心理学报 ›› 2018, Vol. 50 ›› Issue (12): 1323-1335.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.01323
• 研究报告 • 下一篇
收稿日期:
2018-01-05
发布日期:
2018-10-30
出版日期:
2018-11-30
基金资助:
LI Weijun(), LIU Meng, ZHANG Zhenghua, DENG Nali, XING Yushan
Received:
2018-01-05
Online:
2018-10-30
Published:
2018-11-30
摘要:
韵律边界是口语韵律特征的有机组成部分, 在语言理解中发挥着重要作用。口吃作为一种言语节律性障碍, 主要表现为音节经常性的重复、拖长或停顿等。本研究采用ERP, 考察口吃者完成词汇判断和结构判断两种任务时, 加工歧义短语(动宾/偏正歧义结构)内部韵律边界的认知过程。结果发现口吃者和言语流畅者在加工汉语歧义短语过程中, 所诱发的反映韵律切分的脑电成分CPS (closure positive shift)不存在显著差异。在0~300 ms, 不论中线还是两侧, 口吃者和言语流畅者加工两类短语时, 动宾短语韵律边界诱发正效应的头皮分布范围小于偏正短语。在300~600 ms, 中线上, 口吃和言语流畅者在完成两种任务时, 两类短语的韵律边界均稳定诱发了正效应; 在两侧, 结构判断任务中两类短语的韵律边界都诱发了正效应, 但词汇判断任务中只有偏正短语稳定诱发该效应。综上, 口吃者和言语流畅者一样对口语韵律边界敏感, 并且他们加工歧义短语内部韵律边界时, 诱发的脑电效应受到实验任务和短语结构类型的影响。
中图分类号:
李卫君, 刘梦, 张政华, 邓娜丽, 邢钰珊. (2018). 口吃者加工汉语歧义短语的神经过程. 心理学报, 50(12), 1323-1335.
LI Weijun, LIU Meng, ZHANG Zhenghua, DENG Nali, XING Yushan. (2018). Neural processing of ambiguous Chinese phrases of stutters. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 50(12), 1323-1335.
被试 | 口吃被试 | 正常被试 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
年龄(岁) | 性别 | 教育程度 | 严重程度 | 年龄(岁) | 性别 | 教育程度 | |
1 | 24 | 男 | 研究生 | 轻度到中度 | 23 | 男 | 研究生 |
2 | 20 | 女 | 大学 | 中度 | 20 | 女 | 大学 |
3 | 22 | 男 | 大学 | 轻度到中度 | 22 | 男 | 大学 |
4 | 25 | 女 | 研究生 | 中度到严重 | 25 | 女 | 研究生 |
5 | 23 | 男 | 大学 | 中度 | 23 | 男 | 大学 |
6 | 24 | 男 | 大学 | 中度到严重 | 24 | 男 | 大学 |
7 | 22 | 男 | 大学 | 轻度到中度 | 23 | 男 | 大学 |
8 | 23 | 男 | 研究生 | 轻度到中度 | 23 | 男 | 研究生 |
9 | 20 | 男 | 大学 | 中度 | 22 | 男 | 大学 |
10 | 24 | 男 | 大学 | 轻度到中度 | 24 | 男 | 大学 |
11 | 24 | 男 | 大学 | 轻度到中度 | 24 | 男 | 大学 |
12 | 24 | 男 | 大学 | 中度 | 24 | 男 | 大学 |
M | 22.92 | 23.08 | |||||
SD | 1.62 | 1.31 |
表1 被试基本信息
被试 | 口吃被试 | 正常被试 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
年龄(岁) | 性别 | 教育程度 | 严重程度 | 年龄(岁) | 性别 | 教育程度 | |
1 | 24 | 男 | 研究生 | 轻度到中度 | 23 | 男 | 研究生 |
2 | 20 | 女 | 大学 | 中度 | 20 | 女 | 大学 |
3 | 22 | 男 | 大学 | 轻度到中度 | 22 | 男 | 大学 |
4 | 25 | 女 | 研究生 | 中度到严重 | 25 | 女 | 研究生 |
5 | 23 | 男 | 大学 | 中度 | 23 | 男 | 大学 |
6 | 24 | 男 | 大学 | 中度到严重 | 24 | 男 | 大学 |
7 | 22 | 男 | 大学 | 轻度到中度 | 23 | 男 | 大学 |
8 | 23 | 男 | 研究生 | 轻度到中度 | 23 | 男 | 研究生 |
9 | 20 | 男 | 大学 | 中度 | 22 | 男 | 大学 |
10 | 24 | 男 | 大学 | 轻度到中度 | 24 | 男 | 大学 |
11 | 24 | 男 | 大学 | 轻度到中度 | 24 | 男 | 大学 |
12 | 24 | 男 | 大学 | 中度 | 24 | 男 | 大学 |
M | 22.92 | 23.08 | |||||
SD | 1.62 | 1.31 |
图1 偏正结构(灰)和动宾结构(黑)短语口语信号的韵律差异。(a)短语片段和停顿(#)时长以及二者差异:动宾结构表现为动词“理解”的时长延长和之后的无声段插入, 偏正结构则表现为第二个名词“呼声”时间延长以及之前的一个无声段插入。(b)基频:动宾结构的主要重音在动词“理解”上, 而偏正结构的主要重音在名词“呼声”上。
1 | Baddeley, A. ( 1992). Working memory. Science, 255( 5044), 556-559. |
2 |
Bogels S., Schriefers H., Vonk W., Chwilla D. J., & Kerkhofs R . ( 2010). The interplay between prosody and syntax in sentence processing: The case of subject-and object-control verbs. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22( 5), 1036-1053.
doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21269 URL pmid: 19445602 |
3 |
Bosshardt H.-G., Ballmer W., & de Nil L . ( 2002). Effects of category and rhyme decisions on sentence production. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 844-857.
doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2002/068) URL pmid: 12381043 |
4 |
Chang S., Kenney M. K., Loucks T. M. J., & Ludlow C. L . ( 2009). Brain activation abnormalities during speech and non-speech in stuttering speakers. NeuroImage, 46( 1), 201-212.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.066 URL pmid: 2693291 |
5 |
Chang S. E., Horwitz B., Ostuni J., Reynolds R., & Ludlow C. L . ( 2011). Evidence of left inferior frontal-premotor structural and functional connectivity deficits in adults who stutter. Cerebral Cortex, 21( 11), 2507-2518.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr028 URL pmid: 3183422 |
6 |
Christophe A., Peperkamp S., Pallier C., Block E., & Mehler J . ( 2004). Phonological phrase boundaries constrain lexical access. i. Adult data. Journal of Memory and Language, 51( 4), 523-547.
doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.07.001 URL |
7 |
Corbera S., Corral M. J., Escera C., & Idiazábal M. A . ( 2005). Abnormal speech sound representation in persistent developmental stuttering. Neurology, 65( 8), 1246-1252.
doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000180969.03719.81 URL pmid: 16247052 |
8 |
Cuadrado,E. M., & Weber-Fox, C. M . ( 2003). Atypical syntactic processing in individuals who stutter: Evidence from event-related brain potentials and behavioral measures. Journal of Speech Language & Hearing Research, 46, 960-76.
doi: 10.1016/j.ejrad.2008.04.012 URL pmid: 12959473 |
9 |
Cykowski M. D., Fox P. T., Ingham R. J., Ingham J. C., & Robin D. A . ( 2010). A study of the reproducibility and etiology of diffusion anisotropy differences in developmental stuttering: A potential role for impaired myelination. Neuroimage, 52( 4), 1495-1504.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.011 URL pmid: 4135434 |
10 |
Dorman,M. F., & Porter Jr, R. J . ( 1975). Hemispheric lateralization for speech perception in stutterers 1. Cortex, 11( 2), 181-185.
doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(75)80042-6 URL pmid: 1149477 |
11 |
Etchell A. C., Civier O., Ballard K. J., & Sowman P. F . ( 2017). A systematic literature review of neuroimaging research on developmental stuttering between 1995 and 2016. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 55, 6-45.
doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2017.03.007 URL pmid: 28778745 |
12 |
Foundas A. L., Corey D. M., Hurley M. M., & Heilman K. M . ( 2004). Verbal dichotic listening in developmental stuttering: Subgroups with atypical auditory processing. Cognitive & Behavioral Neurology, 17( 4), 224-232.
URL pmid: 15622019 |
13 |
Fox P. T., Ingham R. J., Ingham J. C., Hirsch T. B., Downs J. H., Martin C., Jerabek P., Glass T., & Lancaster J. L . ( 1996). A PET study of the neural systems of stuttering. Nature, 382, 158-162.
doi: 10.1038/382158a0 URL pmid: 8700204 |
14 | Giraud A. L., Neumann K., Bachoud-Levi A. C., von Gudenberg A.W., Euler H.A., Lanfermann H., & Preibisch C . ( 2008). Severity of dysfluency correlates with basal ganglia activity in persistent developmental stuttering. Brain and Language. 104 ( 2), 190-199. |
15 |
Hall, J. W., &Jerger, J. ( 1978). Central auditory function in stutterers. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 21( 2), 324-337.
doi: 10.1016/0378-2166(78)90018-8 URL pmid: 703279 |
16 |
Hannley M, & Dorman, M. F . ( 1982). Some observations on auditory function and stuttering. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 7( 1), 93-108.
doi: 10.1016/S0094-730X(82)80003-X URL |
17 |
Holzgrefe H., Wellmann C., Petrone, C, Truckenbrodt, H, Höhle, B., & Wartenburger I . ( 2013). Brain response to prosodic boundary cues depends on boundary position. Frontiers in Psychology, 4( 14), 1-14.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00421 URL pmid: 3714540 |
18 |
Howell P., Davis S., & Williams S. M . ( 2006). Auditory abilities of speakers who persisted, or recovered, from stuttering. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 31( 4), 257-270.
doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2006.07.001 URL pmid: 16920188 |
19 |
Kerkhofs R., Vonk W., Schriefers H., & Chwilla D. J . ( 2007). Discourse, syntax, and prosody: The brain reveals an immediate interaction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19( 9), 1421-1434.
doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.9.1421 URL pmid: 17714005 |
20 |
Kjelgaard,M. M., & Speer, S. R . ( 1999). Prosodic facilitation and interference in the resolution of temporary syntactic closure ambiguity. Journal of Memory & Language, 40( 2), 153-194.
doi: 10.1006/jmla.1998.2620 URL |
21 |
Knösche T. R., Neuhaus C., Haueisen J., Alter K., Maess B., & Witte O. W., & Friederici A. D . ( 2005). Perception of phrase structure in music. Human Brain Mapping, 24( 4), 259-273.
doi: 10.1002/hbm.20088 URL pmid: 15678484 |
22 |
Li Li & Yang Y . ( 2007). Prosodic resolution of syntactic ambiguity: From the point of view of speaker and listener. Advances in Psychological Science, 15( 2), 282-287.
doi: 10.1016/j.conbuildmat.2005.08.001 URL |
23 |
[ 李卫君, 杨玉芳 . ( 2007). 从讲话者和听话者两个角度看韵律的句法解歧. 心理科学进展, 15( 2), 282-287.]
doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1671-3710.2007.02.015 URL |
24 |
Li W.& Yang , Y. ( 2009). Perception of prosodic hierarchical boundaries in Mandarin Chinese sentences. Neuroscience, 158( 4), 1416-1425.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.10.065 URL pmid: 19111906 |
25 |
Li W& Yang Y . ( 2010). The cognitive processing of prosodic boundary and its related brain effect in quatrain. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 42( 11), 1021-1032.
doi: |
26 |
[ 李卫君, 杨玉芳 . ( 2010). 绝句韵律边界的认知加工及其脑电效应. 心理学报, 42( 11), 1021-1032.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2010.01021 URL |
27 |
, ., Li W& Yang Y . ( 2010). Perception of Chinese poem and its electrophysiological effects. Neuroscience, 168( 3), 757-768.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.03.069 URL pmid: 20382205 |
28 |
Li X. Q., Yang Y. F., & Lu Y . ( 2010). How and when prosodic boundaries influence syntactic parsing under different discourse contexts: An ERP study. Biological Psychology, 83( 3), 250-259.
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.01.009 URL pmid: 20083152 |
29 |
Lincoln M., Packman A., & Onslow M . ( 2006). Altered auditory feedback and the treatment of stuttering: A review. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 31( 2), 71-89.
doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2006.04.001 URL pmid: 16750562 |
30 |
Liu B., Wang Z., & Jin Z . ( 2010). The effects of punctuations in Chinese sentence comprehension: An ERP study. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 23( 1), 66-80.
doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2009.08.004 URL |
31 |
Nan Y., Knosche T. R., & Friederici A. D . ( 2006). The perception of musical phrase structure: A cross-cultural ERP study. Brain Research, 1094( 1), 179-191.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.03.115 URL pmid: 16712816 |
32 |
Pannekamp A., Toepel U., Alter K., Hahne A., & Friederici A. D . ( 2005). Prosody-driven sentence processing: An event-related brain potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17( 3), 407-421.
doi: 10.1162/0898929053279450 URL pmid: 15814001 |
33 |
Peter V., Mcarthur G., & Crain S . ( 2014). Using event-related potentials to measure phrase boundary perception in English. BMC Neuroscience, 15( 1), 1-11.
doi: 10.1186/s12868-014-0129-z URL pmid: 25424987 |
34 |
Ramus F., Hauser M. D., Miller C., Morris D., & Mehler J . ( 2000). Language discrimination by human newborns and by cotton-top tamarin monkeys. Science, 288, 349-351.
doi: 10.1126/science.288.5464.349 URL pmid: 10764650 |
35 |
Salverda A. P., Dahan D., & McQueen J. M . ( 2003). The role of prosodic boundaries in the resolution of lexical embedding in speech comprehension. Cognition, 90( 1), 51-89.
doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00139-2 URL pmid: 14597270 |
36 |
, ., Schepman A0 &Rodway p , ( 2000). Prosody and parsing in coordination structures. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53( 2), 377-396.
doi: 10.1080/713755895 URL pmid: 10881611 |
37 |
Shattuck-Hufnagel S, &Turk A. E . ( 1996). A prosody tutorial for investigators of auditory sentence processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25( 2), 193-246.
doi: 10.1007/BF01708572 URL pmid: 8667297 |
38 |
Smith A, &Weber C . ( 2017). How stuttering develops: The multifactorial dynamic pathways theory. Journal of Speech and Language Hearing Research, 60( 9), 2483-2505.
doi: 10.1044/2017_JSLHR-S-16-0343 URL pmid: 28837728 |
39 |
Steinhauer K., Alter K., & Friederici A. D . ( 1999). Brain potentials indicate immediate use of prosodic cues in natural speech processing. Nature Neuroscience, 2( 2), 191-196.
doi: 10.1038/5757 URL pmid: 10195205 |
40 |
Steinhauer K, &Friederici ,A.D . ( 2001). Prosodic boundaries, comma rules, and brain responses: The closure positive shift in ERPs as a universal marker for prosodic phrasing in listeners and readers. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30( 3), 267-295.
doi: 10.1023/A:1010443001646 URL pmid: 11523275 |
41 |
Weber-Fox C . ( 2001). Neural systems for sentence processing in stuttering. Journal of Speech Language & Hearing Research, 44( 4), 814-825.
doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2001/064) URL pmid: 11521774 |
42 |
Weber-Fox C, &Hampton A . ( 2008). Stuttering and natural speech processing of semantic and syntactic constraints on verbs. Journal of Speech Language & Hearing Research, 51( 5), 1058-1071.
doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2008/07-0164) URL pmid: 2638126 |
43 |
Weber-Fox C., Spencer R. M. C., Spruill J. E., & Smith A . ( 2004). Phonologic processing in adults who stutter: Electrophysiological and behavioral evidence. Journal of Speech Language& Hearing Research, 47( 6), 1244-1258.
doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/094) URL pmid: 15842008 |
44 |
Wei H., Dong Y., Boland J. E., & Yuan F . ( 2016). Structural priming and frequency effects interact in Chinese sentence comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 7( 45), 1-9.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00045 URL pmid: 26869954 |
45 | World Health Organization. ( 2010). International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems. Retrieved from < 2010). International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems. Retrieved from . |
46 |
Yairi E,& Ambrose N , . ( 2013). Epidemiology of stuttering: 21st century advances. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 38( 2), 66-87.
doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2012.11.002 URL pmid: 3687212 |
47 |
Yang X., Shen X., Li W., & Yang Y . ( 2014). How listeners weight acoustic cues to intonational phrase boundaries. Plos One, 9( 7), 1-9.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0102166 URL pmid: 25019156 |
48 |
Yaruss J. S., & Quesal, R. W . ( 2006). Overall assessment of the speaker's experience of stuttering (OASES): Documenting multiple outcomes in stuttering treatment. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 31( 2), 90-115.
doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2006.02.002 URL pmid: 16620945 |
49 |
Zhang J., Jiang C., Zhou L., & Yang Y . ( 2016). Perception of hierarchical boundaries in music and its modulation by expertise. Neuropsychologia, 91, 490-498.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.09.013 URL pmid: 27659874 |
50 | Zhang Y., Zhang H., & Shu H . ( 2000). A study on the processing of ambiguous phrases in Chinese. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 32( 1), 13-19. |
51 | [ 张亚旭, 张厚粲, 舒华 . ( 2000). 汉语偏正/述宾歧义短语加工初探. 心理学报, 32( 1), 13-19.] |
[1] | 陈穗清,张积家,肖二平. 图-词干扰范式下汉语口吃者的语义编码[J]. 心理学报, 2011, 43(09): 1013-1025. |
[2] | 李卫君,杨玉芳. 绝句韵律边界的认知加工及其脑电效应[J]. 心理学报, 2010, 42(11): 1021-1032. |
[3] | 宁宁,杨双,彭聃龄,丁国盛,董方白. 准备间隔对口吃者言语反应速度的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2009, 41(05): 387-396. |
[4] | 张积家,肖二平. 汉语口吃者在不出声言语中的语音编码[J]. 心理学报, 2008, 40(03): 263-273. |
[5] | 宁宁,卢春明,彭聃龄,马振玲,丁国盛. 口吃者的言语计划缺陷 ——来自词长效应的证据 [J]. 心理学报, 2007, 39(2): 215-224. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||