Please wait a minute...
Advances in Psychological Science    2018, Vol. 26 Issue (11) : 1952-1960     DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.01952
Regular Articles |
Face pareidolia and its neural mechanism
Hao WANG,Zhigang YANG()
College of Education, Hebei University, Baoding 071002 China
Download: PDF(330 KB)   HTML
Export: BibTeX | EndNote | Reference Manager | ProCite | RefWorks    
Abstract  

Face pareidolia refers to the compelling illusion of perceiving facial features on inanimate objects, such as an illusory face on the moon surface. Both top-down and bottom-up factors can modulate the occurrence of face pareidolia. In recent years, many studies using behavioral, brain imaging, as well as EEG techniques have been devoted to investigating its influential factors. It was found that the occurrence of face pareidolia depends on whether the stimuli contain face-like structures, whether the internal face template can match the current stimulus, and whether or not there are face related backgrounds. It was also influenced by individual differences and observers’ emotional states. Brain imaging studies suggest that information from the frontal and occipital vision regions can be infused at the fusiform face area (FFA) when experiencing face pareidolia. Future research should focus on exploring the behavioral and neural mechanisms of individual differences in face pareidolia, as well as the interactions and neural mechanisms between different types of top-down modulation.

Keywords face pareidolia      top-down processing      bottom-up processing      face processing     
ZTFLH:  B842  
Corresponding Authors: Zhigang YANG     E-mail: yangzg.psy@gmail.com
Issue Date: 26 September 2018
Service
E-mail this article
E-mail Alert
RSS
Articles by authors
Hao WANG
Zhigang YANG
Cite this article:   
Hao WANG,Zhigang YANG. Face pareidolia and its neural mechanism[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(11): 1952-1960.
URL:  
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.01952     OR     http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/EN/Y2018/V26/I11/1952
[1] Akechi, H., Kikuchi, Y., Tojo, Y., Osanai, H., & Hasegawa, T. ( 2014). Neural and behavioural responses to face-likeness of objects in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Scientific Reports, 4( 4), 3874.
[2] Alaerts, K., Nackaerts, E., Meyns, P., Swinnen, S. P., & Wenderoth, N. ( 2011). Action and emotion recognition from point light displays: an investigation of gender differences. Plos One, 6( 6), e20989.
pmid: 3111458 url: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21695266
[3] Bar, M., Kassam, K. S., Ghuman, A. S., Boshyan, J., Schmid, A. M., Dale, A. M., … Halgren, E. ( 2006). Top-down facilitation of visual recognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103( 2), 449-454.
pmid: 16407167 url: http://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/16407167
[4] Batty, M., & Taylor, M.J. ( 2003). Early processing of the six basic facial emotional expressions. Cognitive Brain Research, 17( 3), 613-620.
[5] Bentin, S., Allison, T., Puce, A., Perez, E., & Mccarthy, G. ( 1996). Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8( 6), 551-565.
pmid: 2927138 url: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/pmc2927138/
[6] Bentin, S., Sagiv, N., Mecklinger, A., Friederici, A., & von Cramon, Y. D. ( 2002). Priming visual face-processing mechanisms: electrophysiological evidence. Psychological Science, 13( 2), 190-193.
pmid: 11934007 url: http://europepmc.org/abstract/med/11934007
[7] Candidi, M., Stienen, B. M. C., Aglioti, S. M., & de Gelder., B. ( 2015). Virtual lesion of right posterior superior temporal sulcus modulates conscious visual perception of fearful expressions in faces and bodies. Cortex, 65, 184-194.
[8] Cataldo, A.M., & Cohen, A.L . ( 2015). The effect of emotional state on visual detection: A signal detection analysis. Emotion, 15( 6), 846-853.
[9] Churches, O., Nicholls, M., Thiessen, M., Kohler, M., & Keage, H. ( 2014). Emoticons in mind: an event-related potential study. Social Neuroscience, 9( 2), 196-202.
[10] Costen, N. P., Parker, D. M., & Craw, I. ( 1996). Effects of high-pass and low-pass spatial filtering on face identification. Percept Psychophys, 58( 4), 602-612.
[11] Cox, D., Meyers, E., & Sinha, P. ( 2004). Contextually evoked object-specific responses in human visual cortex. Science, 304( 5667), 115-117.
[12] Crouzet, S. M., Kirchner, H., & Thorpe, S. J. ( 2010). Fast saccades toward faces: face detection in just 100 ms. Journal of Vision, 10( 4), 1-17.
[13] Epley, N., Akalis, S., Waytz, A., & Cacioppo, J. T. ( 2008). Creating social connection through inferential reproduction loneliness and perceived agency in gadgets, gods, and greyhounds. Psychological Science, 19( 2), 114-120.
[14] Esterman, M., & Yantis, S.( 2010). Perceptual expectation evokes category-selective cortical activity. Cerebral Cortex, 20( 5), 1245-1253.
pmid: 2852509 url: http://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/19759124
[15] Hall, J.A., & Matsumoto, D.( 2004). Gender differences in judgments of multiple emotions from facial expressions. Emotion, 4( 2), 201-206.
pmid: 15222856 url: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15222856
[16] Hansen, B. C., Thompson, B., Hess, R. F., & Ellemberg, D. ( 2010). Extracting the internal representation of faces from human brain activity: an analogue to reverse correlation. Neurolmage, 51( 1), 373-390.
[17] Ichikawa, H., Kanazawa, S., & Yamaguchi, M. K. ( 2011). Finding a face in a face-like object. Perception, 40( 4), 500-502.
[18] Itier, R.J., & Taylor, M.J . ( 2004). N170 or n1? spatiotemporal differences between object and face processing using erps. Cerebral Cortex, 14( 2), 132-142.
pmid: 14704210 url: http://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/14704210
[19] Gajewski, P., D. &, Stoerig, P., ( 2011). N170—an index of categorical face perception? an erp study of human, nonhuman primate, and dog faces. Journal of Psychophysiology, 25( 4), 174-179.
[20] Ge, L., Wang, Z., Mccleery, J. P., & Kang, L. ( 2006). Activation of face expertise and the inversion effect. Psychological Science, 17( 1), 12-16.
pmid: 16371138 url: http://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/16371138
[21] Gosselin, F., & Schyns, P.G. ( 2003). Superstitious perceptions reveal properties of internal representations. Psychological Science, 14( 5), 505-509.
pmid: 12930484 url: http://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/12930484
[22] Jemel, B., Schuller, A. M., Cheref-Khan, Y., Goffaux, V. Crommelinck, M., & Bruyer, R. ( 2003). Stepwise emergence of the face-sensitive n170 event-related potential component. Neuroreport, 14( 16), 2035-2039.
pmid: 14600493 url: http://europepmc.org/abstract/med/14600493
[23] Kanwisher, N., & Yovel, G.( 2006). The fusiform face area: A cortical region specialized for the perception of faces. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 361 ( 1476), 2109-2028.
[24] Kato, M., & Mugitani, R.( 2015). Pareidolia in infants. Plos One, 10( 2), e0118539.
[25] Krombholz, A., Schaefer, F., & Boucsein, W. ( 2007). Modification of n170 by different emotional expression of schematic faces. Biological Psychology, 76( 3), 156-162.
[26] Kuenfner, D., de Heering, A., Jacques, C., Palmero-Soler, E., & Rossion, B. ( 2009). Early visually evoked electrophysiological responses over the human brain (p1, n170) show stable patterns of face-sensitivity from 4 years to adulthood. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 3( 7), 67.
[27] Lahnakoski, J. M., Glerean, E., Salmi, J., J??skel?inen, I. P., Sams, M., & , Hari. ( 2012). Naturalistic fMRI mapping reveals superior temporal sulcus as the hub for the distributed brain network for social perception. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6( 1), 233.
[28] Lee, S. A., Kim, C.-Y., Shim, M., & Lee, S.-H. ( 2017). Gender differences in neural responses to perceptually invisible fearful face—an ERP study. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 11,6.
[29] Liu, J., Li, J., Feng, L., Li, L., Tian, J., & Lee, K. ( 2014). Seeing jesus in toast: neural and behavioral correlates of face pareidolia. Cortex, 53, 60-77.
pmid: 24583223 url: http://europepmc.org/abstract/med/24583223
[30] Liu, J., Li, J., Zhang, H., Rieth, C. A., Huber, D. E., Li, W., Lee, K., & Tian, J. ( 2010). Neural correlates of top-down letter processing. Neuropsychologia, 48( 2), 636-641.
[31] Liu, T., Mu, S., He, H., Zhang, L., Fan, C., Ren, J., … Luo, W ( 2016). The n170 component is sensitive to face-like stimuli: a study of chinese peking opera makeup. Cognitive Neurodynamics, 10( 6), 535-541.
pmid: 27891201 url: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27891201
[32] Lu, L., Zhang, C., & Liang, L. ( 2017). Mental imagery of face enhances face-sensitive event-related potentials to ambiguous visual stimuli. Biological Psychology, 129, 16-24.
url: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0301051117301321
[33] Luo, S. W., Luo, W. B., He, W. Q., Chen, X., & Luo, Y. Y. ( 2013). P1 and n170 components distinguish human-like and animal-like makeup stimuli. Neuroreport, 24( 9), 482-486.
[34] Luo, W. B., Feng, W. F., He, W. Q., Wang, N.-Y., & Luo, Y.-J. ( 2010). Three stages of facial expression processing: erp study with rapid serial visual presentation. Neurolmage, 49( 2), 1857-1867.
pmid: 3794431 url: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053811909009975
[35] Mcbain, R., Norton, D., & Chen, Y. ( 2009). Females excel at basic face perception. Acta Psychologica, 130( 2), 168-173.
[36] Mcclure, E.B. ( 2000). A meta-analytic review of sex differences in facial expression processing and their development in infants, children, and adolescents. Psychological Bulletin, 126( 3), 424-453.
[37] Mechelli, A., Price, C. J., Friston, K. J., & Ishai, A. ( 2004). Where bottom-up meets top-down: Neuronal interactions during perception and imagery. Cerebral Cortex, 14( 11), 1256-1265.
pmid: 15192010 url: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/resolve/reference/PMED?id=15192010
[38] Németh, K., Kovács, P., Vakli, P., Kovács, G., & Zimmer, M. ( 2014). Phase noise reveals early category-specific modulation of the event-related potentials. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 367.
pmid: 24795689 url: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4006031/
[39] Nestor, A., Vettel, J. M., & Tarr, M. J. ( 2013). Internal representations for face detection: An application of noise-based image classification to bold responses. Human Brain Mapping, 34( 11), 3101-3115.
[40] O'Craven, K.M., & Kanwisher, N.( 2000). Mental imagery of faces and places activates corresponding stimulus- specific brain regions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12( 6), 1013-1023.
url: http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/10.1162/08989290051137549
[41] Paras, C.L., & Webster, M.A . ( 2013). Stimulus requirements for face perception: An analysis based on “totem poles”. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 18.
pmid: 23407599 url: http://pubmedcentralcanada.ca/pmcc/articles/PMC3569666/?report=abstract
[42] Partos, T. R., Cropper, S. J., & Rawlings, D. ( 2016). You don’t see what i see: individual differences in the perception of meaning from visual stimuli. Plos One, 11( 3), e0150615.
[43] Pavlova, M. A., Annika, M., Franziska, H., & Sokolov, A. N. ( 2016). Faces on her and his mind: female and likable. Plos One, 11( 6), e0157636.
[44] Pavlova, M. A., Heiz, J., Sokolov, A. N., & Barisnikov, K. ( 2016). Social cognition in williams syndrome: Face tuning. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1131.
url: http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC4969628
[45] Pavlova, M. A., Guerreschi, M., Tagliavento, L., Gitti, F., Sokolov, A. N., Fallgatter, A. J., & Fazzi, E. ( 2017). Social cognition in autism: face tuning. Scientific Reports, 7( 1), 2734.
pmid: 28578379 url: http://pubmedcentralcanada.ca/pmcc/articles/PMC5457440/
[46] Pavlova, M. A., Scheffler, K., & Sokolov, A. N. ( 2015). Face-n-food: gender differences in tuning to faces. Plos One, 10( 7), e0130363.
url: https://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0130363
[47] Pegors, T. K., Kable, J. W., Chatterjee, A., & Epstein, R. A. ( 2015). Common and unique representations in pfc for face and place attractiveness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27( 5), 959-973.
[48] Pourtois, G., Schwartz, S., Seghier, M. L., Lazeyras, F., & Vuilleumier, P. ( 2005). Portraits or people? distinct representations of face identity in the human visual cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17( 7), 1043-1057.
url: http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/10.1162/0898929054475181
[49] Proverbio, A.M., & Galli, J.( 2016). Women are better at seeing faces where there are none: an erp study of face pareidolia. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11( 9), 1501-1512.
pmid: 27217120 url: http://pubmedcentralcanada.ca/pmcc/articles/PMC5015812/
[50] Proverbio, A. M., Zani, A., & Adorni, R. ( 2008). Neural markers of a greater female responsiveness to social stimuli. BMC Neuroscience, 9( 56), 1-10.
url: http://bmcneurosci.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/1471-2202-9-1
[51] Righart, R., Andersson, F., Schwartz, S., Mayer, E., & Vuilleumier, P. ( 2010). Top-down activation of fusiform cortex without seeing faces in prosopagnosia. Cerebral Cortex, 20( 8), 1878-1890.
url: https://academic.oup.com/cercor/article-lookup/doi/10.1093/cercor/bhp254
[52] R?der, B., Ley, P., Shenoy, B. H., Kekunnaya, R., & Bottari, D. ( 2013). Sensitive periods for the functional specialization of the neural system for human face processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110( 42), 16760-16765.
[53] Rossion, B., Gauthier, I., Tarr, M. J., Despland, P., Bruyer, R., Linotte, S., & Crommelinck, M. ( 2000). The N170 occipito- temporal component is delayed and enhanced to inverted faces but not to inverted objects: An electrophysiological account of face-specific processes in the human brain. Neuroreport, 11( 1), 69-74.
url: https://insights.ovid.com/crossref?an=00001756-200001170-00014
[54] Rossion, B., & Jacques, C.( 2008). Does physical interstimulus variance account for early electrophysiological face sensitive responses in the human brain? ten lessons on the n170. Neurolmage, 39(4), 1959-1979.
[55] Saavedra, C., Olivares, E. I., & Iglesias, J. ( 2012). Cognitive decline effects at an early stage: evidence from n170 and vpp. Neuroscience Letters, 518(2), 149-153.
[56] Schindler, S., Zell, E., Botsch, M., & Kissler, J. ( 2017). Differential effects of face-realism and emotion on event-related brain potentials and their implications for the uncanny valley theory. Scientific Reports,7( 45003).
[57] Smith, A. P. R., Stephan, K. E., Rugg, M. D., & Dolan, R. J. ( 2006). Task and content modulate amygdala-hippocampal connectivity in emotional retrieval. Neuron, 49( 4), 631-638.
[58] Smith, M. L., Gosselin, F., & Schyns, P. ( 2012). Measuring internal representations from behavioral and brain data. Current Biology Cb, 22( 3), 191-196.
[59] Takahashi, K., & Watanabe, K.( 2013). Gaze cueing by pareidolia faces. i-Perception, 4( 8), 490-492.
[60] Takahashi, K., & Watanabe, K.( 2015). Seeing objects as faces enhances object detection. i-Perception, 6( 5), 1-14.
[61] Tanskanen, T., N?s?nen, R., Ojanp??, H., & Hari, R. ( 2007). Face recognition and cortical responses: effect of stimulus duration. Neurolmage, 35( 4), 1636-1644.
[62] Taubert, J., Wardle, S. G., Flessert, M., Leopold, D. A., & Ungerleider, L. G. ( 2017). Face pareidolia in the rhesus monkey. Current Biology, 27( 16), 2505-2509.
[63] Thompson, A.E., & Voyer, D.( 2014). Sex differences in the ability to recognise non-verbal displays of emotion: A meta-analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 28( 7), 1164-1195.
[64] Verpooten, J., & Nelissen, M.( 2010). Sensory exploitation and cultural transmission: the late emergence of iconic representations in human evolution. Theory in Biosciences, 129( 2-3), 211-221.
[65] Whitson, J.A., & Galinsky, A.D . ( 2008). Lacking control increases illusory pattern perception. Science, 322( 5898), 115-117.
[66] Xiu, D., Geiger, M. J., & Klaver, P. ( 2015). Emotional face expression modulates occipital-frontal effective connectivity during memory formation in a bottom-up fashion. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 23( 9), 90.
[67] Zhang, H., Liu, J., Huber, D. E., Rieth, C. A., Tian, J., & Lee, K. ( 2008). Detecting faces in pure noise images: A functional MRI study on top-down perception. Neuroreport, 19( 2), 229-233.
pmid: 18185114 url: https://insights.ovid.com/crossref?an=00001756-200801220-00019
[1] PAN Li, HUANG Xiting.  Influencing factors and thought of temporal order perception[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(4): 614-624.
[2] JING Wei, LIU Ziqin.  The diminished attention to eye in the face processing of individuals with autism spectrum disorder[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(3): 476-487.
[3] Hui Xu; Chaoyi Qin; Yunxian Bai; Zheng Tan; Yali Pan; Yanyan Li; Wenjing Zhou; Tao Zhang; Liang Wang. Lateral prefrontal cortex triggers bistable perception switching using human intracranial EEG[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2016, 24(Suppl.): 24-.
[4] ZHOU Guo-Mei;ZHANG Lu-Ran;ZENG Wei-Xian. Theories on Own-Race Effect in Face Recognition[J]. , 2009, 17(02): 278-283.
[5] Xu Yan,Zhang Yaxu,Zhou Xiaolin. The Cognitive Neuroscience Of Face Processing: A Review[J]. , 2003, 11(01): 35-43.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
Copyright © Advances in Psychological Science
Support by Beijing Magtech