心理学报 ›› 2018, Vol. 50 ›› Issue (6): 592-605.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.00592
赵亚军1, 张智君2(), 刘志方3, 刘炜4, 严璘璘5, 唐溢6
收稿日期:
2015-10-19
发布日期:
2018-04-28
出版日期:
2018-06-01
通讯作者:
张智君
E-mail:zjzhang@zju.edu.cn
基金资助:
ZHAO Yajun1, ZHANG Zhijun2(), LIU Zhifang3, LIU Wei4, YAN Linlin5, TANG Yi6
Received:
2015-10-19
Online:
2018-04-28
Published:
2018-06-01
Contact:
ZHANG Zhijun
E-mail:zjzhang@zju.edu.cn
摘要:
结合知觉负载任务和注视适应范式, 考察了知觉负载(高、低知觉负载)对不同注视线索(偏离注视与直视)适应后效的影响。结果发现:(1)偏离注视在低负载条件存在显著的适应后效, 而在高负载条件仅存在微弱的适应后效, 并且前者显著大于后者; (2)直视在低负载和高负载条件都出现了显著的适应后效, 并且二者不存在显著差异; (3)注意资源对偏离注视知觉的调节作用大于对直视知觉的作用。这说明, 偏离注视知觉受到注意资源紧张程度的调节, 而直视知觉不受注意资源的调节, 注意资源对直视与偏离注视的调节作用存在差异, 推测二者可能存在不同的加工机制。
中图分类号:
赵亚军, 张智君, 刘志方, 刘炜, 严璘璘, 唐溢. (2018). 知觉负载对注视知觉适应后效的影响 *. 心理学报, 50(6), 592-605.
ZHAO Yajun, ZHANG Zhijun, LIU Zhifang, LIU Wei, YAN Linlin, TANG Yi. (2018). The influence of perceptual load on gaze perceptual adaptation aftereffect. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 50(6), 592-605.
实验条件与反应 | 注视方向 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
L10 | L5 | D0 | R5 | R10 | |
适应前基线 | |||||
向左 | 0.93 (0.08) | 0.58 (0.18) | 0.10 (0.08) | 0.01 (0.02) | 0.01 (0.02) |
直视 | 0.06 (0.07) | 0.40 (0.18) | 0.78 (0.14) | 0.31 (0.17) | 0.04 (0.04) |
向右 | 0.01 (0.01) | 0.02 (0.04) | 0.12 (0.11) | 0.68 (0.17) | 0.95 (0.05) |
低负载-适应L25 | |||||
向左 | 0.85 (0.12) | 0.42 (0.18) | 0.08 (0.09) | 0.03 (0.04) | 0.00 (0.00) |
直视 | 0.12 (0.10) | 0.53 (0.18) | 0.73 (0.19) | 0.22 (0.13) | 0.02 (0.03) |
向右 | 0.03 (0.05) | 0.05 (0.07) | 0.19 (0.16) | 0.75 (0.13) | 0.98 (0.03) |
低负载-适应R25 | |||||
向左 | 0.95 (0.09) | 0.68 (0.20) | 0.18 (0.11) | 0.08 (0.08) | 0.02 (0.03) |
直视 | 0.04 (0.08) | 0.30 (0.20) | 0.75 (0.18) | 0.50 (0.20) | 0.07 (0.06) |
向右 | 0.01 (0.02) | 0.02 (0.04) | 0.07 (0.12) | 0.42 (0.18) | 0.91 (0.07) |
高负载-适应L25 | |||||
向左 | 0.91 (0.16) | 0.51 (0.17) | 0.08 (0.06) | 0.02 (0.03) | 0.00 (0.01) |
直视 | 0.07 (0.13) | 0.44 (0.17) | 0.73 (0.18) | 0.21 (0.12) | 0.02 (0.02) |
向右 | 0.02 (0.06) | 0.05 (0.10) | 0.19 (0.17) | 0.77 (0.11) | 0.98 (0.02) |
高负载-适应R25 | |||||
向左 | 0.94 (0.08) | 0.64 (0.23) | 0.15 (0.16) | 0.06 (0.06) | 0.01 (0.02) |
直视 | 0.05 (0.07) | 0.32 (0.22) | 0.73 (0.25) | 0.36 (0.22) | 0.05 (0.77) |
向右 | 0.01 (0.02) | 0.04 (0.08) | 0.12 (0.14) | 0.58 (0.20) | 0.94 (0.76) |
表1 实验1不同知觉负载和适应条件下做“向左”、“直视”与“向右”反应的平均比例
实验条件与反应 | 注视方向 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
L10 | L5 | D0 | R5 | R10 | |
适应前基线 | |||||
向左 | 0.93 (0.08) | 0.58 (0.18) | 0.10 (0.08) | 0.01 (0.02) | 0.01 (0.02) |
直视 | 0.06 (0.07) | 0.40 (0.18) | 0.78 (0.14) | 0.31 (0.17) | 0.04 (0.04) |
向右 | 0.01 (0.01) | 0.02 (0.04) | 0.12 (0.11) | 0.68 (0.17) | 0.95 (0.05) |
低负载-适应L25 | |||||
向左 | 0.85 (0.12) | 0.42 (0.18) | 0.08 (0.09) | 0.03 (0.04) | 0.00 (0.00) |
直视 | 0.12 (0.10) | 0.53 (0.18) | 0.73 (0.19) | 0.22 (0.13) | 0.02 (0.03) |
向右 | 0.03 (0.05) | 0.05 (0.07) | 0.19 (0.16) | 0.75 (0.13) | 0.98 (0.03) |
低负载-适应R25 | |||||
向左 | 0.95 (0.09) | 0.68 (0.20) | 0.18 (0.11) | 0.08 (0.08) | 0.02 (0.03) |
直视 | 0.04 (0.08) | 0.30 (0.20) | 0.75 (0.18) | 0.50 (0.20) | 0.07 (0.06) |
向右 | 0.01 (0.02) | 0.02 (0.04) | 0.07 (0.12) | 0.42 (0.18) | 0.91 (0.07) |
高负载-适应L25 | |||||
向左 | 0.91 (0.16) | 0.51 (0.17) | 0.08 (0.06) | 0.02 (0.03) | 0.00 (0.01) |
直视 | 0.07 (0.13) | 0.44 (0.17) | 0.73 (0.18) | 0.21 (0.12) | 0.02 (0.02) |
向右 | 0.02 (0.06) | 0.05 (0.10) | 0.19 (0.17) | 0.77 (0.11) | 0.98 (0.02) |
高负载-适应R25 | |||||
向左 | 0.94 (0.08) | 0.64 (0.23) | 0.15 (0.16) | 0.06 (0.06) | 0.01 (0.02) |
直视 | 0.05 (0.07) | 0.32 (0.22) | 0.73 (0.25) | 0.36 (0.22) | 0.05 (0.77) |
向右 | 0.01 (0.02) | 0.04 (0.08) | 0.12 (0.14) | 0.58 (0.20) | 0.94 (0.76) |
实验条件与反应 | 注视方向 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
L10 | L5 | D0 | R5 | R10 | |
适应前基线 | |||||
向左 | 0.94 (0.11) | 0.65 (0.18) | 0.14 (0.10) | 0.04 (0.06) | 0.00 (0.01) |
直视 | 0.06 (0.10) | 0.33 (0.16) | 0.75 (0.16) | 0.39 (0.16) | 0.06 (0.06) |
向右 | 0.00 (0.01) | 0.02 (0.04) | 0.11 (0.12) | 0.57 (0.18) | 0.94 (0.07) |
低负载-适应D0 | |||||
向左 | 0.98 (0.04) | 0.78 (0.14) | 0.19 (0.10) | 0.05 (0.05) | 0.00 (0.01) |
直视 | 0.02 (0.04) | 0.21 (0.14) | 0.70 (0.18) | 0.28 (0.18) | 0.05 (0.08) |
向右 | 0.00 (0.01) | 0.01 (0.02) | 0.11 (0.12) | 0.67 (0.20) | 0.95 (0.08) |
高负载-适应D0 | |||||
向左 | 0.97 (0.06) | 0.78 (0.18) | 0.13 (0.06) | 0.04 (0.04) | 0.00 (0.01) |
直视 | 0.02 (0.04) | 0.20 (0.16) | 0.73 (0.18) | 0.31 (0.23) | 0.04 (0.05) |
向右 | 0.01 (0.05) | 0.02 (0.05) | 0.14 (0.19) | 0.65 (0.24) | 0.96 (0.06) |
表2 实验2不同知觉负载和适应条件下做“向左”、“直视”与“向右”反应的平均比例
实验条件与反应 | 注视方向 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
L10 | L5 | D0 | R5 | R10 | |
适应前基线 | |||||
向左 | 0.94 (0.11) | 0.65 (0.18) | 0.14 (0.10) | 0.04 (0.06) | 0.00 (0.01) |
直视 | 0.06 (0.10) | 0.33 (0.16) | 0.75 (0.16) | 0.39 (0.16) | 0.06 (0.06) |
向右 | 0.00 (0.01) | 0.02 (0.04) | 0.11 (0.12) | 0.57 (0.18) | 0.94 (0.07) |
低负载-适应D0 | |||||
向左 | 0.98 (0.04) | 0.78 (0.14) | 0.19 (0.10) | 0.05 (0.05) | 0.00 (0.01) |
直视 | 0.02 (0.04) | 0.21 (0.14) | 0.70 (0.18) | 0.28 (0.18) | 0.05 (0.08) |
向右 | 0.00 (0.01) | 0.01 (0.02) | 0.11 (0.12) | 0.67 (0.20) | 0.95 (0.08) |
高负载-适应D0 | |||||
向左 | 0.97 (0.06) | 0.78 (0.18) | 0.13 (0.06) | 0.04 (0.04) | 0.00 (0.01) |
直视 | 0.02 (0.04) | 0.20 (0.16) | 0.73 (0.18) | 0.31 (0.23) | 0.04 (0.05) |
向右 | 0.01 (0.05) | 0.02 (0.05) | 0.14 (0.19) | 0.65 (0.24) | 0.96 (0.06) |
[1] | Al-Janabi S., & Finkbeiner M . ( 2014). Responding to the direction of the eyes: In search of the masked gaze-cueing effect. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 76, 148-161. |
[2] |
Bayliss A. P., Bartlett J., Naughtin C. K., & Kritikos A . ( 2011). A direct link between gaze perception and social attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(3), 634-644.
doi: 10.1037/a0020559 URL pmid: 21038995 |
[3] |
Burra N., Hervais-Adelman A., Kerzel D., Tamietto M., de Gelder B., & Pegna A. J . ( 2013). Amygdala activation for eye contact despite complete cortical blindness. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(25), 10483-10489.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3994-12.2013 URL pmid: 23785160 |
[4] |
Burton A. M., Bindemann M., Langton S. R. H., Schweinberger S. R., & Jenkins R . ( 2009). Gaze perception requires focused attention: Evidence from an interference task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(1), 108-118.
doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.35.1.108 URL pmid: 19170474 |
[5] |
Calder A. J., Jenkins R., Cassel A., & Clifford C. W. G . ( 2008). Visual representation of eye gaze is coded by a nonopponent multichannel system. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(2), 244-261.
doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.244 URL pmid: 18473657 |
[6] |
Cave K. R., & Chen Z . ( 2016). Identifying visual targets amongst interfering distractors: Sorting out the roles of perceptual load, dilution, and attentional zoom. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 78(7), 1822-1838.
doi: 10.3758/s13414-016-1149-9 URL pmid: 27250363 |
[7] | Chen A. R., Dong B., Fang Y., Yu C. Y., & Zhang M . ( 2014). The role of cue type in the subliminal gaze-cueing effect. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 46, 1281-1288. |
[ 陈艾睿, 董波, 方颖, 于长宇, 张明 . ( 2014). 线索类型对阈下注视线索效应的影响. 心理学报, 46, 1281-1288.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2014.01281 URL |
|
[8] |
Chen Y. C., & Yeh S. L . ( 2012). Look into my eyes and I will see you: Unconscious processing of human gaze. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(4), 1703-1710.
doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.10.001 URL pmid: 23117220 |
[9] |
Doherty, M. J . ( 2006). The development of mentalistic gaze understanding. Infant and Child Development, 15, 179-186.
doi: 10.1002/icd.434 URL |
[10] |
Doherty M. J., McIntyre A. H., & Langton S. R. H . ( 2015). Developmentally distinct gaze processing systems: Luminance versus geometric cues. Cognition, 137, 72-80.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.01.001 URL pmid: 25618009 |
[11] |
Duchaine B., Jenkins R., Germine L., & Calder A. J . ( 2009). Normal gaze discrimination and adaptation in seven prosopagnosics. Neuropsychologia, 47(10), 2029-2036.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.011 URL pmid: 19467353 |
[12] |
Fang F., Murray S. O., & He S . ( 2007). Duration-dependent fMRI adaptation and distributed viewer-centered face representation in human visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 17(6), 1402-1411.
doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl053 URL pmid: 16905593 |
[13] |
Frischen A., Bayliss A. P., & Tipper S. P . ( 2007). Gaze cueing of attention: Visual attention, social cognition, and individual differences. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 694-724.
doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.4.694 URL pmid: 17592962 |
[14] | Hu Z. H., Liu Q., Zhao G., & Li H . ( 2013). The measurement of detection superiority of direct gaze affected by stimuli component information. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 45, 1217-1227. |
[ 胡中华, 刘强, 赵光, 李红 . ( 2013). 刺激特征信息影响直视探测优势测量. 心理学报, 45, 1217-1227.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2013.01217 URL |
|
[15] | Hu Z. H., Zhao G., Liu Q., & Li H . ( 2012). The detection superiority of perceived direct gaze in visual search task: Evidence from eye movements. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 44(4), 435-445. |
[ 胡中华, 赵光, 刘强, 李红 . ( 2012). 视觉搜索任务中直视探测优势的眼动研究. 心理学报, 44(4), 435-445.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2012.00435 URL |
|
[16] |
Insch P. M., Slessor G., Warrington J., & Phillips L. H . ( 2017). Gaze detection and gaze cuing in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Cognition, 116, 47-53.
doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2017.03.004 URL pmid: 28365061 |
[17] |
Jenkins R., Beaver J. D., & Calder A. J . ( 2006). I thought you were looking at me: Direction-specific aftereffects in gaze perception. Psychological Science, 17, 506-513.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01736.x URL pmid: 16771801 |
[18] |
Jenkins R., Lavie N., & Driver J . ( 2005). Recognition memory for distractor faces depends on attentional load at exposure. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12, 314-320.
doi: 10.3758/BF03196378 URL pmid: 16082812 |
[19] |
Johnson M. H., Senju A., & Tomalski P . ( 2015). The two-process theory of face processing: Modifications based on two decades of data from infants and adults. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 50, 169-179.
doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.009 URL pmid: 25454353 |
[20] |
Kloth N., & Rhodes G . ( 2016). Gaze direction aftereffects are surprisingly long-lasting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(9), 1311-1319.
doi: 10.1037/xhp0000182 URL pmid: 26962845 |
[21] |
Kloth N., Rhodes G., & Schweinberger S. R . ( 2015). Absence of sex-contingent gaze direction aftereffects suggests a limit to contingencies in face aftereffects. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1829.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01829 URL pmid: 4664652 |
[22] |
Lavie, N. ( 2005). Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 75-82.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.004 URL pmid: 15668100 |
[23] |
Lavie N., & de Fockert J. W . ( 2003). Contrasting effects of sensory limits and capacity limits in visual selective attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 65, 202-212.
doi: 10.3758/BF03194795 URL pmid: 12713239 |
[24] | Luo Y., Feng L. H., Ren M., Gu Q. Y., Zhao S. Y., & Zhang Y . ( 2017). The effect of perceptual load on processing and memorizing negative facial distractor. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 49(10), 1256-1266. |
[ 罗禹, 丰丽红, 任敏, 顾秋瑀, 赵守盈, 张禹 . ( 2017). 知觉负载对负性分心面孔的知觉加工和记忆的影响. 心理学报, 49(10), 1256-1266.] | |
[25] |
Materna S., Dicke P. W., & Their P . ( 2008). Dissociable roles of the superior temporal sulcus and the intraparietal sulcus in joint attention: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(1), 108-119.
doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20.1.108 URL pmid: 18095789 |
[26] |
Murphy G., Groeger J. A., & Greene C. M . ( 2016). Twenty years of load theory—Where are we now, and where should we go next?. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(5), 1316-1340.
doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0982-5 URL pmid: 26728138 |
[27] |
Neumann M. F., & Schweinberger S. R . ( 2008). N250r and N400 ERP correlates of immediate famous face repetition are independent of perceptual load. Brain Research, 1239, 181-190.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.08.039 URL pmid: 18793621 |
[28] |
Nummenmaa L., & Hietanen J. K . ( 2009). How attentional systems process conflicting cues: The superiority of social over symbolic orienting revisited. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1738-1754.
doi: 10.1037/a0016472 URL pmid: 19968432 |
[29] |
Ricciardelli P., & Turatto M . ( 2011). Is attention necessary for perceiving gaze direction? It depends on how you look at it: Evidence from the locus-of-slack method. Visual Cognition, 19, 154-170.
doi: 10.1080/13506285.2010.514140 URL |
[30] |
Rorden C., Guerrini C., Swainson R., Lazzeri M., & Baylis G. C . ( 2008). Event related potentials reveal that increasing perceptual load leads to increased responses for target stimuli and decreased responses for irrelevant stimuli. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 2, 4.
doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.004.2008 URL pmid: 18958205 |
[31] |
Rothkirch M., Madipakkam A. R., Rehn E., & Sterzer P . ( 2015). Making eye contact without awareness. Cognition, 143, 108-114.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.06.012 URL pmid: 26133642 |
[32] |
Sato W., Kochiyama T., Uono S., & Toichi M . ( 2016). Neural mechanisms underlying conscious and unconscious attentional shifts triggered by eye gaze. NeuroImage, 124, 118-126.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.061 URL pmid: 26343316 |
[33] |
Sato W., Okada T., & Toichi M . ( 2007). Attentional shift by gaze is triggered without awareness. Experimental Brain Research, 183, 87-94.
doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1025-x URL pmid: 17624520 |
[34] |
Senju A., & Johnson M. H . ( 2009). The eye contact effect: Mechanisms and development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 127-134.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.009 URL pmid: 19217822 |
[35] |
Shepherd, S. V . ( 2010). Following gaze: Gaze-following behavior as a window into social cognition. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 4, 5.
doi: 10.3389/fnint.2010.00005 URL pmid: 2859805 |
[36] |
Spezio M. L., Huang P. Y., Castelli F., & Adolphs R . ( 2007). Amygdala damage impairs eye contact during conversations with real people. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(15), 3994-3997.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3789-06.2007 URL pmid: 17428974 |
[37] |
Stein T., Peelen M. V., & Sterzer P . ( 2012). Eye gaze adaptation under interocular suppression. Journal of Vision, 12, 1-17.
doi: 10.1167/12.7.1 URL pmid: 22753441 |
[38] |
Stein T., Senju A., Peelen M. V., & Sterzer P . ( 2011). Eye contact facilitates awareness of faces during interocular suppression. Cognition, 119, 307-311.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.01.008 URL pmid: 3796336 |
[39] | Teufel C., Fletcher P. C., & Davis G . ( 2010). Seeing other minds: Attributed mental states influence perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(8), 376-382. |
[40] |
Von Grünau M., & Anston C . ( 1995). The detection of gaze direction: A stare-in-the-crown effect. Perception, 24, 1297-1313.
doi: 10.1068/p241297 URL pmid: 8643334 |
[41] |
Xu S., Zhang S., & Geng H. Y . ( 2011). Gaze-induced joint attention persists under high perceptual load and does not depend on awareness. Vision Research, 51(18), 2048-2056.
doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.07.023 URL pmid: 21840332 |
[42] |
Yeshurun Y., & Marciano H . ( 2013). Degraded stimulus visibility and the effects of perceptual load on distractor interference. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 289.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00289 URL pmid: 3665929 |
[43] |
Yokoyama T., Noguchi Y., & Kita S . ( 2013). Unconscious processing of direct gaze: Evidence from an ERP study. Neuropsychologia, 51(7), 1161-1168.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.04.002 URL pmid: 23603242 |
[44] |
Yokoyama T., Sakai H., Noguchi Y., & Kita S . ( 2014). Perception of direct gaze does not require focus of attention. Scientific Reports, 4, 3858.
doi: 10.1038/srep03858 URL pmid: 24618808 |
[45] | Zhang M. C., Wei P., & Zhang Q . ( 2015). The impact of supra- and sub-liminal facial expressions on the gaze-cueing effect. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 47(11), 1309-1317. |
[ 张美晨, 魏萍, 张钦 . ( 2015). 阈上阈下不同面孔表情下的注视线索提示效应. 心理学报, 47(11), 1309-1317.] |
[1] | 胡金生, 李骋诗, 王琦, 李松泽, 李涛涛, 刘淑清. 孤独症青少年的情绪韵律注意偏向缺陷:低效率的知觉模式*[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(6): 637-646. |
[2] | 张豹, 胡岑楼, 陈颜璋, 缪素媚, 黄赛. 工作记忆与知觉负载对工作记忆表征 引导注意的调节[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(8): 1009-1021. |
[3] | 罗禹, 丰丽红, 任敏, 顾秋瑀, 赵守盈, 张禹. 知觉负载对负性分心面孔的知觉加工和记忆的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(10): 1256-1266. |
[4] | 刘英杰;郭春彦;魏萍. 知觉负载影响注意选择和冲突解决的系列效应[J]. 心理学报, 2014, 46(9): 1271-1280. |
[5] | 胡中华;刘强;赵光;李红. 刺激特征信息影响直视探测优势测量[J]. 心理学报, 2013, 45(11): 1217-1227. |
[6] | 魏萍;康冠兰. 注意捕获效应受知觉负载及目标与干扰项距离的共同影响[J]. 心理学报, 2012, 44(11): 1454-1462. |
[7] | 张庆,张杰栋,胡思源,刘嘉. 不受知觉负载调节的注意捕获效应:生态信息的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2011, 43(11): 1229-1238. |
[8] | 张智君,赵亚军,占琪涛. 注视方向的知觉对注视追随行为的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2011, 43(07): 726-738. |
[9] | 叶榕,余凤琼,蒋玉宝,汪凯. 注意瞬脱范式中的知觉负载对情绪面孔加工的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2011, 43(05): 483-493. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||