ISSN 0439-755X
CN 11-1911/B
主办:中国心理学会
   中国科学院心理研究所
出版:科学出版社

心理学报 ›› 2018, Vol. 50 ›› Issue (6): 592-605.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.00592

• 研究报告 • 上一篇    下一篇

知觉负载对注视知觉适应后效的影响 *

赵亚军1, 张智君2(), 刘志方3, 刘炜4, 严璘璘5, 唐溢6   

  1. 1 西南民族大学社会学与心理学学院, 成都 610041
    2 浙江大学心理与行为科学系, 杭州 310028
    3 杭州师范大学心理学系, 杭州 310036
    4 云南民族大学教育学院, 昆明 650504
    5 浙江理工大学心理学系, 杭州310018
    6 重庆市委党校, 重庆 400041
  • 收稿日期:2015-10-19 发布日期:2018-04-28 出版日期:2018-06-01
  • 通讯作者: 张智君 E-mail:zjzhang@zju.edu.cn
  • 基金资助:
    国家自然科学基金项目(31371039);国家自然科学基金项目(31500879);教育部人文社会科学研究项目(15YJC190022);西南民族大学中央高校基本科研业务费专项资金项目资助(2018SQN04)

The influence of perceptual load on gaze perceptual adaptation aftereffect

ZHAO Yajun1, ZHANG Zhijun2(), LIU Zhifang3, LIU Wei4, YAN Linlin5, TANG Yi6   

  1. 1 School of Sociology and Psychology, Southwest Minzu University, Chengdu 610041, China
    2 Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou 310028, China
    3 Department of Psychology, Hangzhou Normal University, Hangzhou 310036, China
    4 School of Education, Yunnan Minzu University, Kunming 650504, China
    5 Department of Psychology, Zhejiang Sci-Tech University, Hangzhou 310018, China
    6 Party School of the Chongqing municipal Party committee, Chongqing 400041, China
  • Received:2015-10-19 Online:2018-04-28 Published:2018-06-01
  • Contact: ZHANG Zhijun E-mail:zjzhang@zju.edu.cn

摘要:

结合知觉负载任务和注视适应范式, 考察了知觉负载(高、低知觉负载)对不同注视线索(偏离注视与直视)适应后效的影响。结果发现:(1)偏离注视在低负载条件存在显著的适应后效, 而在高负载条件仅存在微弱的适应后效, 并且前者显著大于后者; (2)直视在低负载和高负载条件都出现了显著的适应后效, 并且二者不存在显著差异; (3)注意资源对偏离注视知觉的调节作用大于对直视知觉的作用。这说明, 偏离注视知觉受到注意资源紧张程度的调节, 而直视知觉不受注意资源的调节, 注意资源对直视与偏离注视的调节作用存在差异, 推测二者可能存在不同的加工机制。

关键词: 直视, 偏离注视, 注视知觉, 注视知觉适应后效, 知觉负载

Abstract:

During social communication, eyes gaze indicates the focus of people's attention. There are two types of gaze cue: direct gaze (straight) and averted gaze (left or right), which have different social functions. Previous researchers assume that gaze perception involves two different processing stages. The first stage refers to earlier visual feature analysis of eyes such as luminance contrast and geometry of white sclera and iris. The second stage involves the perceptual integration of eyes’ features and the extraction of gaze direction. However, this assumption ignores the differences in processing mechanism between direct gaze and averted gaze. Other researchers hypothesize that perception of direct gaze is mediated by a fast-track pathway via subcortical face processing route. Direct gaze perception has characteristics of automatic process, such as capturing attention and being processed unconsciously. Its automaticity shows that direct gaze has a processing advantage over averted gaze. We inferred that direct gaze perception was independent of attentional resources, while averted gaze perception required more attentional resources. The present study aimed at investigating how attentional resources affect perception of averted and direct gaze.

We combined Lavie’s perceptual load task and a short-term gaze adaptation paradigm to investigate how attentional resources influence the gaze adaptation aftereffect of direct gaze and averted gaze. Participants attended to a letter string superimposed on gaze adaptor (200 ms), and identified the target letter (X or N) embedded in the letter string of either 6 identical letters (low load) or 6 different letters (high load). Subsequently, a gaze probe was presented for 200 ms. Participants had to make judgements concerning the direction of probe (i.e. leftward, straight, or rightward). Sixteen na?ve participants (8 females) were tested in Experiment 1, which measured the modulation of averted gaze adaptation effect by perceptual loads. And twenty-two na?ve observers (11 females) participated in Experiment 2, which explored the effects of perceptual loads on direct gaze adaptation effect.

The results of Experiment 1 revealed that a significant aftereffect of averted gaze directed toward the adapted side in the low load condition. However, such gaze adaptation effect was eliminated under high load condition. Crucially, the gaze adaptation effect in low load condition was significantly greater than that in high load condition. It was suggested that the extraction of averted gaze direction (leftward or rightward) was modulated by attentional resources (perceptual load). When a demanding competing task has exhausted all available processing capacity, the direction of averted gaze could not be extracted. While the load was low, “spill over” capacity was available to the processing of task-irrelevant gaze direction. Results of Experiment 2 showed a significant direct gaze adaptation effect both under low and high load conditions, which were not significantly different from each other. These results indicated that the perceptual adaptation of direct gaze was not influenced by attentional resources. Although limited processing capacity was fully consumed by a high load task, direct gaze could be processed as well. Compared with averted gaze, processing of direct gaze required fewer attentional resources. Therefore, requirements to attentional resources were different between processing of averted and direct gaze.

In conclusion, the current study demonstrates that the processing of averted gaze direction is not mandatory automatic and it requires some degree of attentional resources. But gaze perception does not require attentional resources for direct gaze. Our findings indicated that processing of averted and direct gaze may be mediated by different underlying mechanisms, although distinct gaze directions are coded by a similar multichannel system with separate channels coding left, direct, and right gaze. Our results replicate and extend recent findings of different processing mechanisms between averted and direct gaze. Moreover, it may promote integration between different gaze perception models, such as multichannel system and fast-track modulator model.

Key words: direct gaze, averted gaze, gaze perception, gaze perceptual adaptation aftereffect, perceptual load

中图分类号: