Please wait a minute...
心理学报
  论文 本期目录 | 过刊浏览 | 高级检索 |
幼儿在地图任务中自我位置与自我朝向表征能力的发展
卢静;胡清芬
(北京师范大学发展心理研究所, 北京 100875)
The Development of Self-Position Representation and Self-Orientation Representation in Children’s Map Task
LU Jing; HU Qingfen
(Institute of Developmental Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, China)
全文: PDF(483 KB)   评审附件 (1 KB) 
输出: BibTeX | EndNote (RIS)      
摘要 

使用地图定向任务, 探讨了不同任务模式下, 4~6岁幼儿自我位置与自我朝向表征能力的发展特点。结果发现, 位置表征能力的获得先于朝向表征能力, 幼儿从4岁起即开始使用地图表征自我位置, 但到5岁才表现出对自我朝向的表征能力。此外, 对于年龄较小的儿童, 相比于空间–地图的任务模式, 地图–空间的任务模式更有利于完成任务。上述结果第一次从个体发展层面证明了位置与朝向表征的分离现象, 说明位置与朝向的表征能力在发展过程中具有一定的顺序性。

服务
把本文推荐给朋友
加入引用管理器
E-mail Alert
RSS
作者相关文章
卢静
胡清芬
关键词 幼儿自我位置表征自我朝向表征地图    
Abstract

Since the 1970 s, study from neurophysiology cognitive have found that animals’ position representation and direction representation in navigation tasks have different neural mechanisms. For example, place cells in hippocampus fire selectively in specific locations (O’Keefe & Dostrovsky, 1971), and head-direction cells in the postsubiculcum fire when the animal face specific orientations (Ranck, 1984). These studies have demonstrated the functional division of labor in animals’ cells responsive to different types of representations in space. However, no research has yet focused on the contrast between these two types of representations, and further the development trajectory of position and direction representations. In the present study, two map tasks were used to explore the developmental trajectory of position and direction representations in human children aged from 4 to 6. In each task, two map-to-space problems and two space-to-map problems were resolved by each child, which were conducted in a symmetrical-furnished square room. For the map-to-space problems of the location task, children were asked to indicate their own locations in the room by placing a cylinder object in the map, whereas for the space-to-map problems, they were given a map with a cylinder object in a specific position and asked to go to the corresponding location in the room. In the direction task, children were asked to indicate their orientations by placing a doll facing the corresponding direction in the map (space-to-map problems), or facing the corresponding direction in the room as the doll indicated in the map (map-to-space problems). We found that 4-year-old children began to perform better than chance in the location task. However, children did not show the ability to solve the direction problems until 5 years old. Besides, the 4-year-olds performed better in the pattern of map-to-place in the location task. These results demonstrated that the ability of using map to represent position and that to represent direction were dissociable in the development. Specifically, the ability of representing one’s own location in a map developed earlier than that of representing one’s own direction. Our study provides novel evidence on the dissociation of position representation and direction representation, extending previous neurophysiological studies. Additionally, the benefit of map-to-space pattern suggested that this pattern might provide a better global understanding of spatial relations in map task and favor young children’s location representation when they start to understand map.

Key wordschildren    self-position representation    self-orientation representation    map
收稿日期: 2014-02-11      出版日期: 2015-02-25
基金资助:

教育部人文社会科学研究青年基金资助项目(10YJCXLX017); 教育部人文社会科学重点研究基地自设项目。

通讯作者: 胡清芬, E-mail:qingfenhu@bnu.edu.cn    
引用本文:   
卢静;胡清芬. 幼儿在地图任务中自我位置与自我朝向表征能力的发展[J]. 心理学报, 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2015.00234.
LU Jing; HU Qingfen. The Development of Self-Position Representation and Self-Orientation Representation in Children’s Map Task. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(2): 234-242.
链接本文:  
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2015.00234      或      http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/Y2015/V47/I2/234
[1] 李婷玉;刘黎;朱莉琪. 4~6岁幼儿经济博弈中的信任行为及其影响因素[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(1): 17-27.
[2] 胡清芬;卢静. 幼儿在地图任务中对自我位置与自我朝向的结合[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(9): 1143-1150.
[3] 邢淑芬;梁熙;岳建宏;王争艳. 祖辈共同养育背景下多重依恋关系及对幼儿社会−情绪性发展的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(5): 518-528.
[4] 钱淼;周立霞;鲁甜甜;翁梦星;傅根跃. 幼儿友好型内隐联想测验的建构及有效性[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(7): 903-913.
[5] 王福兴;李文静;颜志强;段朝辉;李卉. 幼儿对威胁性刺激蛇的注意觉察:来自眼动证据[J]. 心理学报, 2015, 47(6): 774-786.
[6] 贺琼;王争艳;王莉;蒋彩虹;上官芳芳. 新入园幼儿的皮质醇变化与上呼吸道感染的关系:气质的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(4): 516-527.
[7] 彭君;莫雷;黄平;周莹;王靖;昂晨. 工作记忆训练提升幼儿流体智力表现[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(10): 1498-1508.
[8] 王乾东;胡超;傅根跃. 幼儿面孔加工异族效应的眼动研究[J]. 心理学报, 2013, 45(2): 169-178.
[9] 陈璟,孙昕怡,李红,李秀丽 . 愿望采择发展水平对幼儿情感决策的影响[J]. , 2009, 41(10): 958-966.
[10] 刘国雄,方富熹. 学前儿童朴素情绪理论的发展[J]. , 2009, 41(10): 939-946.
[11] 何洁,徐琴美. 幼儿生气和伤心情绪情景理解[J]. , 2009, 41(01): 62-68.
[12] 许晓晖,庞丽娟,张华,周新林,陶沙. 母亲评价与幼儿数学认知发展的关系[J]. , 2007, 39(05): 837-844.
[13] 刘国雄,方富熹. 幼儿对基于信念的惊奇情绪的认知发展[J]. , 2007, 39(04): 662-667.
[14] 任萍,徐芬,张瑞平. 拼音学习对幼儿语音意识发展的影响[J]. , 2006, 38(01): 41-46.
[15] 杨丽珠,王江洋,刘文,Monica Cuskelly,Airong Zhang. 3~5岁幼儿自我延迟满足的发展特点及其中澳跨文化比较[J]. , 2005, 37(02): 224-232.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
版权所有 © 《心理学报》编辑部
本系统由北京玛格泰克科技发展有限公司设计开发  技术支持:support@magtech.com.cn