高功能自闭症个体对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向
华东师范大学教育学部特殊教育学系, 华东师范大学自闭症研究中心, 上海 200062
Attention bias toward threatening emotional faces in individuals with high-functioning autism
Autism Research Center, East China Normal University, Shanghai 200062, China
通讯作者: 贺荟中, E-mail:hzhe@spe.ecnu.edu.cn
收稿日期: 2019-04-19 网络出版日期: 2020-07-15
基金资助: |
|
Received: 2019-04-19 Online: 2020-07-15
高功能自闭症(High-Functioning Autism, HFA)个体智力正常, 但也面临着严重的社会功能障碍。对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向与HFA个体社会功能的发展密切相关。梳理相关研究发现, HFA个体在自动加工阶段和情绪目标参与阶段, 不存在威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向; 而在任务与情绪无关的控制加工阶段, 存在威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向。针对HFA个体威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向的理论解释主要有杏仁核理论、强烈世界理论和执行功能理论等。神经生理机制方面, HFA个体对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向可能与其异常的皮下及皮层通路功能有关, 同时可能会受5-羟色胺系统基因及催产素水平等的影响。未来研究可在综合考虑研究方法及个体因素的基础上, 进一步探究其加工特征及神经生物机制, 着力开发科学有效的干预策略。
关键词:
High-functioning autism (HFA) is a term commonly used to identify patients with autism spectrum disorder who have average or above average intellectual abilities but also face severe social dysfunctions. Attention bias towards threatening emotional faces is closely related to the development of social function in individuals with HFA. By reviewing the related research, the authors found that individuals with HFA do not have threatening emotional face attention bias during the automatic processing stage or emotional target participation stage; however, in the control processing stage, where tasks are unrelated to emotions, they demonstrate threatening emotional face attention bias. The theoretical explanations for this threatening emotional face attention bias in individuals with HFA mainly include the Amygdala theory of autism, Intense world theory, and Executive function theory. In terms of neurophysiological mechanisms, it may be related to their abnormal subcutaneous and cortical pathway functions and may be affected by serotonin system genes and oxytocin levels. Based on a comprehensive consideration of research methods and individual factors, future research can further explore the relevant processing characteristics and neuro-biological mechanisms and make efforts to develop scientific and effective intervention strategies.
Keywords:
本文引用格式
范晓壮, 毕小彬, 谢宇, 贺荟中.
FAN Xiaozhuang, BI Xiaobin, XIE Yu, HE Huizhong.
1 引言
情绪注意偏向(affect-biased attention)是指, 包含情绪信息的刺激相对于无情绪的刺激能更迅速或持久地引发个体的注意, 得到进一步的认知加工(彭晓哲, 周晓林, 2005; 白学军, 贾丽萍, 王敬欣, 2013)。快速、准确地识别他人的情绪信号对于维系社会纽带、增强群体凝聚力和保障群体生存至关重要(van Rooijen, Ploeger, & Kret, 2017), 因此人们会对承载强烈情绪信息的面孔, 特别是传递重要警告信号的威胁性情绪面孔表现出注意偏向(Okon-Singer, Lichtenstein-Vidne, & Cohen, 2013)。威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向是一种特殊的情绪注意偏向, 指威胁性情绪面孔(主要包括愤怒、恐惧、厌恶面孔等)相比于其它情绪面孔, 能更快更多地捕获个体的注意。生物个体对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向是其适应生存和维持社会关系的必备技能, 但过度注意这些威胁信息则会引发个体的社会情绪问题(Fu & Pérez-Edgar, 2019), 束缚个体社交能力的发展(Pérez-Edgar, Taber- Thomas, Auday, & Morales, 2014; Morales, Pérez- Edgar, & Buss, 2014)。
自闭症谱系障碍(Autism Spectrum Disorders, ASD)又称自闭症或孤独症, 是一种发生在儿童早期的神经发育障碍, 其核心症状表现为社会沟通与交往障碍, 兴趣狭窄与行为刻板等(American Psychiatric Association, 2013)。临床上, ASD个体普遍存在社会情绪障碍与问题行为等(Maskey, Warnell, Parr, le Couteur, & McConachie, 2013; Horiuchi et al., 2014; Pisula et al., 2017; Irwanto, Kahfi, Febriyana, Hartini, & Takada, 2019)。研究发现, 对威胁性社会信息特别是威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向会对ASD个体社会功能的发展产生极大的影响。一方面, 对威胁性情绪面孔的异常注意偏向可能会阻碍ASD个体准确判断他人的意图及心理状态, 导致社会情绪加工障碍。如有研究发现, ASD个体较多关注社会情境中的负性情绪信息(Embregts & van Nieuwenhuijzen, 2009)。他们常错误解读面孔表情, 把中性情绪面孔识别为愤怒或伤心情绪面孔(Eack, Mazefsky, & Minshew, 2015)。另一方面, 对威胁性情绪面孔的异常注意可能会引发ASD个体的社交回避行为, 影响其社会沟通能力的发展(Zalla & Sperduti, 2013)。如以往研究发现, ASD个体对愤怒面孔的注意越少, 其社会沟通障碍越严重(García-Blanco, López- Soler et al., 2017; Matsuda, Minagawa, & Yamamoto, 2015)。因此, 对ASD个体威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向的研究, 不仅有利于揭示其情绪与认知过程的交互作用机制, 而且有利于科学干预方法的发展与完善。
目前, 关于ASD个体威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向的实验研究多以高功能自闭症(High-Functioning Autism, HFA)及阿斯伯格综合症(Aspergerʹs syndrome, AS)个体为研究对象。这是因为高功能自闭症和阿斯伯格个体认知能力较好(Koyama, Tachimori, Osada, Takeda, & Kurita, 2007), 能够理解实验要求, 配合完成实验任务。所谓高功能自闭症是指, 自闭症谱系障碍中认知能力较好, 智力正常(IQ≥70)的一部分群体(Koyama et al., 2007)。而阿斯伯格综合症也是自闭症谱系障碍的一部分1 (2013年, 美国精神疾病诊断与统计手册第五版(Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders 5th, DSM-V)取消了阿斯伯格综合症的诊断分类, 而将其纳入到了自闭症谱系障碍之中。在此之后的研究中, 也不再严格区分高功能自闭症和阿斯伯格综合症。因此, 本文也不严格区分两者, 并以高功能自闭症代指两者, 若原参考文献明确指出其研究对象为阿斯伯格个体时, 则仍用阿斯伯格来进行表述。)(American Psychiatric Association, 2013), 但他们在认知和语言能力方面均无显著落后(Macintosh & Dissanayake, 2004; American Psychiatric Association, 2013)。AS和HFA个体认知能力较好, 但他们都会表现出自闭症的核心症状(De Giambattista et al., 2019), 存在显著的社会沟通与交往障碍(Macintosh & Dissanayake, 2004)。综合众多HFA个体威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向的研究发现: (1)HFA个体在自动加工阶段不存在对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向。这可能是由于HFA个体对社会性线索缺乏兴趣, 对威胁性情绪面孔并不敏感, 不会将威胁性情绪面孔加工为威胁信息(Hollocks, Ozsivadjian, Matthews, Howlin, & Simonoff, 2013; May, Cornish, & Rinehart, 2015)。(2)在控制加工阶段, HFA个体表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向。这可能是因为, 在控制加工阶段, HFA个体依赖一定的情绪识别策略, 能够从威胁性情绪面孔中提取威胁信号。为回避这种高度厌恶的感受, 而表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意回避(Markram, Rinaldi, & Markram, 2007; Kamila & Markram, 2010)。此外也可能是由于其执行功能障碍(Hill, 2004), 而表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的过度注意偏向。(3)在(情绪)目标导向的注意阶段, HFA个体受情绪目标的影响, 并未表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向。这可能与该群体的抑制控制能力有关。
2 HFA个体对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向表现
梳理以往研究发现, 一些研究支持HFA个体存在威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向(Ashwin, Wheelwright, & Baron-Cohen, 2006b; Isomura, Ogawa, Yamada, Shibasaki, & Masataka, 2014a; Krysko & Rutherford, 2009; May, Cornish, & Rinehart, 2016; Sasson, Shasteen, & Pinkham, 2016; Zhao, Zhang, Fu, & Maes, 2016)。另一些研究则发现, HFA个体不会表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向(Hollocks et al., 2013; Isomura, Ito, Ogawa, & Masataka, 2014; May et al., 2015; Sato et al., 2017)。分析相关影响因素发现, 上述不一致的研究结果可能与注意的不同阶段有关。
根据注意的双加工理论(Schneider & Shiffrin, 1977), 对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向可能发生在注意的两个阶段, 即自动加工阶段和控制加工阶段(Mogg & Bradley, 1998)。自动加工阶段, 个体不能有意识地识别威胁性刺激; 而控制加工阶段, 个体能有意识地提取威胁信号, 并对威胁性情绪面孔做出自上而下的注意抑制或注意转移(Cisler, Bacon, & Williams, 2009)。研究者认为, 对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向可能是自动加工和控制加工协调作用的结果。一方面, 威胁性刺激的物理特性和生物进化意义, 驱动个体在无意识状态下对其进行自动化加工(Bannerman, Milders, de Gelder, & Sahraie, 2009); 另一方面, 个体会根据当前的目标、任务及已有的知识经验、期望等, 来主动调整对威胁性情绪信息的注意(Sussman, Jin, & Mohanty, 2016)。因此, 我们将从自动加工阶段、控制加工阶段和目标参与阶段对相关研究进行梳理。
2.1 自动加工阶段, HFA个体不存在威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向
有关HFA个体威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向的研究, 多以刺激呈现时间小于500 ms作为自动加工的标准。多数研究发现, 在自动加工阶段, HFA个体不存在对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向(Hollocks et al., 2013; May et al., 2015; Uono, Sato, & Toichi, 2009; García-Blanco, López-Soler et al., 2017)。如, Hollocks等(2013)、García- Blanco和López-Soler等(2017)采用点探测范式, 在屏幕的左右两端同时呈现一张情绪面孔(愤怒或者开心面孔)和一张中性情绪面孔作为线索, 持续500 ms后线索消失, 并出现探测点, 要求被试对探测点做出判断。结果发现HFA组和典型发展(Typically Developmental, TD)组均未表现出对威胁性情绪(愤怒)面孔的注意偏向。根据注意偏向的成分理论, 注意偏向包含注意警觉(vigilance)、注意解除困难(difficulty in disengaging)和注意回避(attentional avoidance or shift)三个成分(Cisler et al., 2009; Cisler & Koster, 2010)。其中, 注意警觉指, 与中性刺激相比, 个体会优先注意威胁性刺激(Koster, Verschuere, Burssens, Custers, & Crombez, 2007)。注意解除困难指, 相比于中性刺激, 个体更难从威胁性刺激中解除注意。注意回避指, 个体回避对威胁性刺激的注意(Cisler & Koster, 2010)。因此, 为进一步检测HFA个体在上述三个成分上的表现, May等(2015)采用改良的点探测范式(即增加“中性-中性”情绪面孔对线索作为参照), 以500 ms作为线索呈现时间, 同样发现HFA个体不存在对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向, 也不存在对威胁性情绪面孔的注意警觉、注意解除困难和注意回避。由上述几项研究可知, 在500 ms刺激呈现时间条件下, HFA个体和TD个体都不存在对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向。但Uono等(2009)以情绪面孔伴随动态视线为线索, 在屏幕上呈现460 ms后, 要求被试对探测点做出反应。结果显示HFA个体没有表现出对恐惧情绪面孔的注意偏向, 而TD个体表现出对恐惧情绪面孔的注意偏向。也有研究采用情绪Stroop任务同样发现, TD个体对威胁性情绪面孔有注意偏向, 而AS个体没有表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向(Ashwin, Wheelwright, & Baron- cohen, 2006a)。此外, 还有少量研究发现, HFA个体在自动加工阶段存在威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向。如Monk等(2010)采用点探测范式发现, HFA个体和TD个体均表现出对开心、愤怒面孔的注意偏向。在注意偏向的成分上, Zhao等(2016)采用空间线索任务, 即先在屏幕的左侧或右侧呈现一张情绪面孔(厌恶或开心、中性情绪面孔)作为线索, 随后消失并出现探测点, 要求被试对探测点做出判断。结果发现HFA个体存在对厌恶面孔的注意偏向, 具体表现为对厌恶情绪面孔的注意警觉。除此之外, 还有一项研究发现HFA伴随特质焦虑个体表现出对愤怒面孔的注意偏向(Hollocks, Pickles, Howlin, & Simonoff, 2016)。
综上所述, 在自动加工阶段, 多数研究支持HFA个体不存在对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向, 少量研究得到了不一致的结论, 可能的原因是: (1)研究中所采用的情绪类型不同。Zhao等(2016)采用的是厌恶面孔, 而其他研究多采用愤怒面孔。有研究指出, 厌恶面孔虽然与恐惧面孔都传达负性信号, 但愤怒面孔传递直接的威胁信息, 而厌恶面孔则传达反感、人际拒绝等信号(Rozin, Lowery, & Ebert, 1994), 因此HFA个体可能会对愤怒和厌恶面孔产生不同的注意反应。(2)研究中HFA个体的焦虑状态不同。如Hollocks等(2016)发现, 伴随特质焦虑的HFA个体表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向, 而无焦虑的HFA个体则没有表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向。(3)点探测/空间线索任务的信度不足。Monk等(2010)、Hollocks等(2013)、García-Blanco和López-Soler等(2017)采用了相同的研究范式、实验程序、和威胁性情绪类型, 但研究结果仍存在差异。这可能是由于500 ms刺激呈现时间中, 个体会有多个隐性注意的转换 (Posner & Peterson, 1990), 使得点探测任务本身信度不足(Waechter, Nelson, Wright, Hyatt, & Oakman, 2014), 而造成研究结果的差异。
2.2 控制加工阶段, HFA个体存在对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向
研究者多以刺激呈现时间大于1000 ms作为控制加工的条件(Yiend, 2010)。已有研究采用视觉搜索任务、点探测/空间线索任务、自由观看(眼动)任务等来检测HFA个体在控制加工阶段的注意偏向表现。上述任务中, 被试均不需要对情绪信息做直接反应。结果显示, 在控制加工阶段, HFA个体存在对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向, 但不同任务中, 注意偏向的具体成分存在差异。
2.2.1 视觉搜索任务中, HFA个体存在威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向
一些研究采用视觉搜索范式发现, HFA个体表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向(Ashwin et al., 2006b; Isomura et al., 2014a; Isomura, Ogawa, Yamada, Shibasaki, & Masataka, 2014b; Krysko & Rutherford, 2009; May et al. 2016; Rosset et al., 2011)。该任务要求被试判断所呈现的面孔是否相同, 以被试做出判断的时间或速度作为注意偏向的指标(Fox et al., 2000)。Ashwin等(2016b)采用简笔画情绪面孔(该研究指出简笔画材料可消除许多低级感知因素的影响, 从而能够更好地控制实验变量), Rosset等(2011)采用卡通画情绪面孔作为刺激材料, 均发现HFA组与TD组都表现出了对愤怒面孔的注意偏向。考虑到刺激材料的生态学效度, Krysko和Rutherford (2009)、May等(2016)采用真人面孔照片为材料, 发现HFA组与TD组一致表现出了“愤怒面孔优势效应”(Anger Superiority Effect)。当然有研究指出, 虽然HFA个体和TD个体都表现出了对愤怒面孔的注意偏向, 但两组个体的加工策略可能并不相同。TD个体采用了整体构形策略, 而HFA个体更偏向采用细节加工策略(Isomura et al., 2014b)。统合上述研究结果发现, HFA个体在视觉搜索任务中表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向, 且不受刺激类型(简笔画、卡通画、真人面孔照片等)的影响, 但HFA个体威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向的加工策略还有待进一步研究。
2.2.2 点探测/空间线索任务中, HFA个体存在威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向
当面孔对线索呈现时间超过1000 ms时, García- Blanco和López-Soler等(2017)、Ghosn等(2018)采用点探测范式的研究发现: HFA个体对威胁性情绪面孔(愤怒)表现出更强的注意回避, 而TD组无此表现。Zhao等(2016)采用空间线索范式的研究结果显示, HFA儿童表现出对厌恶面孔的注意警觉, 随后表现出注意回避趋势。García-Blanco, Yáñez, Vázquez, Marcos和Perea (2017)、Ghosn等(2018)采用点探测范式, 给被试呈现1250 ms情绪场景图片对, 结果发现HFA个体对威胁性情绪场景表现出了优先注意。
上述采用点探测/空间线索任务的研究结果显示, 在控制加工阶段, HFA个体表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向, 但注意偏向的具体成分存在争议。以单纯的情绪面孔作为刺激材料的研究发现, HFA个体表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意回避; 而对威胁性情绪场景的注意偏向表现为优先注意。这可能是因为HFA个体在情绪场景材料中更多采用了细节加工策略, 因而能够优先注意威胁性情绪信息(Ghosn et al., 2018)。
2.2.3 自由观看任务中, HFA个体对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向
自由观看任务中, 被试只需自由注视刺激材料而不用做其它的操作反应, 采用眼动设备记录被试的眼动轨迹和数据。这类任务以注视情绪面孔的首次注视潜伏期、首次注视时长、总注视时间为注意偏向指标(Armstrong & Olatunji, 2012)。总体来说, 在自由观看任务中, HFA个体表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向, 但注意偏向的具体成分尚不一致。一些研究发现, HFA个体对威胁性情绪面孔表现出注意解除困难。如, 陈顺森(2012)给被试呈现单独的情绪面孔时, HFA个体对不同情绪面孔的觉察时间差异不显著, 但对恐惧情绪面孔的注视时间长于中性或愉快面孔, 即存在对恐惧情绪面孔的注意解除困难。为探测背景因素对觉察情绪面孔的影响, 陈顺森、白学军、沈德立、闫国利和张灵聪(2011)将独立的情绪面孔嵌入中性风景图片中, 让被试自由观察时(时间为5s), 同样发现HFA个体表现出对恐惧情绪面孔的注意解除困难。Crawford, Moss, Anderson, Oliver和McCleery (2015)让被试自由观看厌恶-中性情绪或者开心-中性情绪面孔对(每个情绪面孔对呈现1500 ms)。结果发现HFA个体观看厌恶表情的时间比例高于中性情绪面孔, 而观看开心面孔的时间比例与中性情绪面孔无显著性差异, 即HFA个体表现出对厌恶表情的注意解除困难。另一些研究发现, ASD个体对威胁性情绪面孔表现出注意警觉和注意解除困难。若将情绪面孔嵌入乱序的环境背景中时, ASD个体表现出对恐惧情绪面孔的注意警觉和注意解除困难(陈顺森 等, 2011)。当情绪面孔处于与中性情绪面孔的竞争条件中时, Unruh, Bodfish和Gotham (2018)发现HFA个体表现出对愤怒面孔的注意警觉和注意解除困难, 但TD个体对开心面孔存在注意偏向。当威胁性情绪面孔处在更具竞争性的刺激中(包括3张非面孔图, 1张恐惧情绪面孔, 1张开心面孔, 和1张平静面孔)时, HFA个体依然表现出对恐惧情绪面孔的注意警觉和注意解除困难(陈顺森, 2012)。但White, Maddox和Panneton (2015)让被试自由观看面孔对材料时, 以每个500 ms内注视情绪刺激的时长作为统计指标, 发现HFA个体表现出对厌恶面孔的注意回避。
总结上述研究发现, 多数研究支持HFA个体在自由观看任务中表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向, 但注意偏向的具体成分不同。这种不同可能与目标刺激的呈现背景(即有无竞争或干扰刺激)有关。当威胁性情绪面孔独立呈现(无背景的独立面孔或语义不相关背景下的独立面孔)时, HFA个体仅表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意解除困难。而当威胁性情绪面孔处于竞争条件下时(与中性或平静、开心面孔同时出现), HFA个体则表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意警觉和注意解除困难。White等(2015)未发现注意警觉和注意解除困难, 可能是由于该研究采用的注意偏向指标不同。该研究以时段内注视时长为判断指标, 异于其它研究, 且这种时段分析对注意回避更加敏感(Armstrong & Olatunji, 2012)。但首次注视潜伏期、首次注视时长能更直接地反映初始的注意行为, 故当两种分析方式并存时, 研究者倾向于优先采用首次注视潜伏期和首次注视时长作为判定指标(Armstrong & Olatunji, 2012)。
2.3 情绪目标参与阶段, HFA个体不存在威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向
前面所述的研究均采用了情绪信息与操作要求不相关的任务, 即被试不需对情绪面孔直接做反应。当被试要完成的任务与情绪信息相关时, 被试需要意志控制来对情绪信息做精细加工。抑制控制能力受损可能会造成HFA个体不会表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向。Isomura等(2014)以简笔画面孔为材料, 要求被试指出与干扰面孔不同的目标情绪面孔。结果发现HFA组注意愤怒面孔和开心面孔的时间不存在显著差异, 但TD组注意愤怒面孔更快。Farran, Branson和King (2011)采用真人黑白图像为材料, 让被试听录音中的目标表情, 并在干扰面孔中指出所听到的表情。结果发现HFA组未表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向。当刺激材料为更复杂的多种族多性别的情绪面孔时, HFA个体未表现出威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向(Sasson et al., 2016)。此外结合眼动技术的研究也指出, HFA个体不存在对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向。林云强(2012)采用真人面孔材料, 要求被试指出不同情绪面孔的位置, 结果发现, HFA个体存在对开心面孔的注意偏向(首次注视高兴面孔的时间短), 而非威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向。在此基础上, 林云强等通过改变面孔方向(林云强, 刘宝根, 陈冠杏, 2014)、搜索矩阵的大小(林云强, 唐泉, 2015)、面孔群持续时间(林云强, 曹贤佳, 朱慧敏, 李菲菲, 2019)、面孔偏转(林云强, 童叶莹, 2016)等, 均发现HFA个体存在正性情绪面孔注意偏向, 而非威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向。
上述各研究均要求被试指出不同情绪面孔的精确位置或类型。因此, 在任务开始前, 被试便预先知道所呈现的面孔材料矩阵中存在不一致的面孔表情, 从而更多依赖自上而下的搜索策略来完成任务(LoBue & Matthews, 2014)。此外, 被试可能为回避威胁性信号带来的厌恶感受, 而不愿意去点击威胁性刺激, 造成对威胁性情绪面孔的反应时更慢(LoBue & Matthews, 2014)。因此, HFA个体可能能够依赖自上而下的外显策略识别情绪面孔(Harms, Martin, & Wallace, 2010), 但因其无法抑制威胁性情绪面孔的干扰(Herrington et al., 2017), 而不会表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向。
3 HFA个体对威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向的原因探析
综合已有研究结果, HFA个体在自动加工阶段及情绪目标参与阶段, 不存在对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向, 而在(情绪信息与操作任务不相关的)控制加工阶段, 表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向。HFA是自闭症谱系障碍的一部分群体, 以下结合自闭症相关理论尝试对现有结论做分析与解释。
3.1 自闭症的杏仁核理论(The Amygdala Theory of Autism)
一般来说, 人们会对有价值的刺激或与自己相关的刺激表现出注意偏向, 而忽略不相关的刺激(Cisler et al., 2009)。自动加工阶段, ASD个体不存在对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向, 可能是因为他们没有把威胁性情绪面孔加工为重要的或者威胁到自己的信息, 不能从威胁性情绪面孔中提取出威胁信号。
自闭症的杏仁核理论认为, ASD个体在识别情绪面孔时杏仁核激活不足, 因此不能准确识别他人情绪, 不能根据他人的情绪表情推断他人的想法和意图(Baron-Cohen et al., 2000)。已有研究发现, ASD个体对情绪面孔的加工存在障碍, 尤其表现为对负性情绪的加工存在异常。如, Lacroix, Guidetti, Rogé和Reilly (2014)采用情绪标签任务(Emotion labeling task)、情绪匹配任务(Emotion matching task)、情绪识别任务(Emotion identification task)发现4~8岁的ASD儿童表现出负性情绪识别困难。Yeung, Lee和Chan (2019)发现ASD青少年(11~18岁)同样表现出负性情绪面孔识别障碍。也有研究指出, ASD个体能够依赖后天习得的情绪识别策略, 在外显的情绪识别任务中表现出正常的情绪识别能力(Harms et al., 2010)。但在内隐情绪加工或短时情绪知觉时, ASD个体依然存在情绪面孔的加工障碍(Wagner, Hirsch, Vogel-Farley, Redcay, & Nelson, 2013)。因此, 当一些研究以外显的情绪识别任务来检测被试的情绪识别能力时, 会发现HFA个体在自动加工阶段的威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向与其情绪识别能力没有显著性相关(Hollocks et al., 2013; May et al., 2015)。由此推测, 在自动加工阶段, HFA个体内隐地加工情绪信号的能力缺损, 没有把威胁性情绪面孔加工为威胁信息, 而不会对威胁性情绪面孔产生注意偏向。或者HFA个体可能过度依赖策略去识别表情, 而忽略了对威胁性信号的加工(May et al., 2015), 而没有对威胁性情绪面孔产生注意偏向。只有当HFA个体将威胁性情绪面孔和威胁信号建立联结时, 他们才会表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向(Isomura et al., 2014a)。
3.2 强烈世界理论(The Intense World Theory)
从生物进化的角度来看, ASD个体也会表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向, 加之其异常的知觉能力, 使得他们表现出过度或者异于常人的注意偏向表现。强烈世界理论可解释ASD个体在控制加工阶段的表现。
强烈世界理论是基于动物模型(丙戊酸鼠)的神经影像学研究而提出的。该理论认为ASD个体的核心障碍可能是由杏仁核及大脑新皮质等脑区的局部神经微环路(local neural microcircuits)功能过强造成的(Markram et al., 2007; Kamila & Markram, 2010)。具体表现为, 在知觉和认知层面, 受大脑新皮层调节而产生过度知觉(hyper-perception)、过度注意(hyper-attention)、过度记忆(hyper-memory); 在情感层面, 受边缘系统脑区过度功能调节而造成过度情绪反应(hyper-emotionality) (Kamila & Markram, 2010)。因此, ASD个体可能并不是对环境中的社会情绪信号不感兴趣, 而是因为这些情感信号和特征过于强烈, 使得杏仁核过度反应, 将这些信息加工为了反感或厌恶的刺激。ASD个体为回避这种情感压力和紧张感, 而表现出社交退缩行为(Markram et al., 2007; Kamila & Markram, 2010)。该理论得到了一些实证研究的支持, Tottenham等(2014)发现, ASD个体评定中性情绪面孔的威胁性等级越高, 杏仁核激活水平越高。Kleinhans等(2010)在情绪面孔匹配任务中发现, 相比于TD个体, ASD个体左侧前额叶激活减弱, 枕叶激活增强。这种激活模式表明, ASD个体高阶脑区的相对整合力较低, 而感觉区的功能过强。因此回避高唤醒刺激可能是一种适应性反应, 以战胜强烈物理刺激带来的高度情绪唤起及过度的厌恶感受。由此可以解释, 在控制加工阶段, 为克服威胁性情绪面孔诱发的高度痛苦感受, HFA个体会主动调节自身的注意, 从而表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意回避(Dalton et al., 2005; Corden, Chilvers, & Skuse, 2008; Kliemann, Dziobek, Hatri, Steimke, & Heekeren 2010; Zalla & Sperduti, 2013; García-Blanco, López-Soler et al., 2017)。
3.3 执行功能异常假说(Executive Dysfunction)
研究发现, 典型发展个体对威胁性情绪面孔的注意与其抑制控制能力有关(Auday, Taber- Thomas, & Perez-Edgar, 2018)。随着年龄的发展, 典型发展个体会逐渐发展出成熟的抑制能力, 抑制对威胁性刺激的自动加工, 以更好的适应社会生存(Reinholdt-Dunne, Mogg, Esbjorn, & Bradley, 2012; Field & Lester, 2010; Morales, Fu, & Pérez- Edgar, 2016)。ASD个体对威胁性情绪面孔的异常注意偏向可能与其执行功能有关。
执行功能包含认知灵活性、抑制控制、工作记忆等多个高级认知加工过程, 主要指个体根据当前情境或目标任务, 灵活调整自身行为, 包括抑制与当前任务不相符的行为而表现出恰当的行为(Hill, 2004)。ASD个体存在认知灵活性和注意转换障碍(贺荟中, 梁志高, 2013), 表现为难以从当前的任务或刺激离开而注意到新任务或新刺激, 即注意解除困难(Bryson et al., 2018; Sacrey, Armstrong, Bryson, & Zwaigenbaum, 2014)。此外, ASD个体还存在优势行为抑制障碍(Prepotent Response Inhibition)和干扰抑制(Interference Control)障碍(Geurts, van den Bergh, & Ruzzano, 2014), 即无法根据当前情境或目标抑制不恰当的行为(Adams & Jarrold, 2012)。已有研究多采用视觉搜索范式, 发现HFA个体难将注意从威胁性情绪面孔中解除出来(林云强, 2012; 陈顺森 等, 2011)。而在对威胁性情绪面孔的注意抑制能力方面, 已有研究发现ASD个体受不相关负性社会信息的干扰较大(Herrington et al., 2017)。以此推测, 在控制加工阶段, ASD个体的执行功能异常可能会造成对威胁性情绪面孔的注意解除困难; 在情绪目标参与阶段, ASD个体无法抑制威胁性信息的干扰, 因而不会表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向。
4 HFA个体对威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向的潜在神经机制
目前, ASD个体对面孔情绪信息注意的神经机制研究主要集中于与TD个体的对比研究。神经影像学研究表明, 普通发展个体对威胁性情绪信息的注意偏向可能有两条神经通路。一是皮层下快速加工通路, 即“上丘-枕核-杏仁核”皮层下警觉系统, 能够在无意识状态下对威胁性刺激进行快速、粗略的识别和检测(Tamietto & de Gelder, 2010)。然后将信息反馈给视觉皮层, 增强视觉皮层对威胁性情绪信息的反应, 从而产生注意偏向。另一个为皮层通路, 由杏仁核、脑岛、前扣带回、前额皮层、眶额皮层等共同对威胁性刺激进行准确细致地识别加工(Sierra-Mercado, Padilla- Coreano, & Quirk, 2011)。从注意偏向的具体成分上看, 注意警觉主要与杏仁核-前扣带回网络联结有关, 而注意解除与前额叶皮层(特别是眶额叶皮层)功能相关(张禹, 罗禹, 赵守盈, 陈维, 李红, 2014)。从已有研究来看, HFA个体可能存在皮下通路及皮层通路的功能异常。
4.1 HFA个体可能存在皮下通路功能异常
有研究表明, 在行为反应上, HFA个体与TD个体都表现出对开心和愤怒面孔的注意偏向; 但在神经生理反应上, HFA个体表现出了更大的右侧杏仁核激活, 及较弱的杏仁核-颞叶功能联结(Monk et al., 2010)。这说明HFA个体与TD个体加工威胁性情绪面孔的神经机制并不同。为探测具体的脑区反应, Leung等采用脑磁技术(magnetoencephalography, MEG)发现, TD个体加工愤怒面孔时(相对于快乐面孔), 右侧枕下叶、距状沟和中央后区活动更大, 但HFA个体加工不同情绪面孔时的神经反应没有显著性差异(Leung, Pang, Anagnostou, & Taylor, 2018)。这说明HFA个体可能缺乏对不同情绪面孔的神经敏感性。此外, Wagner等(2013)结合眼动和事件相关电位技术(Event Related Potentials, ERP), 发现HFA组与TD组对情绪面孔的视觉注视行为相似, 而脑电反应不同。HFA组在三种表情(愤怒、恐惧、中性面孔)下的N170成分没有显著差异, 而TD组对恐惧情绪面孔有更大的N170反应。该研究推测HFA个体可能经过训练后, 在外显的眼动轨迹上能够达到与TD个体一致的水平, 但HFA个体对情绪面孔的加工机制仍不同于TD个体。综上, HFA个体的皮下通路异常, 可能会造成其威胁性情绪面孔知觉异常(Fishman, Linke, Hau, Carper, & Müller, 2018)。这使得HFA个体在自动加工阶段, 因无法快速知觉威胁信号, 而不会对威胁性情绪面孔产生注意偏向。
4.2 HFA个体可能存在皮层通路功能异常
研究指出, TD儿童早期主要依赖皮下通路(如杏仁核)对威胁性信息进行加工, 随后发展出成熟的威胁性信息加工机制, 即依赖皮上通路来进行加工(Hung, Smith, & Taylor, 2012)。为探测HFA个体对情绪面孔的注意机制, Leung等(2015)让被试注视屏幕, 随后一张情绪面孔图片和一张乱码模糊图分别呈现在屏幕左右两边, 持续80 ms, 要求被试判断乱码图的位置。结果发现TD个体加工威胁性情绪面孔时激活了杏仁核、扣带回区域, 而HFA个体对威胁性情绪面孔表现出扣带回激活减弱, 说明HFA个体未发展出成熟的威胁性情绪面孔的处理能力。此外, 威胁性情绪面孔对HFA个体和TD个体的意义可能并不相同。如Ashwin等给被试呈现单独的情绪面孔时, 发现TD组知觉恐惧情绪面孔时, 负责自动情感评估的社会脑区激活更大, 如左侧杏仁核和左侧眶额叶皮层的激活更大, 这表明恐惧情绪面孔对TD个体具有重要的情感意义(Ashwin, Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, O’Riordan, & Bullmore, 2007)。而高功能自闭症及阿斯伯格组则表现出杏仁核和眶额叶皮层激活不足, 说明他们提取和整合情感信息的能力存在损伤。由于眶额叶皮层与社会抑制、行为调节功能有关(van Honk, Peper, & Schutter, 2005)。HFA个体在加工愤怒面孔时, 眶额叶区域激活不足(Leung et al., 2015)使得他们无法根据他人的情绪信息来调整自己的行为。此外, Courchesne和Pierce (2005)发现, HFA个体还表现出长距离的皮质联结减少, 以及额叶功能衰竭等, 使得其无法有效整合信息, 阻碍向初级感觉区域提供反馈信息。因此, 在控制加工阶段, HFA个体可能因整合情绪信息的能力受损, 且无法有效调整自己的行为, 而表现出异常的威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向。
从电生理反应的研究来看, Torrence和Troup (2018)指出, TD个体对愤怒面孔的注意偏向表现为更负的N2pc; 对恐惧情绪面孔的注意偏向表现出更大的C1及N170反应; 且对出现在威胁性情绪面孔之后的探测点有更大的P1反应。这说明TD个体注意不同情绪面孔时会产生不同的脑电反应, 且对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向会进一步影响个体注意随后出现的刺激。对比TD个体的脑电反应, de Jong, van Engeland和Kemner (2008)发现, ASD个体注视恐惧情绪面孔和中性情绪面孔时诱发出的N170成分没有显著差异, 说明ASD个体可能难以区分恐惧情绪面孔和中性情绪面孔。他们无法从愤怒、恐惧等威胁性情绪面孔中提取威胁或警告信号, 因此, 在注意早期阶段, 他们不会表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向。而在加工的中后期, Dawson等发现, 3~4岁TD儿童观看恐惧情绪面孔比中性情绪面孔表现出更大的N300, 但3~4岁HFA儿童注视恐惧情绪面孔和中性情绪面孔的N300成分的振幅不存在显著差异(Dawson, Webb, Carver, Panagiotides, & Mcpartland, 2004)。在加工的晚期成分上, TD儿童对恐惧情绪面孔的负慢波(Negative Slow Wave, NSW)振幅更大, 而HFA儿童则无此表现(Dawson et al., 2004)。这表明, 早期阶段, HFA儿童未表现出对恐惧情绪面孔的优先注意; 加工后期阶段, HFA个体对恐惧情绪面孔的进一步知觉和概念加工也存在异常。从而也支持了HFA个体对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向可能会表现出阶段性差异, 在不同阶段可能均会表现出异常的加工。
4.3 HFA个体情绪信息注意偏向的相关基因研究进展
研究指出, 脑神经发育异常可能只是下游症状, 非发病机制(武文佼, 张鹏, 2016)。不同基因的变异可能会影响其编码的蛋白质结构及突触传导功能, 进而影响神经发育, 紊乱脑区功能, 造成认知及行为障碍(巨兴达, 宋伟, 徐婧, 2018)。目前关于ASD的常见风险基因多达1000多个(Xiong et al., 2019), 罕见变异基因69个(Ruzzo et al., 2019)。因此, 某些神经递质和基因类型可能与ASD个体对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向有关。5-羟色胺(5-hydroxytryptamine, 5-HT)系统基因被认为是自闭症的候选基因之一, 其中, 5-羟色胺转运体(SLC6A4)等位基因长度会影响个体对情绪信息特别是负性情绪信息的神经反应。如, 短等位基因携带者观看愤怒面孔时, 杏仁核激活更大(von dem Hagen, Passamonti, Nutland, Sambrook, & Calder, 2011), 且主要体现在注意的早期阶段(Faja, Dawson, Aylward, Wijsman, & Webb, 2016)。另外, 5-HT水平是ASD的生化易感性指标之一(Leboyer et al., 1999)。可能在发育早期, ASD个体的脑部5-HT水平存在异常, 进而影响了其额叶、颞叶、顶叶和枕叶等几个皮层区域的发育(Chandana et al., 2005; Bachevalier & Loveland, 2006)。因此, 5-HT水平的改变可能会影响ASD个体的脑神经发育, 进而使ASD个体有异常的行为表现(Fernández, Mollinedo-Gajate, & Peñagarikano, 2018)。另一个与情绪性信息注意有关的候选基因是催产素受体基因(oxytocin receptor gene, OXTR)。研究发现, OXTR多态性可能会影响其蛋白表达和合成, 从而导致边缘脑系统内特定的神经元网络激活不足, 影响情绪信息的加工及空间定向(Kranz et al., 2016)。而催产素(Oxytocin, OXT)本身也是一种重要的神经调节因子, 常被用来作为社会功能紊乱的生物指标(Kanat et al., 2017)。研究发现, 提高ASD个体的催产素水平, 能够调节该群体对面孔的注意(Kanat et al., 2017), 增加其对正性和中性面孔眼睛区域的注意(Kanat, Heinrichs, Mader, van Elst, & Domes, 2015)。这可能是因为催产素增强了“社会脑”各部分之间的联结, 并抑制了对威胁性情绪线索的杏仁核反应, 减弱了对厌恶信号或身体强烈刺激的焦虑和回避, 而提高了自上而下的注意力资源分配能力(Andari, Richard, Leboyer, & Sirigu, 2016)。基因及神经递质的研究使得我们从生物基础层面加深了对ASD的认识, 然而, 引发ASD的特定致病基因尚未明确。多数研究支持ASD是多种基因变异相互影响控制的结果(Pilorge et al., 2016), 不同的基因和基因组的变异可能会产生相同的行为表现(武文佼, 张鹏, 2016)。因此, 要揭示基因变异对ASD个体的影响, 仍需要进行多学科、多层次、多领域的综合探索, 针对已经发现的风险基因, 进一步分析基因对ASD个体的作用机制, 开发有效的治疗方案。
5 总结与展望
综上所述, HFA个体对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向可能具有阶段性特征。在自动加工阶段和情绪目标参与阶段, HFA个体不存在对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向; 而在控制加工阶段, HFA个体表现出对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向, 但注意偏向的具体成分受实验范式及刺激材料等的影响而不同。已有研究不仅丰富了我们对ASD个体的认识, 也为开展科学有效的干预提供了新的思路。结合对已有文献的分析, 未来可考虑从以下几方面开展研究。
第一, 强化对ASD个体威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向的特点及机制研究
一方面, 优化研究方法, 进一步揭示ASD个体对威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向的时间进程和阶段特征。注意是一个持续动态的过程, 但已有的行为实验范式(如点探测/空间线索任务)只能探测到目标出现时刻的注意表现, 无法记录注意偏向发生的时间进程(文涛, 汪亚珉, 丁锦红, 2011)。再加上, 当前的行为实验范式还存在信度较低等问题(Staugaard, 2009; Waechter & Stolz, 2015)。未来研究可结合信度较高且可记录注意时间进程的眼动技术(Sears, Quigley, Fernandez, Newman, & Dobson, 2018)和事件相关电位技术(Kappenman, Farrens, Luck, & Proudfit, 2014), 进一步分析ASD个体对威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向发生的阶段和时间特征。此外, 现有的研究对注意偏向成分的分析还存争议, 这可能与所采用的研究范式有关。如, 目前研究多采用视觉搜索任务发现HFA个体对威胁性情绪面孔表现出注意解除困难, 但该任务在剥离注意偏向成分上并无优势(罗禹, 高朋, 赵守盈, 张禹, 2017)。研究者常用间隙-重叠范式(gap-overlap paradigm)来研究个体的注意解除能力, 发现ASD个体表现出对几何图形(Bryson et al., 2018)、限制性兴趣(齐亚菲, 梁良, 莫书亮, 王福兴, 2016)的注意解除困难; 但对社会性刺激(Katarzyna, Fred, & Ami, 2010)、不熟悉的卡通角色(Wilson & Saldaña, 2018)的注意解除更快。因此, 未来研究可结合间隙-重叠范式, 进一步验证ASD个体对威胁性情绪面孔的注意解除特征。
另一方面, 简化实验任务, 提高研究方法的适用范围。作为一种谱系障碍, ASD群体内部的异质性极大, 智力和语言发展能力等均会影响ASD个体的功能发展(Goodwin, Matthews, & Smith, 2017; Chiang et al., 2018)。Field和Lester (2010)指出对威胁性信息的注意偏向会随着个体的发展(年龄, 认知发展)和个性特征(如气质、焦虑)的变化而改变。如, 儿童早期阶段, 个体普遍会表现出对威胁性信息的注意偏向; 随着年龄及执行功能的发展, 个体能够发展出成熟的抑制注意威胁性信息的能力, 因此不再表现出对威胁性信息的注意偏向(Field & Lester, 2010)。此外, 个人因素(如焦虑、抑郁、恐惧特质)也是造成威胁性信息注意偏向的重要因素, 高焦虑及抑郁个体会表现出特定的威胁性信息注意偏向(Morales et al., 2016)。如前所述, 对ASD个体威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向的研究多以HFA个体为研究对象, 且尚不清楚年龄、认知水平、及共患病(如焦虑、抑郁)等个人因素对ASD个体威胁性信息注意偏向的影响(May et al., 2015; Isomura et al., 2014a; Hollocks et al., 2013)。未来可开发适合低功能及不同类型ASD个体的实验程序, 减少对被试任务理解和操作的要求, 进一步分析不同因素对其注意偏向的影响。
第二, 深入开展ASD个体威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向的神经机制及与基因相关的研究
一方面, 已有的神经机制研究多以单独呈现的情绪面孔为刺激材料(Torrence, & Troup, 2018; Ashwin et al., 2007; Dawson et al., 2004), 不能揭示注意偏向产生时的神经反应。研究指出, 不同刺激之间的竞争可能是注意偏向产生的先决条件(Cisler et al., 2009)。因此未来研究应考虑在创设竞争的条件下(如结合点探测范式), 分析ASD个体对威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向的神经反应。另外, 脑神经发育异常可能只是下游症状(武文佼, 张鹏, 2016), ASD个体神经机制的异常可能与其基因及神经递质功能有关。异常基因及神经递质功能可能会影响关键神经信号的传导和基因表达水平, 进而影响神经发育, 紊乱脑区功能, 造成ASD个体的认知及行为障碍(巨兴达 等, 2018)。因此, 若能将基因功能研究与ASD个体的行为特征研究(如对威胁性情绪面孔的注意偏向)相结合, 一方面可揭示ASD的发病机制, 另一方面也能指导对ASD个体的教育和干预。因此未来研究可结合脑神经科学技术、基因影像技术等, 从脑区结构及功能、神经生物反馈、生物遗传等多方面, 从根源上对ASD个体威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向的作用机制进行探讨。
第三, 开展ASD个体对威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向的治疗或干预研究
对ASD个体威胁性情绪面孔注意的干预不仅有利于减少其情绪问题行为, 还能够促进其社交能力的发展, 帮助其更好地参与社会生活。研究发现, 对情绪信息的注意偏向可以通过神经疗法得到改善, 也可通过行为干预得到提高。如, 对高自闭特质个体鼻喷催产素, 能够提高他们对正性和中性情绪面孔的注意(Xu et al., 2015); 而认知行为疗法能够减少个体对威胁性信息的注意偏向(Hadwin & Richards, 2016)。但是, 目前对ASD个体威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向的干预研究很少, 更没有形成体系(Luxford, Hadwin, & Kovshoff, 2017)。因此, 未来研究可结合现有的研究成果, 开发科学的干预或治疗方法, 为ASD个体提供支持和服务。首先, 结合神经生物学研究成果, 开发药物或针剂治疗方案, 如催产素的使用计量、治疗时间及适用人群分析等(Quintana et al., 2017)。其次, 在已有研究基础上, 开发实用的筛查、评估工具, 以便于分析不同ASD个体对威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向发生的阶段和原因, 为制定个别化的干预方案提供科学的依据。再次, 开发和完善干预方法和技术, 提高干预效果。认知行为干预法、注意偏向训练等, 对改善ASD个体对社会性信息的注意有一定成效(Alvares et al., 2019)。未来研究可进一步分析这些干预方法对ASD个体威胁性情绪面孔注意偏向的作用机制及适用范围, 完善干预体系建设。
参考文献
Inhibition in Autism: Children with Autism have difficulty Inhibiting irrelevant distractors but not prepotent responses
,
Brief social attention bias modification for children with autism spectrum disorder
,
Adaptive coding of the value of social cues with oxytocin, an fMRI study in autism spectrum disorder
,
Eye tracking of attention in the affective disorders: A meta-analytic review and synthesis
,
Differential activation of the amygdala and the ‘social brain’ during fearful face- processing in Asperger syndrome
,DOI:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.014 URL PMID:16806312 [本文引用: 2]
Impaired social cognition is a core feature of autism. There is much evidence showing people with autism use a different cognitive style than controls for face-processing. We tested if people with autism would show differential activation of social brain areas during a face-processing task. Thirteen adults with high-functioning autism or Asperger Syndrome (HFA/AS) and 13 matched controls. We used fMRI to investigate 'social brain' activity during perception of fearful faces. We employed stimuli known to reliably activate the amygdala and other social brain areas, and ROI analyses to investigate brain areas responding to facial threat as well as those showing a linear response to varying threat intensities. We predicted: (1) the HFA/AS group would show differential activation (as opposed to merely deficits) of the social brain compared to controls and (2) that social brain areas would respond to varied intensity of fear in the control group, but not the HFA/AS group. Both predictions were confirmed. The controls showed greater activation in the left amygdala and left orbito-frontal cortex, while the HFA/AS group showed greater activation in the anterior cingulate gyrus and superior temporal cortex. The control group also showed varying responses in social brain areas to varying intensities of fearful expression, including differential activations in the left and right amygdala. This response in the social brain was absent in the HFA/AS group. HFA/AS are associated with different patterns of activation of social brain areas during fearful emotion processing, and the absence in the HFA/AS brain of a response to varying emotional intensity.
Attention bias to faces in Asperger syndrome: A pictorial emotion Stroop study
.DOI:10.1017/S0033291706007203 URL PMID:16512970 [本文引用: 2]
BACKGROUND: Emotional Stroop tasks have shown attention biases of clinical populations towards stimuli related to their condition. Asperger Syndrome (AS) is a neuropsychiatric condition with social and communication deficits, repetitive behaviours and narrow interests. Social deficits are particularly striking, including difficulties in understanding others. METHOD: We investigated colour-naming latencies of adults with and without AS to name colours of pictures containing angry facial expressions, neutral expressions or non-social objects. We tested three hypotheses: whether (1) controls show longer colour-naming latencies for angry versus neutral facial expressions with male actors, (2) people with AS show differential latencies across picture types, and (3) differential response latencies persist when photographs contain females. RESULTS: Controls had longer latencies to pictures of male faces with angry compared to neutral expressions. The AS group did not show longer latencies to angry versus neutral expressions in male faces, instead showing slower latencies to pictures containing any facial expression compared to objects. When pictures contained females, controls no longer showed longer latencies for angry versus neutral expressions. However, the AS group still showed longer latencies to all facial picture types, compared to objects, providing further evidence that faces produce interference effects for this clinical group. CONCLUSIONS: The pictorial emotional Stroop paradigm reveals normal attention biases towards threatening emotional faces. The AS group showed Stroop interference effects to all facial stimuli regardless of expression or sex, suggesting that faces cause disproportionate interference in AS.
Finding a face in the crowd: Testing the anger superiority effect in Asperger syndrome
.
Neural correlates of attention bias to masked facial threat cues: Examining children at-risk for social anxiety disorder
,
The orbitofrontal- amygdala circuit and self-regulation of social-emotional behavior in autism
,DOI:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.07.002 URL PMID:16157377 [本文引用: 1]
Individuals with an autistic spectrum disorder are impaired not only in understanding others' mental states, but also in self-regulation of social-emotional behavior. Therefore, a model of the brain in autism must encompass not only those brain systems that subserve social-cognitive and emotional functioning, but also those that subserve the self-regulation of behavior in response to a changing social environment. We present evidence to support the hypothesis that developmental dysfunction of the orbitofrontal-amygdala circuit of the brain is a critical factor in the development of autism and that some of the characteristic deficits of persons with autism in socio-emotional cognition and behavioral self-regulation are related to early dysfunction of different components of this circuit. A secondary hypothesis posits that the degree of intellectual impairment present in individuals with autism is directly related to the integrity of the dorsolateral prefrontal-hippocampal circuit of the brain. Together, these hypotheses have the potential to help explain the neurodevelopmental basis of some of the primary manifestations of autism as well as the heterogeneity of outcomes.
Orienting to threat: Faster localization of fearful facial expressions and body postures revealed by saccadic eye movements
,
The amygdala theory of Autism
,
Impaired disengagement of attention and its relationship to emotional distress in infants at high-risk for autism spectrum disorder
,
Significance of abnormalities in developmental trajectory and asymmetry of cortical serotonin synthesis in autism
,DOI:10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.08.002 URL PMID:15749243 [本文引用: 1]
The role of serotonin in prenatal and postnatal brain development is well documented in the animal literature. In earlier studies using positron emission tomography (PET) with the tracer alpha[(11)C]methyl-l-tryptophan (AMT), we reported global and focal abnormalities of serotonin synthesis in children with autism. In the present study, we measured brain serotonin synthesis in a large group of autistic children (n = 117) with AMT PET and related these neuroimaging data to handedness and language function. Cortical AMT uptake abnormalities were objectively derived from small homotopic cortical regions using a predefined cutoff asymmetry threshold (>2 S.D. of normal asymmetry). Autistic children demonstrated several patterns of abnormal cortical involvement, including right cortical, left cortical, and absence of abnormal asymmetry. Global brain values for serotonin synthesis capacity (unidirectional uptake rate constant, K-complex) values were plotted as a function of age. K-complex values of autistic children with asymmetry or no asymmetry in cortical AMT uptake followed different developmental patterns, compared to that of a control group of non-autistic children. The autism groups, defined by presence or absence and side of cortical asymmetry, differed on a measure of language as well as handedness. Autistic children with left cortical AMT decreases showed a higher prevalence of severe language impairment, whereas those with right cortical decreases showed a higher prevalence of left and mixed handedness. Global as well as focal abnormally asymmetric development in the serotonergic system could lead to miswiring of the neural circuits specifying hemispheric specialization.
School dysfunction in youth with autistic spectrum disorder in Taiwan: The effect of subtype and ADHD
,
Phenomenological characteristics of attentional biases towards threat: A critical review
,
Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative review
,
Avoidance of emotionally arousing stimuli predicts social-perceptual impairment in Asperger's syndrome
,DOI:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.08.005 URL PMID:17920642 [本文引用: 1]
We combined eye-tracking technology with a test of facial affect recognition and a measure of self-reported social anxiety in order to explore the aetiology of social-perceptual deficits in Asperger's syndrome (AS). Compared to controls matched for age, IQ and visual-perceptual ability, we found a group of AS adults was impaired in their recognition of fearful and sad expressions and spent significantly less time fixating the eye region of all faces. For AS subjects, but not controls, the extent of the failure to fixate the eyes predicted the degree of impairment at recognising fearful expressions. In addition, poor fear recognition and reduced fixation of the eyes were independently associated with greater levels of social anxiety in AS individuals. These findings support the hypothesis that avoidance of emotionally arousing stimuli, such as eyes, contributes to social-perceptual impairment in AS. Furthermore, our findings are consistent with theories implicating amygdala-mediated over-arousal and anxiety in the development of these social-perceptual deficits.
Why the frontal cortex in autism might be talking only to itself: Local over- connectivity but long-distance disconnection
,
Implicit discrimination of basic facial expressions of positive/negative emotion in Fragile X Syndrome and Autism Spectrum Disorder
,
Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism
,DOI:10.1038/nn1421 URL PMID:15750588 [本文引用: 1]
Diminished gaze fixation is one of the core features of autism and has been proposed to be associated with abnormalities in the neural circuitry of affect. We tested this hypothesis in two separate studies using eye tracking while measuring functional brain activity during facial discrimination tasks in individuals with autism and in typically developing individuals. Activation in the fusiform gyrus and amygdala was strongly and positively correlated with the time spent fixating the eyes in the autistic group in both studies, suggesting that diminished gaze fixation may account for the fusiform hypoactivation to faces commonly reported in autism. In addition, variation in eye fixation within autistic individuals was strongly and positively associated with amygdala activation across both studies, suggesting a heightened emotional response associated with gaze fixation in autism.
Young children with autism show atypical brain responses to fearful versus neutral facial expressions of emotion
,
Subtyping the autism spectrum disorder: Comparison of children with high functioning Autism and Asperger Syndrome
,
Attentional effects of gaze shifts are influenced by emotion and spatial frequency, but not in Autism
,DOI:10.1097/CHI.0b013e31816429a6 URL PMID:18356706 [本文引用: 1]
OBJECTIVE: Impaired gaze following is an important hallmark of autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) in clinical settings. Yet, ASD subjects perform normally on laboratory tasks involving gaze shifts. We investigated this contradiction, hypothesizing that impaired gaze following in ASDs is not related to basic impairments in attention orienting but to impaired emotion perception and abnormal processing of spatial frequencies (i.e., local and global information). METHOD: We tested 30 high-functioning, school-age children with ASDs and 30 age- and IQ-matched controls on a task involving gaze shifts that cue the location of targets. The cueing faces differed in emotionality and were filtered for different spatial frequencies. We recorded behavioral responses (reaction times) and brain responses (event-related potentials). RESULTS: ASD subjects performed normally when neutral faces were used. However, emotional faces elicited modified face and gaze cue processing in control subjects, but not in the ASD subjects. Furthermore, the control group was biased toward the use of low spatial frequencies (global information) to process gaze cues, whereas the ASD group was biased toward the use of high spatial frequencies (local information). CONCLUSIONS: We conclude that impaired gaze following in ASDs is related to impaired emotion processing. Moreover, ASD subjects show an abnormal reliance on local information to process gaze cues.
Misinterpretation of facial expressions of emotion in verbal adults with autism spectrum disorder
,
Social information processing in boys with Autistic Spectrum Disorder and mild to borderline intellectual disabilities
,
Early event-related potentials to emotional faces differ for adults with autism spectrum disorder and by serotonin transporter genotype
,
Visual search for basic emotional expressions in autism; impaired processing of anger, fear and sadness, but a typical happy face advantage
,
Neural circuits for social cognition: Implications for Autism
,
Is there room for ‘development’ in developmental models of information processing biases to threat in children and adolescents?
,
Atypical functional connectivity of amygdala related to reduced symptom severity in children with Autism
,DOI:10.1016/j.jaac.2018.06.015 URL PMID:30274651 [本文引用: 1]
OBJECTIVE: Converging evidence indicates that brain abnormalities in autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) involve atypical network connectivity. Given the central role of social deficits in the ASD phenotype, this investigation examined functional connectivity of the amygdala-a brain structure critically involved in processing of social information-in children and adolescents with ASDs, as well as age-dependent changes and links with clinical symptoms. METHOD: Resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) data from 55 participants with ASDs and 50 typically developing (TD) controls, aged 7 to 17 years, were included. Groups were matched for age, gender, IQ, and head motion. Functional connectivity MRI (fcMRI) analysis was applied to examine intrinsic functional connectivity (iFC) of the amygdala, including cross-sectional tests of age-related changes. RESULTS: Direct between-group comparisons revealed reduced functional connectivity between bilateral amygdalae and left inferior occipital cortex, accompanied by greater connectivity between right amygdala and right sensorimotor cortex in the ASD group. This atypical pattern of amygdala connectivity was associated with decreased symptom severity and better overall functioning, as specifically seen in an ASD subgroup with the most atypical amygdala iFC but the least impaired social functioning. Age-related strengthening of amygdala-prefrontal connectivity, as observed in the TD group, was not detected in children with ASDs. CONCLUSION: Findings support aberrant network sculpting in ASDs, specifically atypical integration between amygdala and primary sensorimotor circuits. Paradoxical links between atypical iFC and behavioral measures suggest that abnormal amygdala functional connections may be compensatory in some individuals with ASDs.
Facial expressions of emotion: Are angry faces detected more efficiently?
,
Threat-related attention bias in socioemotional development: A critical review and methodological considerations
,DOI:10.1016/j.dr.2018.11.002 URL PMID:32205901 [本文引用: 1]
Cross-sectional evidence suggests that attention bias to threat is linked to anxiety disorders and anxiety vulnerability in both children and adults. However, there is a lack of developmental evidence regarding the causal mechanisms through which attention bias to threat might convey risks for socioemotional problems, such as anxiety. Gaining insights into this question demands longitudinal research to track the complex interplay between threat-related attention and socioemotional functioning. Developing and implementing reliable and valid assessments tools is essential to this line of work. This review presents theoretical accounts and empirical evidence from behavioral, eye-tracking, and neural assessments of attention to discuss our current understanding of the development of normative threat-related attention in infancy, as well as maladaptive threat-related attention patterns that may be associated with the development of anxiety. This review highlights the importance of measuring threat-related attention using multiple attention paradigms at multiple levels of analysis. In order to understand if and how threat-related attention bias in real-life, social interactive contexts can predict socioemotional development outcomes, this review proposes that future research cannot solely rely on screen-based paradigms but needs to extend the assessment of threat-related attention to naturalistic settings. Mobile eye-tracking technology provides an effective tool for capturing threat-related attention processes in vivo as children navigate fear-eliciting environments and may help us uncover more proximal bio-psycho-behavioral markers of anxiety.
Communication deficits and avoidance of angry faces in children with autism spectrum disorder
,DOI:10.1016/j.ridd.2017.02.002 URL [本文引用: 6]
Modulation of attention by socio- emotional scenes in children with autism spectrum disorder
,
Prepotent response inhibition and interference control in autism spectrum disorders: Two meta-analyses
,
Attentional patterns to emotional faces versus scenes in children with autism spectrum disorders
,
The effects of early language on age at diagnosis and functioning at school age in children with autism spectrum disorder
,
Working memory training and CBT reduces anxiety symptoms and attentional biases to threat: A preliminary study
,
Facial emotion recognition in autism spectrum disorders: A review of behavioral and neuroimaging studies
,
Negative valence in Autism Spectrum Disorder: The relationship between amygdala activity, selective attention, and co-occurring anxiety
,
Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in Autism
,
The relationship between attentional bias and anxiety in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders
,DOI:10.1002/aur.1285 URL PMID:23907924 [本文引用: 7]
Dual cognitive and biological correlates of anxiety in autism spectrum disorders
,
Age- and sex-related emotional and behavioral problems in children with autism spectrum disorders: Comparison with control children
,
Development of ACC-amygdala activations in processing unattended fear
,DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.003 URL PMID:22182768 [本文引用: 1]
The ability to assess facial expressions of others involves specialised brain systems important for emotional and social learning, a skill that emerges over childhood. We investigated the development of neural responses associated with implicit processing of facial emotions using magnetoencephalography in children (7-10 yrs), adolescents (12-15 yrs) and adults. The results demonstrated spatial-temporal activations in the ACC and amygdala emotion-processing systems that changed with age. The processing of emotions first engaged the earlier-developing amygdala responses and then involved the later-maturing ACC system. With increasing age there was a shift in lateralization of amygdala responses sensitive to the fearful faces. The findings contribute to a critical understanding of the development related to functional specialization of fear perception in the frontal-limbic emotion systems. The present study offers critical insights into the developmentally time-sensitive impact on the normal functioning of these brain regions.
Emotional and behavioral problems of pre-school children with autistic spectrum disorder assessed by the Child Behavior Checklist 1-5
,
Absence of predispositional attentional sensitivity to angry faces in children with autism spectrum disorders
,
The effect of inversion on the anger superiority effect in children with and without autism spectrum disorders
,
Preliminary evidence that different mechanisms underlie the anger superiority effect in children with and without autism spectrum disorders
,
The Intense World Theory - A unifying theory of the neurobiology of autism
,DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2010.00224 URL PMID:21191475 [本文引用: 4]
Autism covers a wide spectrum of disorders for which there are many views, hypotheses and theories. Here we propose a unifying theory of autism, the Intense World Theory. The proposed neuropathology is hyper-functioning of local neural microcircuits, best characterized by hyper-reactivity and hyper-plasticity. Such hyper-functional microcircuits are speculated to become autonomous and memory trapped leading to the core cognitive consequences of hyper-perception, hyper-attention, hyper-memory and hyper-emotionality. The theory is centered on the neocortex and the amygdala, but could potentially be applied to all brain regions. The severity on each axis depends on the severity of the molecular syndrome expressed in different brain regions, which could uniquely shape the repertoire of symptoms of an autistic child. The progression of the disorder is proposed to be driven by overly strong reactions to experiences that drive the brain to a hyper-preference and overly selective state, which becomes more extreme with each new experience and may be particularly accelerated by emotionally charged experiences and trauma. This may lead to obsessively detailed information processing of fragments of the world and an involuntarily and systematic decoupling of the autist from what becomes a painfully intense world. The autistic is proposed to become trapped in a limited, but highly secure internal world with minimal extremes and surprises. We present the key studies that support this theory of autism, show how this theory can better explain past findings, and how it could resolve apparently conflicting data and interpretations. The theory also makes further predictions from the molecular to the behavioral levels, provides a treatment strategy and presents its own falsifying hypothesis.
Oxytocin modulates amygdala reactivity to masked fearful eyes
,
Restoring effects of oxytocin on the attentional preference for faces in autism
,
Behavioral and ERP measures of attentional bias to threat in the dot-probe task: Poor reliability and lack of correlation with anxiety
,DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01368 URL PMID:25538644 [本文引用: 1]
The dot-probe task is often considered a gold standard in the field for investigating attentional bias to threat. However, serious issues with the task have been raised. Specifically, a number of studies have demonstrated that the traditional reaction time (RT) measure of attentional bias to threat in the dot-probe task has poor internal reliability and poor test-retest reliability. In addition, although threatening stimuli capture attention in other paradigms, attentional bias to threat has not usually been found in typical research participants in the dot-probe task. However, when attention is measured in the dot-probe task with the N2pc component of the event-related potential waveform, substantial attentional orienting to threat is observed, and the internal reliability is moderate. To provide a rigorous comparison of the reliability of this N2pc measure and the conventional behavioral measure, as well as to examine the relationship of these measures to anxiety, the present study examined the N2pc in conjunction with RT in the dot-probe task in a large sample of participants (N = 96). As in previous studies, RT showed no bias to threatening images across the sample and exhibited poor internal reliability. Moreover, this measure did not relate to trait anxiety. By contrast, the N2pc revealed a significant initial shift of attention to threat, and this measure was internally reliable. However, the N2pc was not correlated with trait anxiety, indicating that it does not provide a meaningful index of individual differences in anxiety in the dot-probe task. Together, these results indicate a serious need to develop new tasks and methods to more reliably investigate attentional bias to threat and its relationship to anxiety in both clinical and non-clinical populations.
Limited attentional bias for faces in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders
,
Association between amygdala response to emotional faces and social anxiety in autism spectrum disorders
,DOI:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.022 URL [本文引用: 1]
AbstractDifficulty interpreting facial expressions has been reported in autism spectrum disorders (ASD) and is thought to be associated with amygdala abnormalities. To further explore the neural basis of abnormal emotional face processing in ASD, we conducted an fMRI study of emotional face matching in high-functioning adults with ASD and age, IQ, and gender matched controls. In addition, we investigated whether there was a relationship between self-reported social anxiety and fMRI activation. During fMRI scanning, study participants were instructed to match facial expressions depicting fear or anger. The control condition was a comparable shape-matching task. The control group evidenced significantly increased left prefrontal activation and decreased activation in the occipital lobes compared to the ASD group during emotional face matching. Further, within the ASD group, greater social anxiety was associated with increased activation in right amygdala and left middle temporal gyrus, and decreased activation in the fusiform face area. These results indicate that level of social anxiety mediates the neural response to emotional face perception in ASD.]]>
Atypical reflexive gaze patterns on emotional faces in autism spectrum disorders
,DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0688-10.2010 URL PMID:20844124 [本文引用: 1]
Atypical scan paths on emotional faces and reduced eye contact represent a prominent feature of autism symptomatology, yet the reason for these abnormalities remains a puzzle. Do individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) fail to orient toward the eyes or do they actively avoid direct eye contact? Here, we used a new task to investigate reflexive eye movements on fearful, happy, and neutral faces. Participants (ASDs: 12; controls: 11) initially fixated either on the eyes or on the mouth. By analyzing the frequency of participants' eye movements away from the eyes and toward the eyes, respectively, we explored both avoidance and orientation reactions. The ASD group showed a reduced preference for the eyes relative to the control group, primarily characterized by more frequent eye movements away from the eyes. Eye-tracking data revealed a pronounced influence of active avoidance of direct eye contact on atypical gaze in ASDs. The combination of avoidance and reduced orientation into an individual index predicted emotional recognition performance. Crucially, this result provides evidence for a direct link between individual gaze patterns and associated social symptomatology. These findings thereby give important insights into the social pathology of ASD, with implications for future research and interventions.
Attention for emotional faces under restricted awareness revisited:Do emotional faces automatically attract attention?
,DOI:10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.285 URL PMID:17516808 [本文引用: 1]
Theoretical models of attention for affective information have assigned a special status to the cognitive processing of emotional facial expressions. One specific claim in this regard is that emotional faces automatically attract visual attention. In three experiments, the authors investigated attentional cueing by angry, happy, and neutral facial expressions that were presented under conditions of limited awareness. In these experiments, facial expressions were presented in a masked (14 ms or 34 ms, masked by a neutral face) and unmasked fashion (34 ms or 100 ms). Compared with trials containing neutral cues, delayed responding was found on trials with emotional cues in the unmasked, 100-ms condition, suggesting stronger allocation of cognitive resources to emotional faces. However, in both masked and unmasked conditions, the hypothesized cueing of visual attention to the location of emotional facial expression was not found. In contrary, attentional cueing by emotional faces was less strong compared with neutral faces in the unmasked, 100-ms condition. These data suggest that briefly presented emotional faces influence cognitive processing but do not automatically capture visual attention.
Cognitive and symptom profiles in Asperger's syndrome and high-functioning autism
,DOI:10.1111/j.1440-1819.2007.01617.x URL PMID:17239046 [本文引用: 1]
Asperger syndrome (AS) and autistic disorder are two subtypes of pervasive developmental disorders (PDD), but there has been considerable debate over whether AS and autistic disorder without mental retardation (IQ > or = 70), called high-functioning autism (HFA), are distinct conditions or not. The aim of the present paper was to clarify this issue through a comparison of cognitive function and autistic symptom profiles. Based on the DSM-IV and ICD-10 definitions of language acquisition, 36 age- and IQ-balanced subjects with AS (mean age, 12.8 years; mean full-scale IQ, 98.3) were compared with 37 subjects with HFA (mean age, 12.6 years; mean full-scale IQ, 94.6) on the Japanese version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scales and the Childhood Autism Rating Scale-Tokyo Version (CARS-TV). Compared with the HFA subjects, the AS subjects scored significantly higher on Verbal IQ, Vocabulary, and Comprehension, but scored significantly lower on Coding. Although the total CARS-TV score did not differ significantly between the two groups, AS subjects scored significantly lower (i.e. less abnormal) on Verbal communication and Non-verbal communication than did the HFA subjects. A history of normal language acquisition in early childhood could predict his/her better verbal ability in mid-childhood or later. Autistic cognitive characteristics shared by both AS and HFA subjects appear to support the validity of the current diagnostic classification of PDD.
Meta-analysis and association of two common polymorphisms of the human oxytocin receptor gene in autism spectrum disorder
,DOI:10.1002/aur.1597 URL PMID:26788924 [本文引用: 1]
Neuropeptides such as oxytocin (OXT) are known facilitators of social behavior across species. Variants of the OXT receptor gene (OXTR) have been tested for association with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) across manifold ethnicities, yielding both positive and negative findings. A recent meta-analysis, comprising 16 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs), has corroborated the implication of OXTR in the etiology of ASD. Here, we genotyped and tested two additional variants (rs237889 and rs237897) for association with ASD in two German predominantly high-functioning ASD samples. We found nominal over-transmission (OR = 1.48, CI95 = 1.06-2.08, P = 0.022) for the minor A allele of variant rs237889G>A in sample 1 (N = 135 complete parent-offspring trios, 29 parent child duos), but not in sample 2 (362 trios, 69 duos). Still, in a meta-analysis comprising four different studies including the two unreported German data sets (N = 542 families), this finding was confirmed (OR = 1.12; CI95 = 1.01-1.24, random effects P = 0.012). In addition, carriers of the minor risk allele rs237889-A showed significantly increased severity scores, as assessed through the autism diagnostic interview - revised (ADI-R), with highly significant increases in social interaction deficits. Our results corroborate the implication of common OXTR variants in the etiology of ASD. There is a need for functional studies to delineate the neurobiological implications of this and other association findings. (172/250). Autism Res 2016, 9: 1036-1045. (c) 2016 International Society for Autism Research, Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
A threat-detection advantage in those with autism spectrum disorders
,DOI:10.1016/j.bandc.2008.10.002 URL [本文引用: 3]
AbstractIdentifying threatening expressions is a significant social perceptual skill. Individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are impaired in social interaction, show deficits in face and emotion processing, show amygdala abnormalities and display a disadvantage in the perception of social threat. According to the anger superiority hypothesis, angry faces capture attention faster than happy faces in individuals with a history of typical development [Hansen, C. H., & Hansen, R. D. (1988). Finding the face in the crowd: An anger superiority effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 917–924]. We tested threat detection abilities in ASD using a facial visual search paradigm. Participants were asked to detect an angry or happy face image in an array of distracter faces. A threat-detection advantage was apparent in both groups: participants showed faster and more accurate detection of threatening over friendly faces. Participants with ASD showed similar reaction time, but decreased overall accuracy compared to controls. This provides evidence for less robust, but intact or learned implicit processing of basic emotions in ASD.]]>
Facial emotion recognition in 4-to 8-year-olds with autism spectrum disorder: A developmental trajectory approach
,DOI:10.1016/j.rasd.2014.05.012 URL [本文引用: 1]
The investigation of emotion recognition in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) has both theoretical and practical implications. However, although many studies have examined facial emotion recognition in ASD, some points remain unclear. We therefore studied facial emotion recognition in young children with ASD across a small age range, in order to determine (1) their ability to recognize emotion and (2) the developmental trajectory of this ability. Twenty-two children with ASD aged 4-8 years were compared with typically developing children matched on either chronological age or verbal mental age. We administered three facial emotion tasks: matching, identification, and labeling. Results showed that children with ASD and typically developing children had difficulty with labeling emotions, but not with matching or identifying them. Happiness was the easiest to recognize, and surprise the hardest. The children with ASD did not exhibit delayed onset in the development of facial emotion recognition. To conclude, emotion recognition difficulties in children with ASD primarily concern the recognition of negative emotions and the identification of surprise, as they do in TD groups. This should be taken into account in future research, as well as in the design of future intervention programs. (C) 2014 Elsevier Ltd.
Whole blood serotonin and plasma beta-endorphin in autistic probands and their first-degree relatives
,DOI:10.1016/s0006-3223(97)00532-5 URL PMID:9951562 [本文引用: 1]
BACKGROUND: Whole blood serotonin (5-HT) and C-terminally directed beta-endorphin protein immunoreactivity (C-ter-beta-EP-ir) are known to be elevated in autistic subjects and might be possible markers of genetic liability to autism. This study thus investigates the familial aggregation of 5-HT and of C-ter-beta-EP-ir levels in first degree relatives of autistic probands. METHODS: In a sample of 62 autistic subjects and 122 of their first-degree relatives, compared to age and sex-matched controls, we measured 5-HT by radioenzymology and C-ter-beta-EP-ir by radioimmunoassay. RESULTS: We confirm the previously reported familiality of hyperserotoninemia in autism as mothers (51%), fathers (45%) and siblings (87%) have elevated levels of 5-HT, and we reveal presence of elevated levels of C-ter-beta-EP-ir in mothers (53%) of autistic subjects. CONCLUSIONS: Familial aggregation of quantitative variables, such as concentration of neurotransmitters, within unaffected relative could serve as an intermediate phenotype and might thus help the search of genetic susceptibility factors in autism.
Young adults with autism spectrum disorder show early atypical neural activity during emotional face processing
DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2020.00193 URL PMID:32581743 [本文引用: 1]
Early neural activation during facial affect processing in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder
,DOI:10.1016/j.nicl.2014.11.009 URL PMID:25610782 [本文引用: 2]
Impaired social interaction is one of the hallmarks of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Emotional faces are arguably the most critical visual social stimuli and the ability to perceive, recognize, and interpret emotions is central to social interaction and communication, and subsequently healthy social development. However, our understanding of the neural and cognitive mechanisms underlying emotional face processing in adolescents with ASD is limited. We recruited 48 adolescents, 24 with high functioning ASD and 24 typically developing controls. Participants completed an implicit emotional face processing task in the MEG. We examined spatiotemporal differences in neural activation between the groups during implicit angry and happy face processing. While there were no differences in response latencies between groups across emotions, adolescents with ASD had lower accuracy on the implicit emotional face processing task when the trials included angry faces. MEG data showed atypical neural activity in adolescents with ASD during angry and happy face processing, which included atypical activity in the insula, anterior and posterior cingulate and temporal and orbitofrontal regions. Our findings demonstrate differences in neural activity during happy and angry face processing between adolescents with and without ASD. These differences in activation in social cognitive regions may index the difficulties in face processing and in comprehension of social reward and punishment in the ASD group. Thus, our results suggest that atypical neural activation contributes to impaired affect processing, and thus social cognition, in adolescents with ASD.
The snake in the grass revisited: An experimental comparison of threat detection paradigms
,DOI:10.1080/02699931.2013.790783 URL PMID:23668328 [本文引用: 2]
The current investigation compares the results of two commonly used visual detection paradigms-the standard adult button-press detection paradigm used in Ohman, Flykt, and Esteves (2001), and the new child-friendly touch-screen detection paradigm used in LoBue and DeLoache (2008)-within the same samples of adult participants. Results suggest that both paradigms produce the same pattern of findings with regard to detection latency for threat-relevant versus threat-irrelevant stimuli: Adults detected threat-relevant targets more quickly than threat-irrelevant targets across the varying procedures. However, results with respect to automaticity of detection as suggested by Ohman et al. (2001) were only replicated with the classic button-press paradigm. The findings validate the touch-screen visual search procedure and have important implications for choosing an appropriate methodology for studying threat detection.
Evaluating the effectiveness of a school-based cognitive behavioural therapy intervention for anxiety in adolescents diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder
,DOI:10.1007/s10803-016-2857-7 URL PMID:27440250 [本文引用: 1]
This study evaluated the effectiveness of a school-based Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT) on symptoms of anxiety, social worry and social responsiveness, and indices of attentional control and attentional biases to threat in adolescents diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Thirty-five young people (11-14 years; IQ > 70) with ASD and elevated teacher or parent reported anxiety were randomly assigned to 6 sessions of the Exploring Feelings CBT intervention (Attwood in Exploring feelings (anxiety). Future Horizons, Arlington, 2004) (n = 18) or a wait-list control group (n = 17). The intervention (compared to the wait-list control) group showed positive change for parent, teacher and self-reported anxiety symptoms, and more marginal effects of increased teacher-reported social responsiveness. The discussion highlights the potential value and limitations of school-based CBT for young people with ASD.
Annotation: The similarities and differences between autistic disorder and Asperger's disorder: A review of the empirical evidence
,DOI:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00234.x URL PMID:15055363 [本文引用: 2]
BACKGROUND: The ongoing controversy over the distinction between autistic disorder and Asperger's disorder is important to resolve because of the implications regarding an understanding of the aetiology and prognosis, and the diagnostic and clinical practices relating to these conditions. This paper provides a critical evaluation of current published research evidence. METHOD: Databases, such as PsychINFO and Medline, as well as book chapters, reference lists from relevant articles, and recent editions of key journals were searched for all relevant studies (until 2002) which incorporated participants diagnosed with high-functioning autism and Asperger's disorder using either cluster analysis or comparative approaches to examine similarities and differences between these groups. Keywords used in the searches included autistic disorder, Asperger's disorder, autism, high-functioning autism, and pervasive developmental disorder. RESULTS: On the basis of the available evidence, there seem to be few qualitative differences between autistic disorder and Asperger's disorder. CONCLUSION: There is currently insufficient evidence to establish the validity of Asperger's disorder as a syndrome distinct from high-functioning autism. The findings are consistent with the view that these disorders belong on an autism spectrum.
The Intense World Syndrome - An alternative hypothesis for autism
,DOI:10.3389/neuro.01.1.1.006.2007 URL PMID:18982120 [本文引用: 3]
Autism is a devastating neurodevelopmental disorder with a polygenetic predisposition that seems to be triggered by multiple environmental factors during embryonic and/or early postnatal life. While significant advances have been made in identifying the neuronal structures and cells affected, a unifying theory that could explain the manifold autistic symptoms has still not emerged. Based on recent synaptic, cellular, molecular, microcircuit, and behavioral results obtained with the valproic acid (VPA) rat model of autism, we propose here a unifying hypothesis where the core pathology of the autistic brain is hyper-reactivity and hyper-plasticity of local neuronal circuits. Such excessive neuronal processing in circumscribed circuits is suggested to lead to hyper-perception, hyper-attention, and hyper-memory, which may lie at the heart of most autistic symptoms. In this view, the autistic spectrum are disorders of hyper-functionality, which turns debilitating, as opposed to disorders of hypo-functionality, as is often assumed. We discuss how excessive neuronal processing may render the world painfully intense when the neocortex is affected and even aversive when the amygdala is affected, leading to social and environmental withdrawal. Excessive neuronal learning is also hypothesized to rapidly lock down the individual into a small repertoire of secure behavioral routines that are obsessively repeated. We further discuss the key autistic neuropathologies and several of the main theories of autism and re-interpret them in the light of the hypothesized Intense World Syndrome.
Emotional and behavioural problems in children with autism spectrum disorder
,DOI:10.1007/s10803-012-1622-9 URL PMID:22895777 [本文引用: 1]
The type, frequency and inter-relationships of emotional and behavioural problems in 863 children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) were investigated using the population-based Database of children with ASD living in the North East of England (Dasl(n)e). A high rate of problems was reported, with 53 % of children having 4 or more types of problems frequently. Sleep, toileting and eating problems, hyperactivity, self injury and sensory difficulties were greater in children with lower language level and in special schooling. However, anxiety, tantrums and aggression towards others were frequent regardless of age, ability or schooling. The frequency of co-existing conditions, including such emotional and behavioural problems, in children with ASD has implications for designing appropriate support services for children and families.
Gaze behavior of children with asd toward pictures of facial expressions
,
Mechanisms of anxiety related attentional biases in children with autism spectrum disorder
,DOI:10.1007/s10803-015-2500-z URL PMID:26070278 [本文引用: 7]
Children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have high levels of anxiety. It is unclear whether they exhibit threat-related attentional biases commensurate with anxiety disorders as manifest in non-ASD populations, such as facilitated attention toward, and difficulties disengaging engaging from, threatening stimuli. Ninety children, 45 cognitively able with ASD and 45 age, perceptual-IQ, and gender matched typically developing children, aged 7-12 years, were administered a visual dot probe task using threatening facial pictures. Parent-reported anxiety symptoms were also collected. Children with ASD showed similarly high levels of anxiety compared with normative data from an anxiety disordered sample. Children with ASD had higher levels of parent-reported anxiety but did not show differences in disengaging from, or facilitated attention toward, threatening facial stimuli compared with typically developing children. In contrast to previously published studies of anxious children, in this study there were no differences in attentional biases in children with ASD meeting clinical cutoff for anxiety and those who did not. There were no correlations between attentional biases and anxiety symptoms and no gender differences. These findings indicate the cognitive mechanisms underlying anxiety in cognitively able children with ASD could differ from those commonly found in anxious children which may have implications for both understanding the aetiology of anxiety in ASD and for anxiety interventions.
Exploring factors related to the anger superiority effect in children with autism spectrum disorder
,DOI:10.1016/j.bandc.2016.05.004 URL PMID:27258410 [本文引用: 3]
Despite face and emotion recognition deficits, individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) appear to experience the anger superiority effect, where an angry face in a crowd is detected faster than a neutral face. This study extended past research to examine the impacts of ecologically valid photographic stimuli, gender and anxiety symptoms on the anger superiority effect in children with and without ASD. Participants were 81, 7-12year old children, 42 with ASD matched on age, gender and perceptual IQ to 39 typically developing (TYP) children. The photographic stimuli did not impact on task performance in ASD with both groups exhibiting the anger superiority effect. There were no gender differences and no associations with anxiety. Age was associated with the effect in the TYP but not ASD group. These findings confirm a robust effect of speeded detection of threat in ASD which does not appear to be confounded by gender or anxiety, but may have different underlying age-associated mechanisms.
A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety
,DOI:10.1016/s0005-7967(98)00063-1 URL PMID:9701859 [本文引用: 1]
Evidence of preattentive and attentional biases in anxiety is evaluated from a cognitive-motivational perspective. According to this analysis, vulnerability to anxiety stems mainly from a lower threshold for appraising threat, rather than a bias in the direction of attention deployment. Thus, relatively innocuous stimuli are evaluated as having higher subjective threat value by high than low trait anxious individuals, and it is further assumed that everyone orients to stimuli that are judged to be significantly threatening. This account is contrasted with other recent cognitive models of anxiety, and implications for the etiology, maintenance and treatment of anxiety disorders are discussed.
Neural circuitry of emotional face processing in autism spectrum disorders
,DOI:10.1503/jpn.090085 URL PMID:20184808 [本文引用: 3]
BACKGROUND: Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are associated with severe impairments in social functioning. Because faces provide nonverbal cues that support social interactions, many studies of ASD have examined neural structures that process faces, including the amygdala, ventromedial prefrontal cortex and superior and middle temporal gyri. However, increases or decreases in activation are often contingent on the cognitive task. Specifically, the cognitive domain of attention influences group differences in brain activation. We investigated brain function abnormalities in participants with ASD using a task that monitored attention bias to emotional faces. METHODS: Twenty-four participants (12 with ASD, 12 controls) completed a functional magnetic resonance imaging study while performing an attention cuing task with emotional (happy, sad, angry) and neutral faces. RESULTS: In response to emotional faces, those in the ASD group showed greater right amygdala activation than those in the control group. A preliminary psychophysiological connectivity analysis showed that ASD participants had stronger positive right amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex coupling and weaker positive right amygdala and temporal lobe coupling than controls. There were no group differences in the behavioural measure of attention bias to the emotional faces. LIMITATIONS: The small sample size may have affected our ability to detect additional group differences. CONCLUSION: When attention bias to emotional faces was equivalent between ASD and control groups, ASD was associated with greater amygdala activation. Preliminary analyses showed that ASD participants had stronger connectivity between the amygdala ventromedial prefrontal cortex (a network implicated in emotional modulation) and weaker connectivity between the amygdala and temporal lobe (a pathway involved in the identification of facial expressions, although areas of group differences were generally in a more anterior region of the temporal lobe than what is typically reported for emotional face processing). These alterations in connectivity are consistent with emotion and face processing disturbances in ASD.
A developmental neuroscience perspective on affect-biased attention
,DOI:10.1016/j.dcn.2016.08.001 URL PMID:27606972 [本文引用: 2]
There is growing interest regarding the impact of affect-biased attention on psychopathology. However, most of the research to date lacks a developmental approach. In the present review, we examine the role affect-biased attention plays in shaping socioemotional trajectories within a developmental neuroscience framework. We propose that affect-biased attention, particularly if stable and entrenched, acts as a developmental tether that helps sustain early socioemotional and behavioral profiles over time, placing some individuals on maladaptive developmental trajectories. Although most of the evidence is found in the anxiety literature, we suggest that these relations may operate across multiple domains of interest, including positive affect, externalizing behaviors, drug use, and eating behaviors. We also review the general mechanisms and neural correlates of affect-biased attention, as well as the current evidence for the co-development of attention and affect. Based on the reviewed literature, we propose a model that may help us better understand the nuances of affect-biased attention across development. The model may serve as a strong foundation for ongoing attempts to identify neurocognitive mechanisms and intervene with individuals at risk. Finally, we discuss open issues for future research that may help bridge existing gaps in the literature.
Attention biases towards and away from threat mark the relation between early dysregulated fear and the later emergence of social withdrawal
,DOI:10.1007/s10802-014-9963-9 URL PMID:25510354 [本文引用: 1]
Fearful temperament, mostly studied as behavioral inhibition (BI), has been extensively associated with social withdrawal in childhood and the later emergence of anxiety disorders, especially social anxiety disorder (SAD). Recent studies have characterized a distinct type of fearful temperament marked by high levels of fear in low threat situations - labeled dysregulated fear. Dysregulated fear has been related to SAD over and above risks associated with BI. However, the mechanism by which dysregulated fear is related to SAD has not been studied. Cognitive mechanisms, such as attentional bias towards threat, may be a possible conduit. We examined differences in attentional bias towards threat in six-year-olds who displayed a pattern of dysregulated fear at age two (N = 23) compared with children who did not display dysregulated fear (N = 33). Moreover, we examined the concurrent relation between attentional bias and social withdrawal. Results indicated that children characterized by dysregulated fear showed a significant bias away from threat, and that this bias was significantly different from the children without dysregulated fear, who showed no significant bias. Moreover, attentional bias towards threat was positively related to social withdrawal only for the dysregulated fear group. These results are discussed in consideration of the existing knowledge of attentional bias to threat in the developmental and pediatric anxiety literatures, as well as recent studies that find important heterogeneity in attentional bias.
Dynamic modulation of emotional processing
,DOI:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.05.010 URL [本文引用: 1]
Recent findings suggest the processing of emotional stimuli is prioritized compared to neutral stimuli; however, it is not necessarily automatic and depends on several modulating factors. The current paper highlights three major factors that affect the reactions to emotional stimuli: (i) stimulus properties, (ii) task demands and attention, and (iii) individual characteristics. The evidence reviewed here suggests that individual characteristics shape the structure, function and connectivity within a neural network that is involved in the reactions to emotional stimuli. This neural network includes regions related to emotion and attention, in line with evidence for reciprocal connections between these two processes. Activation in this network further depends on the emotional value of a certain item, as well as physical features of the stimulus. This integrative view can lead to better understanding of the underlying mechanisms of emotional reactions, as well as better therapeutic approaches. (c) 2012 Elsevier B.V.
Temperament and attention as core mechanisms in the early emergence of anxiety
,DOI:10.1159/000354350 URL PMID:26663953 [本文引用: 1]
Anxiety is a pervasive, impairing, and early appearing form of psychopathology. Even when anxiety remits, children remain at a two- to threefold increased risk for the later emergence of a mood disorder. Therefore, it is imperative to identify and examine underlying mechanisms that may shape early emerging patterns of behavior that are associated with anxiety. One of the strongest and first visible risk factors is childhood temperament. In particular, children who are behaviorally inhibited or temperamentally shy are more likely to exhibit signs of anxiety by adolescence. However, not all shy children do so, despite the early risk. We know that attention mechanisms, particularly the presence of attention biases toward or away from threat, can play a critical role in the emergence of anxiety. The current chapter will bring together these separate lines of research to examine the ways in which attention can modulate the documented link between early temperament and later anxiety. In doing so, the chapter will highlight multiple levels of analysis that focus on the behavioral, cognitive, and neural mechanisms in the temperament-attention-anxiety network. The chapter will help identify both markers and mechanisms of risk, supporting future work aimed at improving theory and intervention by focusing on attention biases to environmental threat.
Genetic and functional analyses demonstrate a role for abnormal glycinergic signaling in autism
,DOI:10.1038/mp.2015.139 URL PMID:26370147 [本文引用: 1]
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a common neurodevelopmental condition characterized by marked genetic heterogeneity. Recent studies of rare structural and sequence variants have identified hundreds of loci involved in ASD, but our knowledge of the overall genetic architecture and the underlying pathophysiological mechanisms remains incomplete. Glycine receptors (GlyRs) are ligand-gated chloride channels that mediate inhibitory neurotransmission in the adult nervous system but exert an excitatory action in immature neurons. GlyRs containing the alpha2 subunit are highly expressed in the embryonic brain, where they promote cortical interneuron migration and the generation of excitatory projection neurons. We previously identified a rare microdeletion of the X-linked gene GLRA2, encoding the GlyR alpha2 subunit, in a boy with autism. The microdeletion removes the terminal exons of the gene (GLRA2(Deltaex8-9)). Here, we sequenced 400 males with ASD and identified one de novo missense mutation, p.R153Q, absent from controls. In vitro functional analysis demonstrated that the GLRA2(Deltaex8)(-)(9) protein failed to localize to the cell membrane, while the R153Q mutation impaired surface expression and markedly reduced sensitivity to glycine. Very recently, an additional de novo missense mutation (p.N136S) was reported in a boy with ASD, and we show that this mutation also reduced cell-surface expression and glycine sensitivity. Targeted glra2 knockdown in zebrafish induced severe axon-branching defects, rescued by injection of wild type but not GLRA2(Deltaex8-9) or R153Q transcripts, providing further evidence for their loss-of-function effect. Glra2 knockout mice exhibited deficits in object recognition memory and impaired long-term potentiation in the prefrontal cortex. Taken together, these results implicate GLRA2 in non-syndromic ASD, unveil a novel role for GLRA2 in synaptic plasticity and learning and memory, and link altered glycinergic signaling to social and cognitive impairments.
Behavioral and emotional problems in high-functioning girls and boys with autism spectrum disorders: Parents' reports and adolescents' self-reports
,DOI:10.1177/1362361316675119 URL PMID:27899716 [本文引用: 1]
The purpose of this study was to investigate sex differences in behavioral and emotional problems in high-functioning girls and boys with autism spectrum disorder. The results obtained by adolescents with autism spectrum disorder were compared with those of typically developing girls and boys. Correlations between parents' and adolescents' ratings were also analyzed. Participants were 35 girls and 35 boys with autism spectrum disorder, aged 11-18 years, matched for chronological age and full-scale IQ. The control group consisted of 24 typically developing girls and 24 boys of the same age and IQ. The parents of adolescent participants were also included in the study. The measures used were the Child Behavior Checklist (4-18) completed by parents and Youth Self-Report (11-18) completed by adolescents. The adolescents with autism spectrum disorder presented higher levels of behavioral and emotional problems than the control group, according to both the parents' reports and the adolescents' self-reports. No sex differences were found in that respect. More differences between the assessments of adolescents and their parents occurred in the control group, and the effect size was larger.
The attention system of the human brain
,DOI:10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325 URL [本文引用: 1]
Dose-dependent social-cognitive effects of intranasal oxytocin delivered with novel breath powered device in adults with autism spectrum disorder: A randomized placebo-controlled double-blind crossover trial
,DOI:10.1038/tp.2017.103 URL PMID:28534875 [本文引用: 1]
The neuropeptide oxytocin has shown promise as a treatment for symptoms of autism spectrum disorders (ASD). However, clinical research progress has been hampered by a poor understanding of oxytocin's dose-response and sub-optimal intranasal delivery methods. We examined two doses of oxytocin delivered using a novel Breath Powered intranasal delivery device designed to improve direct nose-to-brain activity in a double-blind, crossover, randomized, placebo-controlled trial. In a randomized sequence of single-dose sessions, 17 male adults with ASD received 8 international units (IU) oxytocin, 24IU oxytocin or placebo followed by four social-cognitive tasks. We observed an omnibus main effect of treatment on the primary outcome measure of overt emotion salience as measured by emotional ratings of faces (eta(2)=0.18). Compared to placebo, 8IU treatment increased overt emotion salience (P=0.02, d=0.63). There was no statistically significant increase after 24IU treatment (P=0.12, d=0.4). The effects after 8IU oxytocin were observed despite no significant increase in peripheral blood plasma oxytocin concentrations. We found no significant effects for reading the mind in the eyes task performance or secondary outcome social-cognitive tasks (emotional dot probe and face-morphing). To our knowledge, this is the first trial to assess the dose-dependent effects of a single oxytocin administration in autism, with results indicating that a low dose of oxytocin can significantly modulate overt emotion salience despite minimal systemic exposure.
Effects of age and anxiety on processing threat cues in healthy children
,
More than just another face in the crowd: Evidence for an angry superiority effect in children with and without autism
,
Varieties of disgust faces and the structure of disgust
,DOI:10.1037//0022-3514.66.5.870 URL PMID:8014832 [本文引用: 1]
In 3 facial expression identification studies, college students matched a variety of disgust faces to verbally described eliciting situations. The faces depicted specific muscle action movements in accordance with P. Ekman and W. V. Friesen's (1978) Facial Action Coding System. The nose wrinkle is associated with either irritating or offensive smells and, to some extent, bad tastes. Gape and tongue extrusion are associated primarily with what we call core or food-offense disgust and also oral irritation. The broader range of disgust elicitors, including stimuli that remind humans of their animal origins (e.g., body boundary violations, inappropriate sex, poor hygiene, and death), a variety of aversive interpersonal contacts, and certain moral offenses are associated primarily with the raised upper lip. The results support a theory of disgust that posits its origin as a response to bad tastes and maps its evolution onto a moral emotion.
Inherited and De Novo genetic risk for autism impacts shared networks
,DOI:10.1016/j.cell.2019.07.015 URL PMID:31398340 [本文引用: 1]
We performed a comprehensive assessment of rare inherited variation in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) by analyzing whole-genome sequences of 2,308 individuals from families with multiple affected children. We implicate 69 genes in ASD risk, including 24 passing genome-wide Bonferroni correction and 16 new ASD risk genes, most supported by rare inherited variants, a substantial extension of previous findings. Biological pathways enriched for genes harboring inherited variants represent cytoskeletal organization and ion transport, which are distinct from pathways implicated in previous studies. Nevertheless, the de novo and inherited genes contribute to a common protein-protein interaction network. We also identified structural variants (SVs) affecting non-coding regions, implicating recurrent deletions in the promoters of DLG2 and NR3C2. Loss of nr3c2 function in zebrafish disrupts sleep and social function, overlapping with human ASD-related phenotypes. These data support the utility of studying multiplex families in ASD and are available through the Hartwell Autism Research and Technology portal.
Impairments to visual disengagement in autism spectrum disorder: A review of experimental studies from infancy to adulthood
,DOI:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.011 URL PMID:25454358 [本文引用: 1]
Impairments in visual disengagement are a current focus of research in autism spectrum disorders (ASD) and may play a key role in the early expression of social-emotional deficits associated with the disorder. This review summarizes current knowledge of visual disengagement and orienting in ASD. Convergent reports from infancy to adulthood indicate that (1) impairments to visual disengagement are apparent on Gap-Overlap tasks, spatial orienting tasks, and tasks involving social stimuli; and (2) these impairments emerge in the first year of life and continue into adulthood. The relationships between visual disengagement, orienting, joint attention, emotional regulation, and IQ are discussed in relation to ASD.
Brief report: Reduced prioritization of facial threat in adults with Autism
,
Impaired detection of happy facial expressions in autism
,DOI:10.1038/s41598-017-11900-y URL PMID:29042592 [本文引用: 1]
The detection of emotional facial expressions plays an indispensable role in social interaction. Psychological studies have shown that typically developing (TD) individuals more rapidly detect emotional expressions than neutral expressions. However, it remains unclear whether individuals with autistic phenotypes, such as autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and high levels of autistic traits (ATs), are impaired in this ability. We examined this by comparing TD and ASD individuals in Experiment 1 and individuals with low and high ATs in Experiment 2 using the visual search paradigm. Participants detected normal facial expressions of anger and happiness and their anti-expressions within crowds of neutral expressions. In Experiment 1, reaction times were shorter for normal angry expressions than for anti-expressions in both TD and ASD groups. This was also the case for normal happy expressions vs. anti-expressions in the TD group but not in the ASD group. Similarly, in Experiment 2, the detection of normal vs. anti-expressions was faster for angry expressions in both groups and for happy expressions in the low, but not high, ATs group. These results suggest that the detection of happy facial expressions is impaired in individuals with ASD and high ATs, which may contribute to their difficulty in creating and maintaining affiliative social relationships.
Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention
,DOI:10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.127 URL [本文引用: 1]
The reliability of attentional biases for emotional images measured using a free-viewing eye-tracking paradigm
,DOI:10.3758/s13428-018-1147-z URL PMID:30350023 [本文引用: 1]
Cognitive theories of anxiety disorders and depression posit that attentional biases play a role in the development, maintenance, and recurrence of these disorders. Several paradigms have been used to examine attentional biases in anxiety and depression, but information on the reliability of different attentional bias indices is limited. In this study we examined the internal consistency and 6-month test-retest reliability of attentional bias indices derived from a free-viewing eye-tracking paradigm. Participants completed two versions of an eye-tracking task-one that used naturalistic images as stimuli, and one that used face images. In both tasks, participants viewed displays of four images, each display consisting of one threat image, one sad image, one positive/happy image, and one neutral image. The internal consistency of the fixation indices (dwell time and number of fixations) for threat, sad, and positive images over the full 8-s display was moderate to excellent. When the 8-s display was divided into 2-s intervals, the dwell times for the 0- to 2-s and 2- to 4-s intervals showed lower reliability, particularly for the face images. The attentional bias indices for the naturalistic images showed adequate to good stability over the test-retest period, whereas the test-retest reliability estimates for the face images were in the low to moderate range. The implications of these results for attentional bias research are discussed.
Dissociable roles of prelimbic and infralimbic cortices, ventral hippocampus, and basolateral amygdala in the expression and extinction of conditioned fear
.DOI:10.1038/npp.2010.184 URL PMID:20962768 [本文引用: 1]
Current models of conditioned fear expression and extinction involve the basolateral amygdala (BLA), ventral medial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), and the hippocampus (HPC). There is some disagreement with respect to the specific roles of these structures, perhaps due to subregional differences within each area. For example, growing evidence suggests that infralimbic (IL) and prelimbic (PL) subregions of vmPFC have opposite influences on fear expression. Moreover, it is the ventral HPC (vHPC), rather than the dorsal HPC, that projects to vmPFC and BLA. To help determine regional specificity, we used small doses of the GABA(A) agonist muscimol to selectively inactivate IL, PL, BLA, or vHPC in an auditory fear conditioning and extinction paradigm. Infusions were performed prior to extinction training, allowing us to assess the effects on both fear expression and subsequent extinction memory. Inactivation of IL had no effect on fear expression, but impaired the within-session acquisition of extinction as well as extinction memory. In contrast, inactivation of PL impaired fear expression, but had no effect on extinction memory. Inactivation of the BLA or vHPC impaired both fear expression and extinction memory. Post-extinction inactivations had no effect in any structure. We suggest a model in which amygdala-dependent fear expression is modulated by inputs from PL and vHPC, whereas extinction memory requires extinction-induced plasticity in IL, BLA, and/or vHPC.
Reliability of two versions of the dot-probe task using photographic faces
,
Top-down and bottom-up factors in threat-related perception and attention in anxiety
,DOI:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.08.006 URL PMID:27546616 [本文引用: 1]
Anxiety is characterized by the anticipation of aversive future events. The importance of prestimulus anticipatory factors, such as goals and expectations, is well-established in both visual perception and attention. Nevertheless, the prioritized perception of threatening stimuli in anxiety has been attributed to the automatic processing of these stimuli and the role of prestimulus factors has been neglected. The present review will focus on the role of top-down processes that occur before stimulus onset in the perceptual and attentional prioritization of threatening stimuli in anxiety. We will review both the cognitive and neuroscience literature, showing how top-down factors, and interactions between top-down and bottom-up factors may contribute to biased perception of threatening stimuli in normal function and anxiety. The shift in focus from stimulus-driven to endogenous factors and interactions between top-down and bottom-up factors in the prioritization of threat-related stimuli represents an important conceptual advance. In addition, it may yield important clues into the development and maintenance of anxiety, as well as inform novel treatments for anxiety.
Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals
,DOI:10.1038/nrn2889 URL PMID:20811475 [本文引用: 1]
Many emotional stimuli are processed without being consciously perceived. Recent evidence indicates that subcortical structures have a substantial role in this processing. These structures are part of a phylogenetically ancient pathway that has specific functional properties and that interacts with cortical processes. There is now increasing evidence that non-consciously perceived emotional stimuli induce distinct neurophysiological changes and influence behaviour towards the consciously perceived world. Understanding the neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals will clarify the phylogenetic continuity of emotion systems across species and the integration of cortical and subcortical activity in the human brain.
Event-related potentials of attentional bias toward faces in the dot-probe task: A systematic review
,DOI:10.1111/psyp.13051 URL PMID:29266532 [本文引用: 2]
The dot-probe task is a common task to assess attentional bias toward different stimuli and how groups differ (e.g., attentional bias in anxiety disorders). However, measuring reaction time has been suggested to be unreliable. Neuroimaging methods such as fMRI were shown to be more reliable in assessing attentional bias, but fMRI has poor temporal resolution and therefore cannot assess timing of attention. ERPs have been used to examine the time course of attentional bias. Although ERP research may be more reliable than reaction time, there have been inconsistencies in the literature. This review systematically searched for articles that used the dot-probe task with facial expressions and measured neural correlates with ERP. We found that some of the inconsistencies might be the cause of methodological differences (e.g., timing of stimuli), differences in emotional expression, and/or sample differences (e.g., sex, age, etc.). Suggestions on how future research could address the issues presented in this review were discussed.
Elevated amygdala response to faces and gaze aversion in autism spectrum disorder
,DOI:10.1093/scan/nst050 URL PMID:23596190 [本文引用: 1]
Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are often associated with impairments in judgment of facial expressions. This impairment is often accompanied by diminished eye contact and atypical amygdala responses to face stimuli. The current study used a within-subjects design to examine the effects of natural viewing and an experimental eye-gaze manipulation on amygdala responses to faces. Individuals with ASD showed less gaze toward the eye region of faces relative to a control group. Among individuals with ASD, reduced eye gaze was associated with higher threat ratings of neutral faces. Amygdala signal was elevated in the ASD group relative to controls. This elevated response was further potentiated by experimentally manipulating gaze to the eye region. Potentiation by the gaze manipulation was largest for those individuals who exhibited the least amount of naturally occurring gaze toward the eye region and was associated with their subjective threat ratings. Effects were largest for neutral faces, highlighting the importance of examining neutral faces in the pathophysiology of autism and questioning their use as control stimuli with this population. Overall, our findings provide support for the notion that gaze direction modulates affective response to faces in ASD.
Adults with autism and adults with depression show similar attentional biases to social-affective images
,
Dynamic fearful gaze does not enhance attention orienting in individuals with Asperger's disorder
,DOI:10.1016/j.bandc.2009.08.015 URL PMID:19781841 [本文引用: 2]
Although impaired joint attention is one of the core clinical features of pervasive developmental disorder including autistic disorder and Asperger's disorder, experimental studies failed to report its impairment. This discrepancy might be the result of differences between real-life and experimental situations. The present study examined joint attention in 11 individuals with Asperger's disorder and 11 age-matched controls under naturalistic conditions using a target detection paradigm with dynamic emotional gaze cues. Although both groups showed gaze-triggered attention orienting as assessed by the differences in reaction time for invalid minus valid cues, enhancement of joint attention by fearful (vs. neutral) gaze was observed in the control, but not in the Asperger group. This suggests that the integration of emotion and gaze direction that elicits strong joint attention is impaired in individuals with Asperger's disorder.
Testosterone reduces unconscious fear but not consciously experienced anxiety: Implications for the disorders of fear and anxiety
,DOI:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.003 URL PMID:15939408 [本文引用: 1]
BACKGROUND: The fear-reducing properties of testosterone have been firmly established in animals but not in humans. However, human data on the relation between testosterone, fear, and anxiety have predominantly involved questionnaires that index cortically executed conscious appraisal of anxious mood. Animal studies, on the other hand, indicate that the effects of testosterone on motivation and emotion are of subcortical origin and of unconscious nature. Presently, it was hypothesized that a single testosterone administration to humans would reduce unconscious fear but not consciously experienced anxiety. METHODS: In a placebo-controlled, double-blind crossover design, a single dose of testosterone (.5 mg) or placebo was administered to 16 healthy female volunteers. Afterward, a masked emotional Stroop task measured unconscious emotional responses to fearful faces, while multiple self-reports of mood indexed consciously experienced anxiety. RESULTS: As hypothesized, the habitual vigilant emotional response to the masked fearful face observed in the placebo condition was significantly reduced after testosterone was administered, while the self-reported measures of anxiety remained unaffected. CONCLUSIONS: These data provide the first direct evidence for fear-reducing properties of testosterone in humans. Furthermore, by dissociating specific aspects of fear and anxiety in humans, this outcome highlights that testosterone's effects on motivation and emotion concern the subcortical affective pathways of the brain.
The dot-probe task to measure emotional attention: A suitable measure in comparative studies?
,DOI:10.3758/s13423-016-1224-1 URL PMID:28092078 [本文引用: 1]
For social animals, attending to and recognizing the emotional expressions of other individuals is of crucial importance for their survival and likely has a deep evolutionary origin. Gaining insight into how emotional expressions evolved as adaptations over the course of evolution can be achieved by making direct cross-species comparisons. To that extent, experimental paradigms that are suitable for investigating emotional processing across species need to be developed and evaluated. The emotional dot-probe task, which measures attention allocation toward emotional stimuli, has this potential. The task is implicit, and subjects need minimal training to perform the task successfully. Findings in nonhuman primates, although scarce, show that they, like humans, have an attentional bias toward emotional stimuli. However, the wide literature on human studies has shown that different factors can have important moderating effects on the results. Due to the large heterogeneity of this literature, these moderating effects often remain unnoticed. We here review this literature and show that subject characteristics and differences in experimental designs affect the results of the dot-probe task. We conclude with specific recommendations regarding these issues that are particularly relevant to take into consideration when applying this paradigm to study animals.
The serotonin transporter gene polymorphism and the effect of baseline on amygdala response to emotional faces
,DOI:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.013 URL PMID:21167188 [本文引用: 1]
Previous research has found that a common polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR) is an important mediator of individual differences in brain responses associated with emotional behaviour. In particular, relative to individuals homozygous for the l-allele, carriers of the s-allele display heightened amygdala activation to emotional compared to non-emotional stimuli. However, there is some debate as to whether this difference is driven by increased activation to emotional stimuli, resting baseline differences between the groups, or decreased activation to neutral stimuli. We performed functional imaging during an implicit facial expression processing task in which participants viewed angry, sad and neutral faces. In addition to neutral faces, we included two further baseline conditions, houses and fixation. We found increased amygdala activation in s-allele carriers relative to l-homozygotes in response to angry faces compared to neutral faces, houses and fixation. When comparing neutral faces to houses or fixation, we found no significant difference in amygdala response between the two groups. In addition, there was no significant difference between the groups in response to fixation when compared with a houses baseline. Overall, these results suggest that the increased amygdala response observed in s-allele carriers to emotional faces is primarily driven by an increased response to emotional faces rather than a decreased response to neutral faces or an increased resting baseline. The results are discussed in relation to the tonic and phasic hypotheses of 5-HTTLPR-mediated modulation of amygdala activity.
Measuring attentional bias to threat: Reliability of dot probe and eye movement indices
,
Trait anxiety, state anxiety, and attentional bias to threat: Assessing the psychometric properties of response time measures
,
Eye-tracking, autonomic, and electrophysiological correlates of emotional face processing in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder
,DOI:10.1007/s10803-012-1565-1 URL PMID:22684525 [本文引用: 2]
Individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) often have difficulty with social-emotional cues. This study examined the neural, behavioral, and autonomic correlates of emotional face processing in adolescents with ASD and typical development (TD) using eye-tracking and event-related potentials (ERPs) across two different paradigms. Scanning of faces was similar across groups in the first task, but the second task found that face-sensitive ERPs varied with emotional expressions only in TD. Further, ASD showed enhanced neural responding to non-social stimuli. In TD only, attention to eyes during eye-tracking related to faster face-sensitive ERPs in a separate task; in ASD, a significant positive association was found between autonomic activity and attention to mouths. Overall, ASD showed an atypical pattern of emotional face processing, with reduced neural differentiation between emotions and a reduced relationship between gaze behavior and neural processing of faces.
Fear of negative evaluation influences eye gaze in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: A pilot study
,DOI:10.1007/s10803-014-2349-6 URL PMID:25578337 [本文引用: 2]
Social anxiety is common among adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). In this modest-sized pilot study, we examined the relationship between social worries and gaze patterns to static social stimuli in adolescents with ASD (n = 15) and gender-matched adolescents without ASD (control; n = 18). Among cognitively unimpaired adolescents with ASD, self-reported fear of negative evaluation predicted greater gaze duration to social threat cues (i.e., faces depicting disgust and anger). By comparison, there was no relationship between self-reported social fears and gaze duration in the controls. These findings call attention to the potential import of the impact of co-occurring psychopathology such as social anxiety, and particularly fear of negative evaluation, on social attention and cognition with adolescents who have ASD.
No evidence of atypical attentional disengagement in autism: A study across the spectrum
,DOI:10.1177/1362361318768025 URL PMID:29663829 [本文引用: 1]
The ability to disengage attention and reengage elsewhere has been proposed as a fundamental deficit in the autism spectrum, potentially disrupting development of higher cognitive domains. Eye-movements were recorded while 16 autism spectrum children of mixed ability, and 18 typically developing age-matched controls, completed the Gap-Overlap paradigm. A significant difference in latency to fixate target was found between Gap and Overlap conditions. A significant interaction with group was due to autism spectrum participants' shorter latencies to fixate target in the Gap condition, but similar group responses in the Overlap condition. Considerable within-group variability emerged. We predicted that attentional disengaging would be related to specific features of the phenotype; however, there was no evidence of an association with receptive language, non-verbal IQ, sensory behaviors, or autistic severity in autism spectrum or typically developing groups. In conclusion, while atypical visual attention mechanisms may be a feature of autism spectrum, this is not explained by impaired visual disengaging but is more likely due to increased susceptibility of visual fixation offset cueing. Despite best efforts, nine additional autism spectrum children could not complete testing, and data from a further six were unusable; more work is needed to develop research methods that enable individuals across the spectrum to participate.
Neurological diseases with autism spectrum disorder: Role of ASD risk genes
,
Oxytocin enhances attentional bias for neutral and positive expression faces in individuals with higher autistic traits
,DOI:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.09.002 URL PMID:26372768 [本文引用: 1]
There is considerable interest in the potential therapeutic role of the neuropeptide oxytocin in altering attentional bias towards emotional social stimuli in psychiatric disorders. However, it is still unclear whether oxytocin primarily influences attention towards positive or negative valence social stimuli. Here in a double-blind, placebo controlled, between subject design experiment in 60 healthy male subjects we have used the highly sensitive dual-target rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) paradigm to investigate whether intranasal oxytocin (40IU) treatment alters attentional bias for emotional faces. Results show that oxytocin improved recognition accuracy of neutral and happy expression faces presented in the second target position (T2) during the period of reduced attentional capacity following prior presentation of a first neutral face target (T1), but had no effect on recognition of negative expression faces (angry, fearful, sad). Oxytocin also had no effect on recognition of non-social stimuli (digits) in this task. Recognition accuracy for neutral faces at T2 was negatively associated with autism spectrum quotient (ASQ) scores in the placebo group, and oxytocin's facilitatory effects were restricted to a sub-group of subjects with higher ASQ scores. Our results therefore indicate that oxytocin primarily enhances the allocation of attentional resources towards faces expressing neutral or positive emotion and does not influence that towards negative emotion ones or non-social stimuli. This effect of oxytocin is strongest in healthy individuals with higher autistic trait scores, thereby providing further support for its potential therapeutic use in autism spectrum disorder.
Impaired recognition of negative facial expressions is partly related to facial perception deficits in adolescents with high- functioning autism spectrum disorder
,
The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information
,
The amygdala and the relevance detection theory of autism: An evolutionary perspective
,DOI:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00894 URL PMID:24416006 [本文引用: 2]
In the last few decades there has been increasing interest in the role of the amygdala in psychiatric disorders and, in particular, in its contribution to the socio-emotional impairments in autism spectrum disorders (ASDs). Given that the amygdala is a component structure of the
Attentional biases to faces expressing disgust in children with autism spectrum disorders: an exploratory study
,DOI:10.1038/srep19381 URL PMID:26758779 [本文引用: 4]
Previous studies on attentional bias towards emotional faces in individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) provided mixed results. This might be due to differences in the examined attentional bias components and emotional expressions. This study assessed three bias components, hypervigilance, disengagement, and avoidance, using faces with a disgust, happy, or neutral expression in a dot-probe and external cuing task in 18 children with ASD and 21 typically developing (TD) children. The children with ASD initially displayed hypervigilance towards the disgust faces, followed by a general tendency to avoid looking back at the spatial location at which any face, irrespective of its emotional expression, had been presented. These results highlight the importance of differentiating between attentional bias components in research on ASD.
/
〈 | 〉 |