[1] |
Burke D. M., MacKay D. G., Worthley J. S., & Wade E . ( 1991). On the tip of the tongue: What causes word finding failures in young and older adults? Journal of Memory and Language, 30( 5), 542-579.
|
[2] |
Burke, D. M., & Shafto, M. A . ( 2008). Aging and language production. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13( 1), 21-24.
|
[3] |
Castro, N., & James, L. E . ( 2014). Differences between young and older adults’ spoken language production in descriptions of negative versus neutral pictures, Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 21( 2), 222-238.
|
[4] |
Cleary A. M., Konkel K. E., Nomi J. S., & McCabe D. P . ( 2010). Odor recognition without identification. Memory & Cognition, 38( 4), 452-460.
|
[5] |
Cohen, J. ( 1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
|
[6] |
Cutting, J. C., & Ferreira, V. S . ( 1999). Semantic and phonological information flow in the production lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25( 2), 318-344.
|
[7] |
Dell'Acqua R., Sessa P., Peressotti F., Mulatti C., Navarrete E., & Grainger J . ( 2010). ERP evidence for ultra-fast semantic processing in the picture-word interference paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 1( 177).
|
[8] |
Dhooge E., de Baene W., & Hartsuiker R. J . ( 2013). A late locus of the distractor frequency effect in picture-word interference: Evidence from event-related potentials. Brain and Language, 124( 3), 232-237.
|
[9] |
Dhooge, E., & Hartsuiker, R. J . ( 2010). The distractor frequency effect in picture-word interference: Evidence for response exclusion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36( 4), 878-891.
|
[10] |
Duncan, J. ( 2004). Selective attention in distributed brain areas. In M. I. Posner (Ed.), Cognitive neuroscience of attention (pp. 105-113). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
|
[11] |
Fieder N., Nickels L., & Biedermann B . ( 2014). Representation and processing of mass and count nouns: a review. Frontiers in Psychology, 5( 589).
|
[12] |
Finkbeiner, M., & Caramazza , A. ( 2006). Now you see it, now you don't: On turning semantic interference into facilitation in a Stroop-like task. Cortex, 42( 6), 790-796.
|
[13] |
Forstmann B. U., Jahfari S., Scholte H. S., Wolfensteller U., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., & Ridderinkhof K. R . ( 2008). Function and structure of the right inferior frontal cortex predict individual differences in response inhibition: A model-based approach. Journal of Neuroscience, 28( 39), 9790-9796.
|
[14] |
Glaser, W. R., & Düngelhoff, F. J . ( 1984). The time course of picture-word interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10( 5), 640-654.
|
[15] |
He, J. Y., & Zhang, Q. F . ( 2017). The temporal courses of word frequency effect and syllable frequency effect of Chinese handwritten production in the old: An ERP study. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 49( 12), 1483-1493.
|
|
[ 何洁莹, 张清芳 . ( 2017). 老年人书写产生中词汇频率和音节频率效应的时间进程:ERP研究. 心理学报, 49( 12), 1483-1493.]
|
[16] |
Jescheniak, J. D., & Levelt, W. J. M . ( 1994). Word frequency effects in speech production: Retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 20( 4), 824-843.
|
[17] |
Jescheniak J. D., Meyer A. S ., & Levelt, W. J. M. ( 2003). Specific-word frequency is not all that counts in speech production: Comments on Caramazza, Costa, et al. (2001) and new experimental data. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29( 3), 432-438.
|
[18] |
Kandel S., Álvarez C., & Vallée N . ( 2006). Syllables as processing units in handwriting production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32( 1), 18-31.
|
[19] |
Logan G. D. ( 1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: a users’ guide to the stop signal paradigm. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr, Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp.189-239). San Diego, CA, US: Academic Press.
|
[20] |
MacKay, D. G., & Abrams, L. ( 1998). Age-linked declines in retrieving orthographic knowledge: empirical, practical, and theoretical implications. Psychology and Ageing, 13( 4), 647-662.
|
[21] |
MacKay D. G., Abrams L., & Pedroza M. J . ( 1999). Aging on the input versus output side: Theoretical implications of age-linked asymmetries between detecting versus retrieving orthographic information. Psychology and Aging, 14( 1), 3-17.
|
[22] |
Mahon B. Z., Costa A., Peterson R., Vargas K. A., & Caramazza A . ( 2007). Lexical selection is not by competition: A reinterpretation of semantic interference and facilitation effects in the picture-word interference paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33( 3), 503-535.
|
[23] |
Miozzo, M., & Caramazza A. ( 2003). When more is less: a counterintuitive effect of distractor frequency in the picture-word interference paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132( 2), 228-252.
|
[24] |
Mortensen L., Meyer A. S., & Humphreys G. W . ( 2006). Age-related effects on speech production: A review. Language and Cognition. Process. 21( 1-3), 238-290.
|
[25] |
Nigg, J. T . ( 2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, 12 6(2), 220-246.
|
[26] |
Peng, H. M., & Mao, X. F . ( 2018). Will the deficit in inhibition increase the rates of tip-of-the-tongue among the elderly? Acta Psychologica Sinica, 50( 10), 1142-1150.
|
|
[ 彭华茂, 毛晓飞 . ( 2018). 抑制对老年人舌尖现象的影响. 心理学报, 50( 10), 1142-1150.
|
[27] |
Protopapas A., Archonti A., & Skaloumbakas C . ( 2007). Reading ability is negatively related to Stroop interference. Cognitive Psychology, 54, 251-282.
|
[28] |
Qu Q., Zhang Q., & Damian M. F . ( 2016). Tracking the Time Course of Lexical Access in Orthographic Production: An Event-Related Potential Study of Word Frequency Effects in Written Picture Naming, Brain and Language, 159, 118-126.
|
[29] |
Roelofs, A. ( 2005). From Popper to Lakatos: A case for cumulative computational modeling. In A. Cutler (Ed.), Twenty-first century psycholinguistics: Four cornerstones ( pp. 313-330). Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.
|
[30] |
Roelofs A., Piai V., & Schriefers H . ( 2011). Selective attention and distractor frequency in naming performance: Comment on Dhooge and Hartsuiker (2010). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37( 4), 1032-1038.
|
[31] |
Schaie, K. W . ( 2000). The impact of longitudinal studies on understanding development from young adulthood to old age. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24( 3), 257-266.
|
[32] |
Schriefers H., Meyer A. S ., & Levelt, W. J. M. ( 1990). Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production: Picture-word interference studies. Journal of Memory and Language, 29( 1), 86-102.
|
[33] |
Shao Z., Meyer A. S., & Roelofs A . ( 2013). Selective and nonselective inhibition of competitors in picture naming. Memory & cognition, 41( 8), 1200-1211.
|
[34] |
Snyder, P., & Lawson, S. ( 1993). Evaluating results using corrected and uncorrected effect size estimates. Journal of Experimental Education, 61, 334-349.
|
[35] |
Sörös P., Bose A., Sokoloff L. G., Graham S. J., & Stuss D. T . ( 2011). Age-related changes in the functional neuroanatomy of overt speech production. Neurobiology of Aging, 32( 8), 1505-513.
|
[36] |
Spaulding, T. J . ( 2010). Investigating mechanisms of suppression in preschool children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53( 3), 725-738.
|
[37] |
Starreveld P. A., & la Heij, W. ( 1995). Semantic interference, orthographic facilitation, and their interaction in naming tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21( 3), 686-698.
|
[38] |
Starreveld, P. A., & la Heij, W. ( 1996). Time-course analysis of semantic and orthographic context effects in picture naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22( 4), 896-918.
|
[39] |
Stemberger, J. P., & Macwhinney, B. ( 1986) Form-oriented inflectional errors in language processing. Cognitive Psychology, 18( 3), 329-354.
|
[40] |
Sternberg, S. ( 1969). The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276-315.
|
[41] |
Verbruggen F., Logan G. D., & Stevens M. A . ( 2008). STOP-IT: Windows executable software for the stop-signal paradigm. Behavior Research Methods, 40( 2), 479-483.
|
[42] |
Yang, Q., & Zhang, Q. F . ( 2015). Aging of word frequency, syllable frequency and phonological facilitation effects in Chinese speech production. Journal of Psychological Science, 38( 6), 1303-1310.
|
|
[ 杨群, 张清芳 . ( 2015). 口语产生中词频效应, 音节频率效应和语音促进效应的认知年老化. 心理科学, 38( 6), 1303-1310.]
|
[43] |
Zhang, Q. F., & Wang, C. ( 2014). Syllable frequency and word frequency effects in spoken and written word production in a non-alphabetic script. Frontiers in Psychology. 5, 120.
|
[44] |
Zhang, Q. F. & Yang, Y. F . ( 2003). The lexical access theory in speech production. Advances in Psychological Science, 11, 6-11.
|
|
[ 张清芳, 杨玉芳 . ( 2003). 言语产生中的词汇通达理论. 心理科学进展, 11, 6-11.]
|
[45] |
Zhang Q. F., Zhu X. B., & Damian M. F . ( 2018). Phonological activation of category coordinates in spoken word production: Evidence for cascaded processing in English but not in Mandarin. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39( 5), 835-860.
|
[46] |
Zheng H. M., Wen Z. L., & Wu Y . ( 2011). The appropriate effect sizes and their calculations in psychological research. Advances in Psychological Science, 19( 12), 1868-1878.
|
|
[ 郑昊敏, 温忠麟, 吴艳 . ( 2011). 心理学常用效应量的选用与分析. 心理科学进展, 19( 12), 1868-1878.]
|
[47] |
Zhu X. B., Damian M. F., & Zhang Q. F . ( 2015). Seriality of semantic and phonological processes during overt speech in Mandarin as revealed by event-related brain potentials. Brain and language, 144, 16-25.
|
[48] |
Zhu X. B., Zhang Q. F., & Damian M. F . ( 2016). Additivity of semantic and phonological effects: evidence from speech production in mandarin. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69( 11), 2285-2304.
|