和Segerstrom (2014)用冷加压任务诱发被试的急性疼痛, 控制组被试用他们自己熟悉的方式应对疼痛, 实验组被试根据简短的正念指导语的指示应对疼痛, 结果只在控制组发现高的HRV能够预测高的疼痛忍耐度。也就是说, 对于这些完全没有正念经验的被试, 正念作为一种新的不熟悉的策略反而会降低对疼痛的忍耐度。

研究主要有以下不足。第一, 被试都是年轻的大学生, 限制了结论推广到其他年龄段的群体; 第二, 适应的指标主要为HRV, 没有其他的效标变量做参考; 第三, 研究最主要的结果是特质正念(MAAS总分)与HRV自相似之间的相关, 相关关系并不能得出因果关系的推论。

基于本研究的发现及研究的不足之处, 今后考虑从以下两方面进一步推进研究。一, 研究通过引入自相似的算法, 初步得出特质正念与HRV自相似之间的相关, 后续需要更多的数据去验证结果的稳定性。比如, 本研究环节2设置的冷加压任务是比较偏物理性的刺激, 换成更为情绪性的任务或者更为认知性的任务, 是否能得出与本研究同样的结果, 需要进一步验证。二, 本研究从特质正念的角度探讨了正念与自主神经系统活动之间的关系, 由结果可以初步推测, 正念维持适应的机制可能在于维持机体功能的内稳态。正念的概念来源于佛学, 正式的正念练习称为“冥想”或“禅修”。那么, 有着长期冥想经验的禅修者与新手禅修者在不同状态转换时的自主神经系统活动会有怎样的差异?禅修老手在状态切换的过程中是否比禅修新手更能维持自身的功能内稳态?对于这个问题的回答可以更为直接且深入地揭示正念维持适应的机制。

致谢:实验前得到灵犀App呼吸空间音频作者童慧琦老师的授权使用和大力支持, 在此表示感谢。

参考文献

Baer R. A., Smith G. T., & Allen K. B . ( 2004).

Assessment of mindfulness by self-report: The Kentucky inventory of mindfulness skills

Assessment, 11( 3), 191-206.

[本文引用: 2]

Barnes S., Brown K. W., Krusemark E., Campbell W. K., & Rogge R. D . ( 2007).

The role of mindfulness in romantic relationship satisfaction and responses to relationship stress

Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 33( 4), 482-500.

DOI:10.1111/j.1752-0606.2007.00033.x      URL     PMID:17935531      [本文引用: 1]

Two studies examined the role of mindfulness in romantic relationship satisfaction and in responses to relationship stress. Using a longitudinal design, Study 1 found that higher trait mindfulness predicted higher relationship satisfaction and greater capacities to respond constructively to relationship stress. Study 2 replicated and extended these findings. Mindfulness was again shown to relate to relationship satisfaction; then, using a conflict discussion paradigm, trait mindfulness was found to predict lower emotional stress responses and positive pre- and postconflict change in perception of the relationship. State mindfulness was related to better communication quality during the discussion. Both studies indicated that mindfulness may play an influential role in romantic relationship well-being. Discussion highlights future research directions for this new area of inquiry.

Berntson G. G., Bigger T. J., Eckberg D. L., Grossman P., Kaufmann P. G., Malik M., .. VAN Der Molen M. W. ., ( 1997).

Heart rate variability: origins, methods, and interpretive caveats

Psychophysiology, 34( 6), 623-648.

DOI:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02140.x      URL     PMID:9401419      [本文引用: 3]

Components of heart rate variability have attracted considerable attention in psychology and medicine and have become important dependent measures in psychophysiology and behavioral medicine. Quantification and interpretation of heart rate variability, however, remain complex issues and are fraught with pitfalls. The present report (a) examines the physiological origins and mechanisms of heart rate variability, (b) considers quantitative approaches to measurement, and (c) highlights important caveats in the interpretation of heart rate variability. Summary guidelines for research in this area are outlined, and suggestions and prospects for future developments are considered.

Bigelow J. ., (1835).

A Discourse on self-limited diseases. Nathan Hale

[本文引用: 1]

Billman G.E . ( 2011).

Heart rate variability-A historical perspective. Frontiers in Physiology, 2, 86

DOI:10.3389/fphys.2011.00086      URL     PMID:22144961      [本文引用: 1]

Heart rate variability (HRV), the beat-to-beat variation in either heart rate or the duration of the R-R interval – the heart period, has become a popular clinical and investigational tool. The temporal fluctuations in heart rate exhibit a marked synchrony with respiration (increasing during inspiration and decreasing during expiration – the so called respiratory sinus arrhythmia, RSA) and are widely believed to reflect changes in cardiac autonomic regulation. Although the exact contributions of the parasympathetic and the sympathetic divisions of the autonomic nervous system to this variability are controversial and remain the subject of active investigation and debate, a number of time and frequency domain techniques have been developed to provide insight into cardiac autonomic regulation in both health and disease. It is the purpose of this essay to provide an historical overview of the evolution in the concept of heart rate variability. Briefly, pulse rate was first measured by ancient Greek physicians and scientists. However, it was not until the invention of the “Physician’s Pulse Watch” (a watch with a second hand that could be stopped) in 1707 that changes in pulse rate could be accurately assessed. The Rev. Stephen Hales (1733) was the first to note that pulse varied with respiration and in 1847 Carl Ludwig was the first to record RSA. With the measurement of the ECG (1895) and advent of digital signal processing techniques in the 1960’s, investigation of HRV and its relationship to health and disease has exploded. This essay will conclude with a brief description of time domain, frequency domain, and non-linear dynamic analysis techniques (and their limitations) that are commonly used to measure heart rate variability.

Bishop S. R., Lau M., Shapiro S., Carlson L., Anderson N. D., Carmody J., … Devins G . ( 2004).

Mindfulness: a proposed operational definition

Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11( 3), 230-241.

DOI:10.1093/clipsy.bph077      URL     [本文引用: 1]

There has been substantial interest in mindfulness as an approach to reduce cognitive vulnerability to stress and emotional distress in recent years. However, thus far mindfulness has not been defined operationally. This paper describes the results of recent meetings held to establish a consensus on mindfulness and to develop conjointly a testable operational definition. We propose a two-component model of mindfulness and specify each component in terms of specific behaviors, experiential manifestations, and implicated psychological processes. We then address issues regarding temporal stability and situational specificity and speculate on the conceptual and operational distinctiveness of mindfulness. We conclude this paper by discussing implications for instrument development and briefly describing our own approach to measurement.

Braeken M. A. K. A., Otte R. A., Nyklicek I., &Van den Bergh B. R. H. M. ., ( 2012).

Mindful during pregnancy: a positive relation with heart rate variability

Psychophysiology, 49( S1), S111-S111.

[本文引用: 2]

Brown K.W., &Ryan , R. M . ( 2003).

The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84( 4), 822-848.

DOI:10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822      URL     PMID:12703651      [本文引用: 3]

Mindfulness is an attribute of consciousness long believed to promote well-being. This research provides a theoretical and empirical examination of the role of mindfulness in psychological well-being. The development and psychometric properties of the dispositional Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) are described. Correlational, quasi-experimental, and laboratory studies then show that the MAAS measures a unique quality of consciousness that is related to a variety of well-being constructs, that differentiates mindfulness practitioners from others, and that is associated with enhanced self-awareness. An experience-sampling study shows that both dispositional and state mindfulness predict self-regulated behavior and positive emotional states. Finally, a clinical intervention study with cancer patients demonstrates that increases in mindfulness over time relate to declines in mood disturbance and stress.

Brown K. W., Ryan R. M., Loverich T. M., Biegel G. M., & West A. M . ( 2011).

Out of the armchair and into the streets: measuring mindfulness advances knowledge and improves interventions: reply to Grossman (2011)

Psychological Assessment ,23(4), 1041-1046.

DOI:10.1037/a0025781      URL     [本文引用: 1]

ABSTRACT We address 3 critiques raised by Grossman (2011) of self-report measures of mindfulness and the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) and Mindful Attention Awareness Scale-Adolescent (MAAS-A) in particular. Grossman questioned whether self-report measures actually assess mindfulness, whether the construct of mindfulness can be understood apart from mindfulness training, and whether there is empirical evidence to support the validity of mindfulness measures. In response we discuss established theory that attention (and secondarily meta-awareness) is core to the meaning of mindfulness and is the central feature of the MAAS and MAAS-A. We then argue that mindfulness is an inherent capacity that varies between and within persons and is not, as Grossman claimed, a concept applicable to only a trained few. Further, as assessed by the MAAS and MAAS-A, mindfulness is associated with the same variety of outcomes as mindfulness training is theorized to yield. Finally, we provide considerable evidence that the MAAS and MAAS-A are valid instruments. We conclude that although construct measurement is inevitably imperfect, such efforts are critical to building basic knowledge and refining effective interventions. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved).

Burg J. M., Wolf O. T., & Michalak J . ( 2012).

Mindfulness as self-regulated attention: Associations with heart rate variability

Swiss Journal of Psychology, 71(3), 135-139.

DOI:10.1024/1421-0185/a000080      URL    

ABSTRACT Empirical research has demonstrated associations between heart rate variability (HRV) and the regulation of emotion and behavior. Similarly, self-regulation of attention to one’s experience of the present moment in an accepting and nonjudgmental manner is an essential characteristic of mindfulness that promotes emotional and behavioral regulation and psychological well-being. The present study investigated the relationship between mindfulness and HRV. A total of 23 undergraduate psychology students completed a recently developed measure of mindfulness, the mindful breathing exercise (MBE), which assesses the ability to mindfully stay in contact with one’s breath during breathing meditation. Moreover, indices of HRV were measured during a short version of the MBE. As predicted, positive correlations were found between indices of HRV and mindfulness. The findings demonstrate that the ability to mindfully regulate one’s attention is associated with higher HRV, a physiological correlate of physical and psychological health, and therefore support on a physiological level the potential benefit of the implemented mindfulness exercises in mindfulness-based clinical interventions.

, Carlson L.E., &Brown K.W, . ( 2005).

Validation of the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale in a cancer population

Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 58( 1), 29-33.

DOI:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2004.04.366      URL     PMID:15771867      [本文引用: 1]

This study examined the construct and criterion validity of the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) in cancer outpatients, using matched community members as controls. Cancer outpatients ( n=122) applying for enrollment in a mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) program completed the MAAS and measures of mood disturbance and stress. Local community members ( n=122) matched to the patients on gender, age, and education level completed the same measures. The single-factor structure of the MAAS was invariant across the groups. Higher MAAS scores were associated with lower mood disturbance and stress symptoms in cancer patients, and the structure of these relations was invariant across groups. The MAAS appears to have appropriate application in research examining the role of mindfulness in the psychological well-being of cancer patients, with or without comparisons to nonclinical controls.

Chambers R., Gullone E., & Allen N. B . ( 2009).

Mindful emotion regulation: an integrative review

.Clinical Psychology Review, 29(6),560-572.

DOI:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.06.005      URL     PMID:19632752      [本文引用: 2]

This review aims to integrate the constructs of mindfulness and emotion regulation. Research into both of these areas is relatively new, and while several reviews have emerged for each area independently, none has directly proposed a conceptual integration. The current review explores how key axioms and assumptions of traditional psychological models of emotion regulation and the psychological interventions that are derived from them (e.g., cognitive behavior therapy) differ fundamentally from mindfulness-based approaches in terms of the underlying processes they address. Accordingly, mindfulness and emotion regulation are each reviewed, followed by a conceptual integration. Fundamental difficulties arising from the attempt to integrate the two domains are highlighted, especially as to the “reality” of thoughts, the relationship between thoughts and emotions, and the need to move beyond a valence model of emotion. Finally, a model is proposed outlining the likely critical processes and mechanisms that underlie “mindful emotion regulation.”

Chrisman J. A., Christopher J. C., & Lichtenstein S. J . ( 2009).

Qigong as a mindfulness practice for counseling students: A qualitative study

Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 49( 2), 236-257.

DOI:10.1177/0022167808327750      URL     [本文引用: 1]

ABSTRACT This qualitative study explores the effects of qigong, an ancient Chinese mindfulness practice involving movement, on master's-level counseling students. Students responded in writing both after an initial experience of qigong and after practicing the movements for 15 weeks during a mindfulness-based course in self-care. Themes of physical, emotional, and mental changes were present in both sets of responses. Additional themes of familiarity with the practice as well as group consciousness and interdependence emerged in the final experience of qigong. The results of this study indicate qigong is a contemplative practice that could have positive outcomes for counseling students. Because of its accessible nature, immediate results, and ability to foster connectedness, qigong is currently underutilized as a form of teaching mindfulness.

Creswell J. D. ( 2015). Biological pathways linking mindfulness with health. In Brown, K. W., Creswell, J. D., & Ryan, R. M. (Eds.). Handbook of mindfulness: theory, research, and practice. (pp.426-440). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

[本文引用: 1]

Creswell J.D., &Lindsay E.K, . ( 2014).

How does mindfulness training affect health? A mindfulness stress buffering account

Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23( 6), 401-407.

DOI:10.1177/0963721414547415      URL     Magsci    

Abstract Initial well-controlled studies have suggested that mindfulness training interventions can improve a broad range of mental and physical health outcomes (e.g., HIV pathogenesis, depression relapse, inflammation, drug abuse), yet the underlying pathways linking mindfulness and health are poorly understood. In this article, we offer a mindfulness stress buffering account to explain these health outcomes, which posits that mindfulness-based health effects are mostly likely to be observed in high-stress populations for which stress is known to affect the onset or exacerbation of disease pathogenic processes. We then offer an evidence-based biological model of mindfulness, stress buffering, and health.

Creswell J. D., Myers H. F., Cole S. W., & Irwin M. R . ( 2009).

Mindfulness meditation training effects on Cd4+ T lymphocytes in HIV-1 infected adults: a small randomized controlled trial

Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 23( 2), 184-188.

DOI:10.1016/j.bbi.2008.07.004      URL     PMID:18678242     

Mindfulness meditation training has stress reduction benefits in various patient populations, but its effects on biological markers of HIV-1 progression are unknown. The present study tested the efficacy of an 8-week Mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) meditation program compared to a 1-day control seminar on CD4+ T lymphocyte counts in stressed HIV infected adults. A single-blind randomized controlled trial was conducted with enrollment and follow-up occurring between November 2005 and December 2007. A diverse community sample of 48 HIV-1 infected adults was randomized and entered treatment in either an 8-week MBSR or a 1-day control stress reduction education seminar. The primary outcome was circulating counts of CD4+ T lymphocytes. Participants in the 1-day control seminar showed declines in CD4+ T lymphocyte counts whereas counts among participants in the 8-week MBSR program were unchanged from baseline to post-intervention (time treatment condition interaction, p=.02). This effect was independent of antiretroviral (ARV) medication use. Additional analyses indicated that treatment adherence to the mindfulness meditation program, as measured by class attendance, mediated the effects of mindfulness meditation training on buffering CD4+ T lymphocyte declines. These findings provide an initial indication that mindfulness meditation training can buffer CD4+ T lymphocyte declines in HIV-1 infected adults. Clinical Trials Registration: clinicaltrials.gov, Identifier: NCT00600561.

Demarzo M. M. P., Montero-Marin J., Stein P. K., Cebolla A., Provinciale J. G., & García-Campayo J . ( 2014).

Mindfulness may both moderate and mediate the effect of physical fitness on cardiovascular responses to stress: A speculative hypothesis

Frontiers in Physiology, 5( 105), 1-8.

DOI:10.3389/fphys.2014.00105      URL     PMID:3971190      [本文引用: 1]

The psychological construct of mindfulness refers to an awareness that emerges by intentionally paying attention to the present experience in a non-judgmental or evaluative way. This particular quality of awareness has been associated to several indicators of physical and psychological health, and can be developed using mindfulness-based interventions (MBIs), and therefore MBIs have been successfully applied as preventive and complementary interventions and therapies in medicine and psychology. Together with quiet sitting and lying meditation practices, mindful physical exercises such as “mindful walking” and “mindful movement” are key elements in MBIs and couple muscular activity with an internally directed focus, improving interoceptive attention to bodily sensations. In addition, MBIs seem to share similar mechanisms with physical fitness by which they may influence cardiovascular responses to stress. Based on these facts, it is feasible to raise the question of whether physical training itself may induce the development of that particular quality of awareness associated with mindfulness, or if one’s dispositional mindfulness (the tendency to be more mindful in daily life) could moderate the effects of exercise on cardiovascular response to stress. The role of mindfulness as a mediator or moderator of the effect of exercise training on cardiovascular responses to stress has barely been studied. In this study, we have hypothesized pathways (moderation and mediation) by which mindfulness could significantly influence the effects of physical fitness on cardiovascular responses to stress and have discuss potential practical ways to test these hypotheses.

Deng Y. Q., Li S., Tang Y. Y., Zhu L. H., Ryan R., & Brown K . ( 2012).

Psychometric properties of the Chinese translation of the mindful attention awareness scale (MAAS)

Mindfulness, 3( 1), 10-14.

DOI:10.1007/s12671-011-0074-1      URL     Magsci     [本文引用: 1]

AbstractThe present study examined the reliability and validity of a Chinese translation of the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS). Three questionnaires, the MAAS, the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS), and the brief version of the World Health Organization's Quality of Life (WHOQOL-BREF), were completed by 263 Chinese undergraduates (207 males, 56 females). Seventy of these students were assessed again with the MAAS after 2002days to evaluate the scale's test–retest reliability. Results from confirmatory factory analysis indicated that a one-factor solution fit the MAAS data satisfactorily. Reliability coefficients, including Cronbach's alpha, Guttman split-half, item–total correlations, and test–retest, were also satisfactory. Addressing validity, the MAAS was negatively correlated with PANAS negative affect and positively associated with PANAS positive affect as well as with the quality of life indexed by the WHOQOL-BREF. The Chinese version of the MAAS appears to be a reliable and valid instrument to assess levels of mindfulness in a Chinese college population.

Didonna F.., ( Eds). ( 2009).

Clinical Handbook of Mindfulness

Springer-Verlag New York.

[本文引用: 1]

Ditto B., Eclache M., & Goldman N . ( 2006).

Short-term autonomic and cardiovascular effects of mindfulness body scan meditation

Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 32(3), 227-234.

[本文引用: 1]

Eberth J. &Sedlmeier P. ,( 2012).

The effects of mindfulness meditation: a meta-analysis

Mindfulness, 3( 3), 174-189.

DOI:10.1007/s12671-012-0101-x      URL     Magsci     [本文引用: 1]

Abstract$ \overline r = 0.27 $ averaged across all studies and dependent variables. The effects differed widely across dependent variables. Moreover, we found large differences between the effect sizes reported for complete Mindfulness-based Stress Reduction (MBSR) programs vs. “pure” meditation. MBSR seems to have its most powerful effect on attaining higher psychological well-being, whereas pure mindfulness meditation studies reported the largest effects on variables associated with the concept of mindfulness. This raises the question if some effect sizes found for MBSR might be partly inflated by effects that are not attributable to its mindfulness meditation component. Future theorizing should address meditation-specific concepts more extensively to account for the changes in healthy practitioners.

Evans D. R., Eisenlohr-Moul T. A., Button D. F., Baer R. A., & Segerstrom S. C . ( 2014).

Self-regulatory deficits associated with unpracticed mindfulness strategies for coping with acute pain

Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 44( 1), 23-30.

DOI:10.1111/jasp.12196      URL     PMID:4383260      Magsci    

Training in mindfulness is a well-supported therapeutic strategy for pain conditions, though short-term mindfulness training for acute pain is not always effective. To explore the possibility that initial attempts at mindfulness in people without previous training may drain self-regulatory resources, the current study used a student sample (N65=6563) to test the hypothesis that brief instruction in mindfulness would lead to reduced pain tolerance on a cold pressor task, compared to more familiar strategies for coping with acute pain. We also investigated whether high heart rate variability (HRV), a physiological indicator of self-regulatory capacity, would predict pain tolerance. Higher HRV predicted greater pain tolerance only in the control group, suggesting that applying unfamiliar mindfulness strategies while attempting to tolerate pain more rapidly sapped self-regulatory strength.

Fletcher L. B., Schoendorff B., & Hayes S. C . ( 2010).

Searching for mindfulness in the brain: A process-oriented approach to examining the neural correlates of mindfulness

Mindfulness, 1( 1), 41-63.

DOI:10.1007/s12671-010-0006-5      URL     Magsci     [本文引用: 1]

There has been great interest of late in trying to capture the benefits of meditation by scanning meditators brains. In this paper, we argue that a successful neuroscience of mindfulness needs to be based on an adequate psychological analysis. We present a definition of mindfulness based on four psychological processes that are relatively well understood, and we show how this model may help organize neuroimaging research and create a bridge to clinical applications. This framework provides an approach to neuroscience research grounded in psychological principles and theory. We propose that this is critical for advancing scientific endeavors such that the knowledge gained helps improve the human condition.

Fogarty F. A., Lu L. M., Sollers J. J., Krivoschekov S. G., Booth R. J., & Consedine N. S . ( 2015).

Why it pays to be mindful: trait mindfulness predicts physiological recovery from emotional stress and greater differentiation among negative emotions

Mindfulness, 6( 2), 175-185.

DOI:10.1007/s12671-013-0242-6      URL    

Although mindfulness has been generally linked to superior emotional functioning, several areas remain unclear. In extending prior work, the current report evaluated the link between trait mindfulness and physiological patterns of recovery from negative emotion and investigated possible associations between trait mindfulness and emotion differentiation. After completing a trait mindfulness measure, 80 healthy volunteers were block-randomized (matched on gender and relatively high versus relatively low trait mindfulness) to complete either emotional (EN) or neutral (NE) writing tasks first. In the EN order, participants wrote about an upsetting experience and, in the NE order, about the events of an average day. In partial support of expectation, relatively more mindful men showed greater physiological reactivity to an emotional task followed by superior recovery, but only in the EN order; supplementary analyses suggest that greater non-reactivity scores among males may be involved in the physiological regulation of emotional stress. As expected, relatively more versus relatively less mindful participants also differentiated more among discrete negative emotions but, again, only in the EN order. Taken together, findings offer preliminary evidence that the more differentiated emotional responding associated with aspects of trait mindfulness may facilitate more adaptive responding under stress and contribute to superior mental and physical health.

Force T. ( 1996).

Guidelines: heart rate variability

European Heart Journal, 17, 354-381.

[本文引用: 1]

Fries M ( 2007).

Mindfulness based stress reduction for the changing work environment

Journal of Academic and Business Ethics, 55( 1), 1-10.

[本文引用: 1]

Galatzer-Levy R.M . ( 1995).

Psychoanalysis and dynamical systems theory: prediction and self-similarity.

[J] ournal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 43( 4), 1085-1113.

DOI:10.1177/000306519504300407      URL     PMID:8926326      [本文引用: 1]

Abstract The theory of dynamical systems (sometimes called chaos theory) has emerged in the past two decades as a powerful tool for understanding the evolution of complex systems. Attempts to develop psychoanalysis along the lines of nineteenth century physical science have proven unsatisfactory. The theory of dynamical systems provides another route for development. It suggests that prediction should aim at describing the overall evolution of systems and that the possibilities for such evolution are broader than classical theory suggested. It also shows that complex systems often involve structures that repeat basic features on several different levels of observation. This suggests a method for systematically exploring the overly rich data of psychoanalysis.

Garland E.L . ( 2011).

Trait mindfulness predicts attentional and autonomic regulation of alcohol cue-reactivity

Journal of Psychophysiology, 25( 4), 180-189.

DOI:10.1027/0269-8803/a000060      URL     PMID:3748643     

BACKGROUND: The trait of mindfulness varies among meditation-na茂ve individuals and is associated with attentional and autonomic regulation, two neurocognitive functions that become impaired in addiction. It was hypothesized that alcohol dependent inpatients with comparatively high levels of trait mindfulness would exhibit significant autonomic recovery from stress-primed alcohol cues mediated by greater attentional disengagement from such cues. METHODS: 58 alcohol dependent inpatients participated in affect-modulated psychophysiological cue-reactivity protocol and a spatial cueing task designed to assess alcohol attentional bias (AB). Associations between trait mindfulness, alcohol AB, and an index of autonomic activity, high-frequency heart rate variability (HFHRV), were examined via multivariate path analysis. RESULTS: Higher trait mindfulness was significantly associated with less difficulty resisting the urge to drink and greater HFHRV recovery from stress-primed alcohol cues. After statistically controlling for the correlation of mindfulness and perceived difficulty resisting drinking urges, the association between mindfulness and HFHRV recovery was partially mediated by attentional disengagement from alcohol cues (model R(2) = .30). DISCUSSION: Alcohol dependent inpatients higher in mindfulness are better able to disengage attention from alcohol cues, which in turn predicts the degree of HFHRV recovery from such cues. Trait mindfulness may index cognitive control over appetitive responses reflected in superior attentional and autonomic regulation of stress-primed alcohol cue-reactivity.

Garland E., Gaylord S., & Park J . ( 2009).

The role of mindfulness in positive reappraisal

EXPLORE: The Journal of Science and Healing, 5( 1), 37-44.

DOI:10.1016/j.explore.2008.10.001      URL     PMID:19114262      [本文引用: 1]

Mindfulness meditation is increasingly well known for therapeutic efficacy in a variety of illnesses and conditions, but its mechanism of action is still under debate in scientific circles. In this paper, we propose a hypothetical causal model that argues for the role of mindfulness in positive reappraisal coping. Positive reappraisal is a critical component of meaning-based coping that enables individuals to adapt successfully to stressful life events. Mindfulness, as a metacognitive form of awareness, involves the process of decentering, a shifting of cognitive sets that enables alternate appraisals of life events. We review the concept of positive reappraisal in transactional stress and coping theory, then describe research and traditional literature related to mindfulness and positive reappraisal, and detail the central role of mindfulness in the reappraisal process. With this understanding, we present a causal model explicating the proposed mechanism. The discussion has implications for clinical practice, suggesting how mindfulness-based integrative medicine interventions can be designed to support adaptive coping processes.

Godfrin K.A., & van Heeringen C. , ( 2010).

The effects of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy on recurrence of depressive episodes, mental health and quality of life: A randomized controlled study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(8), 738-746

[本文引用: 1]

Goldin P.R., &Gross , J. J . ( 2010).

Effects of mindfulness- based stress reduction (MBSR) on emotion regulation in social anxiety disorder

Emotion, 10( 1), 83-91.

DOI:10.1037/a0018441      URL     PMID:20141305      [本文引用: 1]

Mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) is an established program shown to reduce symptoms of stress, anxiety, and depression. MBSR is believed to alter emotional responding by modifying cognitive-affective processes. Given that social anxiety disorder (SAD) is characterized by emotional and attentional biases as well as distorted negative self-beliefs, we examined MBSR-related changes in the brain-behavior indices of emotional reactivity and regulation of negative self-beliefs in patients with SAD. Sixteen patients underwent functional MRI while reacting to negative self-beliefs and while regulating negative emotions using 2 types of attention deployment emotion regulation-breath-focused attention and distraction-focused attention. Post-MBSR, 14 patients completed neuroimaging assessments. Compared with baseline, MBSR completers showed improvement in anxiety and depression symptoms and self-esteem. During the breath-focused attention task (but not the distraction-focused attention task), they also showed (a) decreased negative emotion experience, (b) reduced amygdala activity, and (c) increased activity in brain regions implicated in attentional deployment. MBSR training in patients with SAD may reduce emotional reactivity while enhancing emotion regulation. These changes might facilitate reduction in SAD-related avoidance behaviors, clinical symptoms, and automatic emotional reactivity to negative self-beliefs in adults with SAD.

Greenberg J.., &Meiran N. , ( 2014).

Is mindfulness meditation associated with “feeling less?”

Mindfulness, 5( 5), 471-476.

DOI:10.1007/s12671-013-0201-2      Magsci     [本文引用: 3]

Following previous research which has suggested that mindfulness meditators are less affected by emotional stimuli, the current study examined the hypothesis that mindfulness meditation is associated with decreased emotional engagement, by inducing moods and asking participants to generate as many autobiographical memories opposite in valence as possible. Experienced mindfulness meditators took twice as long as non-meditators to generate the first opposite mood memory yet generated the same total number of memories as non-meditators. Contrary to the initial hypothesis, results indicate that mindfulness may be associated with increased emotional engagement, increased contact with emotions, and rapid recovery from the emotional experience. The effect of mindfulness on implicit and explicit aspects of emotion is discussed, as well as potential implications for treatment of related disorders.

Grossman P., Niemann L., Schmidt S., & Walach H . ( 2004).

Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health benefits: A meta-analysis

Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 57( 1), 35-43.

DOI:10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00573-7      Magsci    

<h2 class="secHeading" id="section_abstract">Abstract</h2><h4 id="absSec_N2420f370N2a9dc738">Objective</h4><p id="">Mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) is a structured group program that employs mindfulness meditation to alleviate suffering associated with physical, psychosomatic and psychiatric disorders. The program, nonreligious and nonesoteric, is based upon a systematic procedure to develop enhanced awareness of moment-to-moment experience of perceptible mental processes. The approach assumes that greater awareness will provide more veridical perception, reduce negative affect and improve vitality and coping. In the last two decades, a number of research reports appeared that seem to support many of these claims. We performed a comprehensive review and meta-analysis of published and unpublished studies of health-related studies related to MBSR.</p><h4 id="absSec_N2420f370N2a9dc798">Methods</h4><p id="">Sixty-four empirical studies were found, but only 20 reports met criteria of acceptable quality or relevance to be included in the meta-analysis. Reports were excluded due to (1) insufficient information about interventions, (2) poor quantitative health evaluation, (3) inadequate statistical analysis, (4) mindfulness not being the central component of intervention, or (5) the setting of intervention or sample composition deviating too widely from the health-related MBSR program. Acceptable studies covered a wide spectrum of clinical populations (e.g., pain, cancer, heart disease, depression, and anxiety), as well as stressed nonclinical groups. Both controlled and observational investigations were included. Standardized measures of physical and mental well-being constituted the dependent variables of the analysis.</p><h4 id="absSec_N2420f370N2a9dc7f8">Results</h4><p id="">Overall, both controlled and uncontrolled studies showed similar effect sizes of approximately 0.5 (<em>P</em>&lt;.0001) with homogeneity of distribution.</p><h4 id="absSec_N2420f370N2a9dc888">Conclusion</h4><p id="">Although derived from a relatively small number of studies, these results suggest that MBSR may help a broad range of individuals to cope with their clinical and nonclinical problems.</p>

Grossman P., Taylor E.W . ( 2007).

Toward understanding respiratory sinus arrhythmia: Relations to cardiac vagal tone, evolution and biobehavioral functions

Biological Psychology, 74( 2), 263-285.

DOI:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.11.014      URL     PMID:17081672      [本文引用: 1]

Respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA, or high-frequency heart-rate variability) is frequently employed as an index of cardiac vagal tone or even believed to be a direct measure of vagal tone. However, there are many significant caveats regarding vagal tone interpretation: 1. Respiratory parameters can confound relations between RSA and cardiac vagal tone. 2. Although intraindividual relations between RSA and cardiac vagal control are often strong, interindividual associations may be modest. 3. RSA measurement is profoundly influenced by concurrent levels of momentary physical activity, which can bias estimation of individual differences in vagal tone. 4. RSA magnitude is affected by beta-adrenergic tone. 5. RSA and cardiac vagal tone can dissociate under certain circumstances. 6. The polyvagal theory contains evolution-based speculations that relate RSA, vagal tone and behavioral phenomena. We present evidence that the polyvagal theory does not accurately depict evolution of vagal control of heart-rate variability, and that it ignores the phenomenon of cardiac aliasing and disregards the evolution of a functional role for vagal control of the heart, from cardiorespiratory synchrony in fish to RSA in mammals. Unawareness of these issues can lead to misinterpretation of cardiovascular autonomic mechanisms. On the other hand, RSA has been shown to often provide a reasonable reflection of cardiac vagal tone when the above-mentioned complexities are considered. Finally, a recent hypothesis is expanded upon, in which RSA plays a primary role in regulation of energy exchange by means of synchronizing respiratory and cardiovascular processes during metabolic and behavioral change.

Hartmann M., Kopf S., Kircher C., Faude-Lang V., Djuric Z., Augstein F., … Nawroth P. P . ( 2012).

Sustained effects of a mindfulness-based stress-reduction intervention in type 2 diabetic patients: Design and first results of a randomized controlled trial (the Heidelberger diabetes and stress-study)

Diabetes Care, 35( 5), 945-947.

DOI:10.2337/dc11-1343      Magsci     [本文引用: 1]

OBJECTIVE-To determine whether a mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) intervention is effective for reducing psychosocial distress (i.e., depression, psychosocial stress) and the progression of nephropathy (i.e., albuminuria) and for improving the subjective health status of patients with type 2 diabetes.<br/>RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODS-Patients with type 2 diabetes and microalbuminuria were randomized to a mindfulness-based intervention (n = 53) or a treatment-as-usual control (n = 57) group. The study is designed to investigate long-term outcomes over a period of 5 years. We present data up to the first year of follow-up (FU).<br/>RESULTS-At FU, the MBSR group showed lower levels of depression (d = 0.71) and improved health status (d = 0.54) compared with the control group. No significant differences in albuminuria were found. Per-protocol analysis also showed higher stress reduction in the intervention group (d = 0.64).<br/>CONCLUSIONS-MBSR intervention achieved a prolonged reduction in psychosocial distress. The effects on albuminuria will be followed up further.

Henderson V. P., Clemow L., Massion A. O., Hurley T. G., Druker S., & Hebert J. R . ( 2012).

The effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on psychosocial outcomes and quality of life in early-stage breast cancer patients: a randomized trial

Breast Cancer Research and Treatment, 131( 1), 99-109.

DOI:10.1007/s10549-011-1738-1      URL     PMID:21901389      Magsci     [本文引用: 1]

AbstractThe aim of this study was determine the effectiveness of a mindfulness-based stress-reduction (MBSR) program on quality of life (QOL) and psychosocial outcomes in women with early-stage breast cancer, using a three-arm randomized controlled clinical trial (RCT). This RCT consisting of 172 women, aged 20–65 with stage I or II breast cancer consisted of the 8-week MBSR, which was compared to a nutrition education program (NEP) and usual supportive care (UC). Follow-up was performed at three post-intervention points: 402months, 1, and 202years. Standardized, validated self-administered questionnaires were adopted to assess psychosocial variables. Statistical analysis included descriptive and regression analyses incorporating both intention-to-treat and post hoc multivariable approaches of the 163 women with complete data at baseline, those who were randomized to MBSR experienced a significant improvement in the primary measures of QOL and coping outcomes compared to the NEP, UC, or both, including the spirituality subscale of the FACT-B as well as dealing with illness scale increases in active behavioral coping and active cognitive coping. Secondary outcome improvements resulting in significant between-group contrasts favoring the MBSR group at 402months included meaningfulness, depression, paranoid ideation, hostility, anxiety, unhappiness, and emotional control. Results tended to decline at 1202months and even more at 2402months, though at all times, they were as robust in women with lower expectation of effect as in those with higher expectation. The MBSR intervention appears to benefit psychosocial adjustment in cancer patients, over and above the effects of usual care or a credible control condition. The universality of effects across levels of expectation indicates a potential to utilize this stress reduction approach as complementary therapy in oncologic practice.

Hosemans D., ( 2015).

Meditation: a process of cultivating enhanced well-being

Mindfulness, 6( 2), 338-347.

DOI:10.1007/s12671-013-0266-y      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Meditation is the practise of training attention and has increasingly become implemented in mainstream healthcare. The current study compared non-meditators with two general approaches to meditation as follows: concentrative and insight-oriented. The latter entails focusing attention on a single object, whereas the former opens attention, in an accepting and non-judgmental way, to whatever arises within the mind. Compared to non-meditators, both meditation approaches demonstrated significantly enhanced mindfulness and also indicated lower perceived stress. However, when compared to non-meditators, only insight-oriented meditators reported significantly greater subjective well-being (SWB), which is thought to arise from the expansion of attention to unwholesome thoughts, cultivating acceptance of such thoughts, and therefore decreasing the surrounding emotional context. Nonetheless, no significant difference was noted on SWB between concentrative and insight-oriented meditators.

Jäger M.J . ( 2016).

An investigation of the relationship between heart rate variability, mindfulness and resilience in the context of emotion regulation (Bachelor's thesis,). University of Twente

[本文引用: 2]

Kabat-Zinn J., ( 1982).

An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: Theoretical considerations and preliminary results

General Hospital Psychiatry, 4( 1), 33-47.

DOI:10.1016/0163-8343(82)90026-3      URL     PMID:7042457     

The practice of mindfulness meditation was used in a 10-week Stress Reduction and Relaxation Program to train chronic pain patients in self-regulation. The meditation facilitates an attentional stance towards proprioception known as detached observation. This appears to cause an “uncoupling” of the sensory dimension of the pain experience from the affectivel evaluative alarm reaction and reduce the experience of suffering via cognitive reappraisal. Data are presented on 51 chronic pain patients who had not improved with traditional medical care. The dominant pain categories were low back, neck and shoulder, and headache. Facial pain, angina pectoris, noncoronary chest pain, and GI pain were also represented. At 10 weeks, 65% of the patients showed a reduction of ≥33% in the mean total Pain Rating Index (Melzack) and 50% showed a reduction of ≥50%. Similar decreases were recorded on other pain indices and in the number of medical symptoms reported. Large and significant reductions in mood disturbance and psychiatric symptomatology accompanied these changes and were relatively stable on follow-up. These improvements were independent of the pain category. We conclude that this form of meditation can be used as the basis for an effective behavioral program in self-regulation for chronic pain patients. Key features of the program structure, and the limitations of the present uncontrolled study are discussed.

Kabat-Zinn J., ( 1994).

Wherever you go, there you are: Mindfulness meditation in everyday life. New York: Hyperion

[本文引用: 1]

Keng S. L., Smoski M. J., & Robins C. J . ( 2011).

Effects of mindfulness on psychological health: A review of empirical studies

Clinical Psychology Review, 31( 6), 1041-1056.

DOI:10.1016/j.cpr.2011.04.006      URL     PMID:21802619      Magsci     [本文引用: 1]

Within the past few decades, there has been a surge of interest in the investigation of mindfulness as a psychological construct and as a form of clinical intervention. This article reviews the empirical literature on the effects of mindfulness on psychological health. We begin with a discussion of the construct of mindfulness, differences between Buddhist and Western psychological conceptualizations of mindfulness, and how mindfulness has been integrated into Western medicine and psychology, before reviewing three areas of empirical research: cross-sectional, correlational research on the associations between mindfulness and various indicators of psychological health; intervention research on the effects of mindfulness-oriented interventions on psychological health; and laboratory-based, experimental research on the immediate effects of mindfulness inductions on emotional and behavioral functioning. We conclude that mindfulness brings about various positive psychological effects, including increased subjective well-being, reduced psychological symptoms and emotional reactivity, and improved behavioral regulation. The review ends with a discussion on mechanisms of change of mindfulness interventions and suggested directions for future research.Research highlights? Mindfulness refers to attending to experience on purpose and non-judgmentally. ? Trait mindfulness and meditation practice correlate with psychological well-being. ? Mindfulness intervention programs reduce psychological symptoms and distress. ? Mindfulness instructions reduce emotional reactivity in laboratory studies. ? Mechanisms of mindfulness' effects need to be a focus of future studies.

Kleiger R. E., Stein P. K., & Bigger J. T . ( 2005).

Heart rate variability: measurement and clinical utility

Annals of Noninvasive Electrocardiology, 10( 1), 88-101.

DOI:10.1111/j.1542-474X.2005.10101.x      URL     PMID:15649244      [本文引用: 1]

Electrocardiographic RR intervals fluctuate cyclically, modulated by ventilation, baroreflexes, and other genetic and environmental factors that are mediated through the autonomic nervous system. Short term electrocardiographic recordings (5 to 15 minutes), made under controlled conditions, e.g., lying supine or standing or tilted upright can elucidate physiologic, pharmacologic, or pathologic changes in autonomic nervous system function. Long-term, usually 24-hour recordings, can be used to assess autonomic nervous responses during normal daily activities in health, disease, and in response to therapeutic interventions, e.g., exercise or drugs. RR interval variability is useful for assessing risk of cardiovascular death or arrhythmic events, especially when combined with other tests, e.g., left ventricular ejection fraction or ventricular arrhythmias.

Krygier J. R., Heathers J. A. J., Shahrestani S., Abbott M., Gross J. J., & Kemp A. H . ( 2013).

Mindfulness meditation, well-being, and heart rate variability: A preliminary investigation into the impact of intensive Vipassana meditation

International Journal of Psychophysiology, 89( 3), 305-313.

DOI:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.06.017      URL     PMID:23797150      Magsci     [本文引用: 1]

Mindfulness meditation has beneficial effects on brain and body, yet the impact of Vipassana, a type of mindfulness meditation, on heart rate variability (HRV) - a psychophysiological marker of mental and physical health is unknown. We hypothesised increases in measures of well-being and HRV, and decreases in ill-being after training in Vipassana compared to before (time effects), during the meditation task compared to resting baseline (task effects), and a time by task interaction with more pronounced differences between tasks after Vipassana training. HRV (5-minute resting baseline vs. 5-minute meditation) was collected from 36 participants before and after they completed a 10-day intensive Vipassana retreat. Changes in three frequency-domain measures of HRV were analysed using 2 (Time; pre- vs. post-Vipassana) x 2 (Task; resting baseline vs. meditation) within subjects ANOVA. These measures were: normalised high-frequency power (HF n.u.), a widely used biomarker of parasympathetic activity; log-transformed high frequency power (ln HF), a measure of RSA and required to interpret normalised HF; and Traube-Hering-Mayer waves (THM), a component of the low frequency spectrum linked to baroreflex outflow. As expected, participants showed significantly increased well-being, and decreased ill-being. In HF increased overall during meditation compared to resting baseline, while there was a time * task interaction for THM. Further testing revealed that pre-Vipassana only In HF increased during meditation (vs. resting baseline), consistent with a change in respiration. Post-Vipassana, the meditation task increased HF n.u. and decreased THM compared to resting baseline, suggesting post-Vipassana task-related changes are characterised by a decrease in absolute LF power, not parasympathetic-mediated increases in HF power. Such baroreflex changes are classically associated with attentional load, and our results are interpreted in light of the concept of 'flow'-a state of positive and full immersion in an activity. These results are also consistent with changes in normalised HRV reported in other meditation studies. (C) 2013 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Libby D. J., Worhunsky P. D., Pilver C. E., & Brewer J. A . ( 2012).

Meditation-induced changes in high-frequency heart rate variability predict smoking outcomes

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6( 12), 54.

DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00054      URL     PMID:3307046      [本文引用: 1]

BACKGROUND: High-frequency heart rate variability (HF-HRV) is a measure of parasympathetic nervous system (PNS) output that has been associated with enhanced self-regulation. Low resting levels of HF-HRV are associated with nicotine dependence and blunted stress-related changes in HF-HRV are associated with decreased ability to resist smoking. Meditation has been shown to increase HF-HRV. However, it is unknown whether tonic levels of HF-HRV or acute changes in HF-HRV during meditation predict treatment responses in addictive behaviors such as smoking cessation. PURPOSE: To investigate the relationship between HF-HRV and subsequent smoking outcomes. METHODS: HF-HRV during resting baseline and during mindfulness meditation was measured within two weeks of completing a 4-week smoking cessation intervention in a sample of 31 community participants. Self-report measures of smoking were obtained at a follow up 17-weeks after the initiation of treatment. RESULTS: Regression analyses indicated that individuals exhibiting acute increases in HF-HRV from resting baseline to meditation smoked fewer cigarettes at follow-up than those who exhibited acute decreases in HF-HRV (b = -4.89, p = 0.008). CONCLUSION: Acute changes in HF-HRV in response to meditation may be a useful tool to predict smoking cessation treatment response.

Liu C. Y., Hu S. J., Li X. Y., Wei H. J., Chen T. S., Yang X. B., Li M. Y., .( 2016).

Quantitative difference and its application in sports science

Journal of Physical Education, 23( 1), 11-17.

URL    

Statistical methods can tell qualitative differences between two sets of data, but cannot tell their quantitative differences. The authors measured differences by improving the Fibonacci sequence and its golden section constant τ=(5~(1/2)-1)/2≈0.618. As for x and y(xy), the absolute values of any two numbers, their level l difference is defined as l=|logx(xy)|. As for ages, l0.22, 0.22≤l0.44, 0.44≤l1.00 and l≥1.00 indicate no quantitative difference at all, slight quantitative difference, significant quantitative difference and extremely significant quantitative difference respectively. As for critical unnecessary functions, organs and overall levels, l0.27, 0.27≤l0.47, 0.47≤l0.80 and l≥0.80 and molecular cell levels l0.47, 0.47≤l0.80, 0.80≤l1.27 and l≥1.27 indicate no quantitative difference at all, slight quantitative difference, significant quantitative difference and extremely significant quantitative difference respectively. The authors discussed the initial application of quantitative difference in researches on sports life span, life span and sports effect.

[ 刘承宜, 胡少娟, 李晓云, 魏华江, 陈同生, 杨湘波, ... 李末月, . ( 2016).

定量差异及其在体育科学中的应用

体育学刊, 23( 1), 11-17.]

[本文引用: 2]

Liu C. Y., Zhu L., Li F. H., Liu Y. Y., Duan R., Zhang Q. G., & Long C . ( 2017).

Self-similar constant and quantitative difference as well as their applications in sports science

Journal of Physical Education, 24( 6), 72-78.

[本文引用: 4]

[ 刘承宜, 朱玲, 李方晖, 刘延莹, 段锐, 张全光, 龙程 . ( 2017).

自相似常数和定量差异及其在体育科学中的应用

体育学刊, 24( 6), 72-78.]

URL     [本文引用: 3]

传统的统计方法可以得出两组数据的定性分布差异,而定量差异(QD)可以得出它们的平均值差异。以黄金分割常数为底的对数称为黄金对数,研究发现一个新的自然常数可以表示黄金分割常数,且黄金对数保持不变,称其为自相似常数。自相似常数不但能表示部分基本物理常数,而且能表示部分运动记录和生理极限。用自相似常数表示QD在分子细胞水平、组织器官水平和整体水平功能3个方面与运动成绩的特征参数,并讨论了QD在体育科学中的初步应用。

Liu C. Y., Liu G., Hu S. J., Zhu L., Yang X. B., Zhang Q. G . ( 2017).

Quantitative Biology of Exercise-Induced Signal Transduction Pathways

Advances in Experimental Medicine Biology, 977, 419-424.

Liu X. H., Wang S. S., Chang S. C., Chen W. J., & Si M . ( 2013).

Effect of brief mindfulness intervention on tolerance and distress of pain induced by cold-pressor task

Stress and Health, 29( 3), 199-204.

DOI:10.1002/smi.2446      URL     PMID:22961992      [本文引用: 2]

A number of studies have demonstrated that short-term meditation intervention can lead to greater tolerance and lower pain or distress ratings of experimentally induced pain. However, few attempts have been made to examine the effects of short-term mindfulness-based intervention on the tolerance and distress of pain, when delivered in a therapist-free form. The present research explored the effect of brief mindfulness intervention using pre-recorded instruction on pain experimentally induced by the cold-pressor task. The effects of the mindfulness strategy, the distraction strategy and spontaneous strategy, all through the instructions of pre-recorded voices, were compared. The subjects were drawn from healthy college students and randomly assigned to the aforementioned three groups. Our results showed that compared with using spontaneous strategies, the mindfulness intervention significantly improved the participants' pain tolerance and reduced their immersion distress. The distraction strategy also significantly improved the participants' pain tolerance. However, it did not have a significant effect on the participants' level of distress during the immersion period. Our results suggest that brief mindfulness intervention without a therapist's personal involvement is capable of helping people cope with pain induced by the cold-pressor task. Copyright 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

MacKillop J., & Anderson E.J . ( 2007).

Further psychometric validation of the mindful attention awareness scale (MAAS)

Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 29( 4), 289-293.

DOI:10.1007/s10862-007-9045-1      URL     Magsci     [本文引用: 1]

Mindfulness is an increasingly prominent construct in health research but its study has been impeded by a lack of well-validated measures. The Mindful Attention and Awareness Scale (MAAS; Brown and Ryan, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 84:822–848, 2003) is a promising measure and the goal of the present study was to further validate the MAAS in a large university sample ( n 65=65711). Confirmatory factor analysis supported the unidimensional factor structure of the MAAS in the overall sample. No gender differences in MAAS performance were evident, but the factor structure was not confirmed in the subsample of men, apparently due to power limitations. No categorical differences were evident based on experience with meditation, and MAAS performance was not significantly associated with experience with meditation. These findings are interpreted as broadly supporting the MAAS as a valid measure of mindfulness, but suggesting that novice-level experience with meditation should not be presumed to be associated with greater mindfulness.

Malinowski P., &Hui J.L . ( 2015).

Mindfulness at work: positive affect, hope, and optimism mediate the relationship between dispositional mindfulness, work engagement, and well-being

Mindfulness, 6( 6), 1250-1262.

DOI:10.1007/s12671-015-0388-5      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Mindfulness has been described as a state of awareness characterized by refined attentional skills and a non-evaluative attitude toward internal and external events. Recently, it has been suggested...

Mankus A. M., Aldao A., Kerns C., Mayville E. W., & Mennin D. S . ( 2013).

Mindfulness and heart rate variability in individuals with high and low generalized anxiety symptoms

Behaviour Research and Therapy, 51( 7), 386-391.

DOI:10.1016/j.brat.2013.03.005      URL     PMID:23639305      [本文引用: 1]

61We examined the relationship between mindfulness and heart rate variability (HRV).61The relationship was examined in the context of high and low generalized anxiety symptoms (GA).61In high GA, but not low GA, mindfulness was associated with higher HRV.

Matousek R. H., Dobkin P. L., & Pruessner J . ( 2010).

Cortisol as a marker for improvement in mindfulness-based stress reduction

Complementary Therapies in Clinical Practice, 16( 1), 13-19.

DOI:10.1016/j.ctcp.2009.06.004      URL     PMID:20129404      Magsci     [本文引用: 2]

While much attention has been devoted to examining the beneficial effects of Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction programs on patients' ability to cope with various chronic medical conditions, most studies have relied on self-report measures of improvement. Given that these measures may not accurately reflect physiological conditions, there is a need for an objective marker of improvement in research evaluating the beneficial effects of stress management programs. Cortisol is the major stress hormone in the human organism and as such is a promising candidate measure in the study of the effects of Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction programs. In conjunction with other biological measures, the use of cortisol levels as a physiological marker of stress may be useful to validate self-reported benefits attributed to this program. In the current manuscript, we review the available literature on the role of cortisol as a physiological marker for improvement with regards to mindfulness practice, and make recommendations for future study designs.

, McCraty R., &Childre D., , ( 2010).

Coherence: Bridging personal, social, and global health

Alternative Therapies in Health and Medicine, 16( 4), 10-24.

DOI:10.1016/j.jep.2010.04.035      URL     PMID:20653292      [本文引用: 1]

Institute of HeartMath, Boulder Creek, California, USA. rollin@heartmath.org

Moses Z. B., Luecken L. J., & Eason J. C . ( 2007).

Measuring task-related changes in heart rate variability. Poster session presented at International conference of the IEEE engineering in medicine and biology society, 644-647

Muzzio F. J., Swanson P. D., & Ottino J. M . ( 1992).

Mixing distributions produced by multiplicative stretching in chaotic flows

International Journal of Bifurcation and Chaos, 2( 1), 37-50.

DOI:10.1142/S0218127492000057      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Chaotically advected fluids are both a visual demonstration of stretching and folding leading to chaos and a prototypical example of a multiplicative process with weakly correlated steps. Complementary aspects of the process are studied by means of stretching calculations for different flows under both globally and partially chaotic conditions. Stretching is examined in two different ways: (i) as stretching plots, focusing primarily on stretching at small scales and on the comparison of the spatial distribution of stretching with dye structures and with unstable manifolds, and (ii) as time evolving probability density functions, analyzed using scaling techniques that renormalize the distributions by means of their moments. The first approach leads to the conclusion that the manifold structure generates the striation pattern observed in dye deformation experiments, and that both manifolds and dye patterns agree with stretching plots even at small scales. The second approach demonstrates that the multiplicative nature of stretching generates universal statistics which are reflected in self-similar scaling distributions.

Novaco, R. W . ( 2007).

Anger dysregulation. In T. A. Cavell, K.T. Malcolm, T. A. Cavell, & K. T. Malcolm. (Eds.), Anger, aggression and interventions for interpersonal violence (pp.3-54). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers

Pagani M., Lombardi F., Guzzetti S., Sandrone G., Rimoldi O., Malfatto G., .. Malliani A . ( 1984).

Power spectral density of heart rate variability as an index of sympatho-vagal interaction in normal and hypertensive subjects

Journal of hypertension, 2( 3), S383-385.

URL     PMID:6599685      [本文引用: 1]

Instantaneous heart rate reflects sympatho-vagal influences on pace-maker activity. Hence computer analysis of heart rate variability might provide a quantitative index of that interaction. The power spectral density (PSD) estimate of heart rate variability was obtained in normal controls and in uncomplicated hypertensives, both at rest and during a non-hypotensive sympathetic stimulus (tilting). In normal controls PSD shows three major peaks of frequencies P1 = 0.07, P2 = 0.12, P3 = 0.25 cycles/beat. P1, which is associated with sympathetic activity, represents only a minor portion of total variability at rest, while becoming predominant with tilting. P2 and P3 are associated with vagal activity, and represent the major part of variability at rest, while they are reduced by tilting. In hypertensive patients PSD is altered, as P1 is already predominant at rest and increases only slightly with tilting. Thus PSD of heart rate variability is capable of detecting an early alteration in sympatho-vagal balance of cardiac control present in uncomplicated hypertension.

Park T., Reillyspong M., & Gross C. R . ( 2013).

Mindfulness: a systematic review of instruments to measure an emergent patient-reported outcome (pro)

Quality of Life Research, 22( 10), 2639-2659.

DOI:10.1007/s11136-013-0395-8      URL     PMID:3745812      Magsci     [本文引用: 1]

Purpose Mindfulness has emerged as an important health concept based on evidence that mindfulness interventions reduce symptoms and improve health-related quality of life. The objectives of this study were to systematically assess and compare the properties of instruments to measure self-reported mindfulness.Methods Ovid Medline (R), CINAHL (R), and PsycINFO (R) were searched through May 2012, and articles were selected if their primary purpose was development or evaluation of the measurement properties (validity, reliability, responsiveness) of a self-report mindfulness scale. Two reviewers independently evaluated the methodological quality of the selected studies using the COnsensus-based Standards for the selection of health status Measurement INstruments checklist. Discrepancies were discussed with a third reviewer and scored by consensus. Finally, a level of evidence approach was used to synthesize the results and study quality.Results Our search strategy identified a total of 2,588 articles. Forty-six articles, reporting 79 unique studies, met inclusion criteria. Ten instruments quantifying mindfulness as a unidimensional scale (n = 5) or as a set of 2-5 subscales (n = 5) were reviewed. The Mindful Attention Awareness Scale was evaluated by the most studies (n = 27) and had positive overall quality ratings for most of the psychometric properties reviewed. The Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire received the highest possible rating ("consistent findings in multiple studies of good methodological quality") for two properties, internal consistency and construct validation by hypothesis testing. However, none of the instruments had sufficient evidence of content validity. Comprehensiveness of construct coverage had not been assessed; qualitative methods to confirm understanding and relevance were absent. In addition, estimates of test-retest reliability, responsiveness, or measurement error to guide users in protocol development or interpretation of scores were lacking.Conclusions Current mindfulness scales have important conceptual differences, and none can be strongly recommended based solely on superior psychometric properties. Important limitations in the field are the absence of qualitative evaluations and accepted external referents to support construct validity. Investigators need to proceed cautiously before optimizing any mindfulness intervention based on the existing scales.

Quaglia J. T., Braun S. E., Freeman S. P., McDaniel M. A., & Brown K. W . ( 2016).

Meta-analytic evidence for effects of mindfulness training on dimensions of self-reported dispositional mindfulness

Psychological Assessment, 28( 10),803-818.

DOI:10.1037/pas0000268      URL     PMID:27078183      [本文引用: 2]

Abstract Improvements in stable, or dispositional, mindfulness are often assumed to accrue from mindfulness training and to account for many of its beneficial effects. However, research examining these assumptions has produced mixed findings, and the relation between dispositional mindfulness and mindfulness training is actively debated. A comprehensive meta-analysis was conducted on randomized controlled trials (RCTs) of mindfulness training published from 2003-2014 to investigate whether (a) different self-reported mindfulness scale dimensions change as a result of mindfulness training, (b) key aspects of study design (e.g., control condition type, population type, and intervention type) moderate training-related changes in dispositional mindfulness scale dimensions, and (c) changes in mindfulness scale dimensions are associated with beneficial changes in mental health outcomes. Scales from widely used dispositional mindfulness measures were combined into 5 categories for analysis: Attention, Description, Nonjudgment, Nonreactivity, and Observation. A total of 88 studies (n = 5,787) were included. Changes in scale dimensions of mindfulness from pre to post mindfulness training produced mean difference effect sizes ranging from small to moderate (g = 0.28-0.49). Consistent with the theorized role of improvements in mindfulness in training outcomes, changes in dispositional mindfulness scale dimensions were moderately correlated with beneficial intervention outcomes (r = .27-0.30), except for the Observation dimension (r = .16). Overall, moderation analyses revealed inconsistent results, and limitations of moderator analyses suggest important directions for future research. We discuss how the findings can inform the next generation of mindfulness assessment. (PsycINFO Database Record

Rosmond R.., ( 2005).

Role of stress in the pathogenesis of the metabolic syndrome

Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30( 1), 1-10.

DOI:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.05.007      URL     PMID:15358437      [本文引用: 1]

Excess body fat, obesity, is one of the most common disorders in clinical practice. In addition, there is a clustering of several risk factors with obesity, including hypertension, glucose intolerance, diabetes mellitus, and hyperlipidemia, which is observed more frequently than by chance alone. This has led to the suggestion that these represent a single syndrome and is referred to as the Metabolic Syndrome. A growing body of evidence suggests that glucocorticoid secretion is associated with this complex phenotype. Continuously changing and sometimes threatening external environment may, when the challenge exceeds a threshold, activate central pathways that stimulate the adrenals to release glucocorticoids. In this review, we will discuss how such processes mediate a pathogenetic role in the Metabolic Syndrome.

Sauer S., Walach H., Schmidt S., Hinterberger T., Lynch S., Büssing A., & Kohls N . ( 2013).

Assessment of mindfulness: Review onstate of the art

Mindfulness, 4( 1), 3-17.

DOI:10.1007/s12671-012-0122-5      Magsci     [本文引用: 1]

Abstract<br/><p class="a-plus-plus">Although alternative methods have been proposed, mindfulness is predominantly measured by means of self-assessment instruments. Until now, several scales have been published and to some degree also psychometrically validated. The number of scales reflects the widespread research interest. While some authors have started to compare the underlying concepts and operationalizations of these scales, up to now no overview has been presented describing, contrasting, and evaluating the different methodological approaches towards measuring mindfulness including questionnaires and alternative approaches. In light of this, the present article summarizes the state of mindfulness measurement. Recommendations on how current measurement practice may be improved are provided, as well as recommendations as to what measurement instruments are deemed to be most appropriate for a particular research context.</p><br/>

Sedlmeier P., Eberth J., Schwarz M., Zimmermann D., Haarig F., Jaeger S., & Kunze S . ( 2012).

The psychological effects of meditation: a meta-analysis

Psychological Bulletin, 138( 6), 1139-1171.

DOI:10.1037/a0028168      URL     PMID:22582738      [本文引用: 1]

In this meta-analysis, we give a comprehensive overview of the effects of meditation on psychological variables that can be extracted from empirical studies, concentrating on the effects of meditation on nonclinical groups of adult meditators. Mostly because of methodological problems, almost of an initially identified 595 studies had to be excluded. Most studies appear to have been conducted without sufficient theoretical background. To put the results into perspective, we briefly summarize the major theoretical approaches from both East and West. The 163 studies that allowed the calculation of effect sizes exhibited medium average effects (r = .28 for all studies and r = .27 for the n = 125 studies from reviewed journals), which cannot be explained by mere relaxation or cognitive restructuring effects. In general, results were strongest (medium to large) for changes in emotionality and relationship issues, less strong (about medium) for measures of attention, and weakest (small to medium) for more cognitive measures. However, specific findings varied across different approaches to meditation (transcendental meditation, mindfulness meditation, and other meditation techniques). Surprisingly, meditation experience only partially covaried with long-term impact on the variables examined. In general, the dependent variables used cover only some of the content areas about which predictions can be made from already existing theories about meditation; still, such predictions lack precision at present. We conclude that to arrive at a comprehensive understanding of why and how meditation works, emphasis should be placed on the development of more precise theories and measurement devices.

Segal Z. V., Teasdale J. D., Williams J. M., & Gemar M. C . ( 2002).

The mindfulness-based cognitive therapy adherence scale: Inter-rater reliability, adherence to protocol and treatment distinctiveness

Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 9( 2), 131-138.

DOI:10.1002/cpp.320      URL    

The development of the Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy Adherence Scale (MBCT-AS) is described. This 17-item scale measures therapist adherence to the treatment protocol for Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy (MBCT), a treatment for the prevention of recurrence in Major Depressive Disorder. The MBCT-AS assesses therapist behaviours specific to (MBCT) as well as therapy practices that MBCT shares with Cognitive Behaviour Therapy (CBT). To determine the utility of this scale, we compared delivery of group MBCT against group CBT, with independent ratings of taped sessions provided to measure adherence to MBCT and CBT for therapists in both groups. The results showed that: (a) raters can reliably use the MBCT-AS; (b) MBCT therapists demonstrated adherence to the treatment protocol, as measured by the MBCT-AS; and (c) MBCT is distinguishable from CBT on both the MBCT-AS and a scale measuring adherence to CBT (CBT-AS). These findings indicate that the MBCT-AS may be a useful tool for ensuring the proper delivery of MBCT in future research, and may be helpful in determining the elements of MBCT that are unique to that treatment. Copyright 2002 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Segal Z. V., Williams J. M. G., & Teasdale J. D . ( 2012).

Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression

Guilford Press

[本文引用: 1]

Shaffer F., McCraty R., & Zerr C. L . ( 2014).

A healthy heart is not a metronome: an integrative review of the heart's anatomy and heart rate variability

Frontiers in psychology, 5, 1040.

DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01040      URL     PMID:4179748      [本文引用: 2]

Abstract Heart rate variability (HRV), the change in the time intervals between adjacent heartbeats, is an emergent property of interdependent regulatory systems that operate on different time scales to adapt to challenges and achieve optimal performance. This article briefly reviews neural regulation of the heart, and its basic anatomy, the cardiac cycle, and the sinoatrial and atrioventricular pacemakers. The cardiovascular regulation center in the medulla integrates sensory information and input from higher brain centers, and afferent cardiovascular system inputs to adjust heart rate and blood pressure via sympathetic and parasympathetic efferent pathways. This article reviews sympathetic and parasympathetic influences on the heart, and examines the interpretation of HRV and the association between reduced HRV, risk of disease and mortality, and the loss of regulatory capacity. This article also discusses the intrinsic cardiac nervous system and the heart-brain connection, through which afferent information can influence activity in the subcortical and frontocortical areas, and motor cortex. It also considers new perspectives on the putative underlying physiological mechanisms and properties of the ultra-low-frequency (ULF), very-low-frequency (VLF), low-frequency (LF), and high-frequency (HF) bands. Additionally, it reviews the most common time and frequency domain measurements as well as standardized data collection protocols. In its final section, this article integrates Porges' polyvagal theory, Thayer and colleagues' neurovisceral integration model, Lehrer et al.'s resonance frequency model, and the Institute of HeartMath's coherence model. The authors conclude that a coherent heart is not a metronome because its rhythms are characterized by both complexity and stability over longer time scales. Future research should expand understanding of how the heart and its intrinsic nervous system influence the brain.

Shapiro S. L., Carlson L. E., Astin J. A., & Freedman B . ( 2006).

Mechanisms of mindfulness. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(3), 373-386

[本文引用: 1]

Siegling A.B., &Petrides K.V, . ( 2014).

Measures of trait mindfulness: Convergent validity, shared dimensionality, and linkages to the five-factor model. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1164

DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01164      URL     PMID:4195279      [本文引用: 1]

This study investigated, and partially aimed to replicate, important construct validity aspects and the homogeneity of trait mindfulness measures. Specifically, the study set out to examine whether a single dimension can explain the shared variance among these measures as well as the extent to which they converge with one another and in terms of their linkages to the five-factor model (FFM). Two samples completed all trait measures of the construct and one of them additionally completed a measure of the Big Five personality traits. Results showed that a single dimension explains the shared variance among measures based on the original, Eastern conceptualization of mindfulness, although not all of them seem to represent this construct comprehensively. Intercorrelations, dimensionality analysis, as well as linkages to the FFM indicated that the Eastern and Western conceptualizations, and their respective measures, reflect distinct constructs. However, the amount of variance overlap with the FFM was similar across the two conceptualizations.

Soer R., de Jong A. B., Hofstra B. L., Schiphorst Preuper H. R., & Reneman M. F . ( 2015).

Does mindfulness improve after heart coherence training in patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain and healthy subjects? A pilot study

Global Advances in Health and Medicine, 4( 4), 50-55.

DOI:10.7453/gahmj.2015.028      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Mindfulness and heart coherence training (HCT) training are applied increasingly in the treatment of patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain (CMP). Questionnaires have been developed to assess changes in mindfulness but no gold standard is available. Explore the relationship between changes in mindfulness scores and changes in heart coherence after 3 sessions of HCT in patients with CMP and in healthy subjects. Ten patients with CMP and 15 healthy subjects were trained in self-regulation with the use of HCT following a standardized stress relief program developed by the HeartMath Institute. A heart coherence-score (HC-score) was constructed with scores ranging from 0-100 with higher scores reflecting more heart rate variability (HRV) coherency. Change scores, Spearman correlation coefficients, and Wilcoxon Signed Rank test were calculated to test relationships and differences between HC-score, the Mindfulness Attention and Awareness Scale (MAAS) and Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ). A new questionnaire was constructed to explore on which mindfulness-related domains patients with CMP report changes after HCT. Increases were present on HC-score in healthy subjects (P<.01) and in patients (P<.01) between baseline and follow-up. Effect sizes on change on the MAAS and FFMQ were low. Weak (r<0.25) and non-significant correlations were observed in change scores between HC-score and MAAS or FFMQ. Patients reported significant favorable differences on 6 mindfulness related domains in the new questionnaire: breathing rhythm, physical awareness, positive or negative emotions, recognition of stressful situations, thoughts, and tendency to actively self-regulate. In this pilot study, mindfulness as assessed by the MAAS and FFMQ does not appear to improve after HCT. HRV coherency, MAAS, and FFMQ measure different constructs and are weakly related. It is of great importance to choose and develop valid measures that reflect patients' states of mindfulness. Content and face validity of measures of mindfulness may be considered in the light of performance-based measures.

Sterling P.., . ( 2012).

Allostasis: a model of predictive regulation

Physiology & Behavior, 106( 1), 5-15.

DOI:10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.06.004      URL     PMID:21684297      [本文引用: 1]

78 homeostasis (feedback error-correction) is inefficient; it cannot be a key regulator. 78 allostasis (predictive error-prevention) is highly efficient; it is the key regulator. 78 allostasis employs a dedicated organ for prediction and control -- the brain. 78 efficient regulation uses a push-pull: non-adapting “stick”+fast-adapting “carrot”. 78 modern life disturbs this mechanism, thereby generating addictive behaviors.

Sterling P. & Eyer J.

(1988). Allostasis: a new paradigm to explain arousal pathology. In: S Fisher, J. Reason. (Eds). Handbook of life stress, cognition and health (pp. 629-649)

. NewYork: John Wiley & Sons.

[本文引用: 1]

Takahashi T., Murata T., Hamada T., Omori M., Kosaka H., Kikuchi M. … Wada Y. J. ., ( 2005).

Changes in EEG and autonomic nervous activity during meditation and their association with personality traits. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 55(2), 199-207

DOI:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2004.07.004      URL     PMID:15649551      [本文引用: 1]

Meditation is the attainment of a restful yet fully alert physical and mental state practiced by many as a self-regulatory approach to emotion management, but the psychophysiological properties and personality traits that characterize this meditative state have not been adequately studied. We quantitatively analyzed changes in psychophysiological parameters during Zen meditation in 20 normal adults, and evaluated the results in association with personality traits assessed by Cloninger's Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI). During meditation, increases were observed in fast theta power and slow alpha power on EEG predominantly in the frontal area, whereas an increase in the normalized unit of high-frequency (nuHF) power (as a parasympathetic index) and decreases in the normalized unit of low-frequency (nuLF) power and LF/HF (as sympathetic indices) were observed through analyses of heart rate variability. We analyzed the possible correlations among these changes in terms of the percent change during meditation using the control condition as the baseline. The percent change in slow alpha EEG power in the frontal area, reflecting enhanced internalized attention, was negatively correlated with that in nuLF as well as in LF/HF and was positively correlated with the novelty seeking score (which has been suggested to be associated with dopaminergic activity). The percent change in fast theta power in the frontal area, reflecting enhanced mindfulness, was positively correlated with that in nuHF and also with the harm avoidance score (which has been suggested to be associated with serotonergic activity). These results suggest that internalized attention and mindfulness as two major core factors of behaviors of mind during meditation are characterized by different combinations of psychophysiological properties and personality traits.

Tang Y. Y., Ma Y. H., Fan Y. X., Feng H. B., Wang J. H., Feng S G., … Fan M . ( 2009).

Central and autonomic nervous system interaction is altered by short-term meditation. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(22), 8865-8870

[本文引用: 1]

Thayer J. F., Åhs F., Fredrikson M., Sollers J. J., & Wager T. D . ( 2012).

A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: implications for heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36( 2), 747-756.

DOI:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.009      URL     PMID:22178086      Magsci     [本文引用: 3]

The intimate connection between the brain and the heart was enunciated by Claude Bernard over 150 years ago. In our neurovisceral integration model we have tried to build on this pioneering work. In the present paper we further elaborate our model and update it with recent results. Specifically, we performed a meta-analysis of recent neuroimaging studies on the relationship between heart rate variability and regional cerebral blood flow. We identified a number of regions, including the amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex, in which significant associations across studies were found. We further propose that the default response to uncertainty is the threat response and may be related to the well known negativity bias. Heart rate variability may provide an index of how strongly ‘top–down’ appraisals, mediated by cortical-subcortical pathways, shape brainstem activity and autonomic responses in the body. If the default response to uncertainty is the threat response, as we propose here, contextual information represented in ‘appraisal’ systems may be necessary to overcome this bias during daily life. Thus, HRV may serve as a proxy for ‘vertical integration’ of the brain mechanisms that guide flexible control over behavior with peripheral physiology, and as such provides an important window into understanding stress and health.

Thayer, J. F., &Brosschot , J. F . ( 2005).

Psychosomatics and psychopathology: looking up and down from the brain

Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30( 10), 1050-1058.

DOI:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.04.014      URL     PMID:16005156      [本文引用: 2]

The autonomic nervous system (ANS) plays a role in a wide range of somatic and mental diseases. Using a model of neurovisceral integration, this article describes how autonomic imbalance and decreased parasympathetic tone in particular may be the final common pathway linking negative affective states and conditions to ill health. The central nervous system (CNS) network that regulates autonomic balance (central autonomic network, CAN) is closely related and partially overlaps with networks serving executive, social, affective, attentional, and motivated behavior (anterior executive region, AER; and Damasio's [Damasio, A.R., 1998. Emotion in the perspective of an integrated nervous system. Brain Res. Rev. 26, 83–86.] ‘emotion circuit’). A common reciprocal inhibitory cortico-subcortical neural circuit serves to regulate defensive behavior, including autonomic, emotional and cognitive features. This inhibitory cortico-subcortical circuit may structurally, as well as functionally, link psychological processes with health-related physiology. When the prefrontal cortex is taken ‘offline’ for whatever reason, parasympathetic inhibitory action is withdrawn and a relative sympathetic dominance associated with disinhibited defensive circuits is released, which can be pathogenic when sustained for long periods. This state is indicated by low heart rate variability (HRV), which is a marker for low parasympathetic activation and prefrontal hypoactivity. Consistent with this, HRV is associated with a range of psychological and somatic pathological conditions, including immune dysfunction. Finally, we discuss supportive evidence from recent studies of the reflexive startle blink, attention and working memory, which shows that low HRV predicts hypervigilance and inefficient allocation of attentional and cognitive resources.

, Thayer J.F., &Lane R.D, . ( 2000).

A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation

Journal of Affective Disorders, 61( 3), 201-216.

DOI:10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00338-4      URL     PMID:11163422      [本文引用: 5]

In the present paper we present the outlines of a model that integrates autonomic, attentional, and affective systems into a functional and structural network that may help to guide us in our understanding of emotion regulation and dysregulation. We will emphasize the relationship between attentional regulation and affective processes and propose a group of underlying physiological systems that serve to integrate these functions in the service of self-regulation and adaptability of the organism. We will attempt to place this network in the context of dynamical systems models which involve feedback and feedforward circuits with special attention to negative feedback mechanisms, inhibitory processes, and their role in response selection. From a systems perspective, inhibitory processes can be viewed as negative feedback circuits that allow for the interruption of ongoing behavior and the re-deployment of resources to other tasks. When these negative feedback mechanisms are compromised, positive feedback loops may develop as a result (of dis-inhibition). From this perspective, the relative sympathetic activation seen in anxiety disorders may represent dis-inhibition due to faulty inhibitory mechanisms.

, Thayer J.F., &Lane R.D, . ( 2009).

Claude Bernard and the heart-brain connection: Further elaboration of a model of neurovisceral integration

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33(2), 81-88.

DOI:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.004      URL     PMID:18771686      Magsci     [本文引用: 2]

The intimate connection between the brain and the heart was enunciated by Claude Bernard over 150 years ago. In our neurovisceral integration model we have tried to build on this pioneering work. In the present paper we further elaborate our model. Specifically we review recent neuroanatomical studies that implicate inhibitory GABAergic pathways from the prefrontal cortex to the amygdala and additional inhibitory pathways between the amygdala and the sympathetic and parasympathetic medullary output neurons that modulate heart rate and thus heart rate variability. We propose that the default response to uncertainty is the threat response and may be related to the well known negativity bias. We next review the evidence on the role of vagally mediated heart rate variability (HRV) in the regulation of physiological, affective, and cognitive processes. Low HRV is a risk factor for pathophysiology and psychopathology. Finally we review recent work on the genetics of HRV and suggest that low HRV may be an endophenotype for a broad range of dysfunctions.

Tirch D.D . ( 2010).

Mindfulness as a context for the cultivation of compassion

International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 3( 2), 113-123.

Vieten C., Astin J. A., Buscemi R., & Galloway G. P . ( 2010).

Development of an acceptance based coping intervention for alcohol dependence relapse prevention

Substance Abuse, 31( 2), 108-116.

DOI:10.1080/08897071003641594      URL     PMID:20408062      [本文引用: 1]

Both psychological and neurobiological findings lend support to the long-standing clinical observation that negative affect is involved in the development and maintenance of alcohol dependence, and difficulty coping with negative affect is a common precipitant of relapse after treatment. Although many current approaches to relapse prevention emphasize change-based strategies for managing negative cognitions and affect, acceptance-based strategies for preventing relapse to alcohol use are intended to provide methods for coping with distress that are fundamentally different from, though in theory complementary to, approaches that emphasize control and change. This paper describes the development of Acceptance-Based Coping for Relapse Prevention (ABCRP), a new intervention for alcohol-dependent individuals who are within 6 months of having quit drinking. Results of preliminary testing indicate that the intervention is feasible with this population; and a small uncontrolled pilot study (N = 23) showed significant (P < .01) improvements in self-reported negative affect, emotional reactivity, perceived stress, positive affect, psychological well-being, and mindfulness level, as well as a trend (P = .06) toward reduction in craving severity between pre- and postintervention assessments. The authors conclude that this acceptance-based intervention seems feasible and holds promise for improving affect and reducing relapse in alcohol-dependent individuals, warranting further research.

Weinstein N., Brown K. W., & Ryan R. M . ( 2009).

A multi-method examination of the effects of mindfulness on stress attribution, coping, and emotional well-being. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(3), 374-385

DOI:10.1016/j.jrp.2008.12.008      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Mindful individuals orient to ongoing events and experiences in a receptive, attentive manner. This experiential mode of processing suggests implications for the perception of and response to stress situations. Using laboratory-based, longitudinal, and daily diary designs, four studies examined the role of mindfulness on appraisals of and coping with stress experiences in college students, and the consequences of such stress processing for well-being. Across the four studies (’s02=0265026102141), results demonstrated that mindful individuals made more benign stress appraisals, reported less frequent use of avoidant coping strategies, and in two studies, reported higher use of approach coping. In turn, more adaptive stress responses and coping partially or fully mediated the relation between mindfulness and well-being. Implications for the role of mindfulness in stress and well-being are discussed.

West G. ( 2017). Scale: The Universal Laws of Growth, Innovation, Sustainability, and the Pace of Life in Organisms, Cities, Economies, and Companies. New York: Penguin Press.

[本文引用: 2]

Wright S., Day A., & Howells K . ( 2009).

Mindfulness and the treatment of anger problems

Aggression & Violent Behavior, 14( 5), 396-401.

DOI:10.1016/j.avb.2009.06.008      URL     [本文引用: 3]

Clinical interventions based on training in mindfulness skills are an increasingly common part of psychological practice. Mindfulness training can lead to reductions in a variety of problematic conditions including pain, stress, anxiety, depressive relapse, psychosis, and disordered eating but to date there have been few attempts to investigate the effectiveness of this approach with problematic anger. In this paper, the literature in relation to the theory and treatment of problematic anger is reviewed, with the aim of determining whether a rationale exists for the use of mindfulness with angry individuals. It is concluded that anger as an emotion seems particularly appropriate for the application of mindfulness-based interventions, and the potential mechanisms for its proposed effects in alleviating the cognitive, affective and behavioral manifestations of anger are discussed.

Zeltzer L. K., Fanurik D., & Lebaron S . ( 1989).

The cold pressor pain paradigm in children: feasibility of an intervention model (part ii)

Pain, 37( 3), 305-313.

DOI:10.1016/0304-3959(89)90195-4      URL     PMID:2755712      [本文引用: 1]

The purpose of this study was to examine the feasibility of testing a psychological approach (hypnosis) to pain reduction in children using the cold pressor paradigm. Children's pain ratings at 10 sec intervals and duration of arm immersion (40 sec maximum) in 15 °C (n = 37) and 12 °C water (n = 29) were assessed in 6–12-year-old children during 2 baseline trials (alternating arms), followed by 2 more trials after randomization to a control or hypnosis treatment condition. Hypnosis was found to reduce pain significantly more than the control condition in both 15 °C and 12 °C water. Hypnotic susceptibility was not strongly related to hypnotic pain reduction. However, age was significant, with younger children showing higher pain ratings and early arm withdrawal rates and less response to hypnosis than older children. In 15 °C water, females had higher pain ratings and early withdrawal rates than males, but this sex discrepancy disappeared in 12 °C water. This study demonstrated the feasibility of the cold pressor paradigm for testing intervention strategies and its potential for enhancing our understanding of pain in children.

/


版权所有 © 《心理学报》编辑部
地址:北京市朝阳区林萃路16号院 
邮编:100101 
电话:010-64850861 
E-mail:xuebao@psych.ac.cn
备案编号:京ICP备10049795号-1 京公网安备110402500018号

本系统由北京玛格泰克科技发展有限公司设计开发