ISSN 1671-3710
CN 11-4766/R
主办:中国科学院心理研究所
出版:科学出版社

心理科学进展, 2018, 26(9): 1689-1699 doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.01689

研究前沿

赌博游戏中的near-miss效应

索涛,1, 杨雁2, 范增1, 王梦梦1, 赵国祥,1

1河南大学教育科学学院, 心理与行为研究所, 认知、脑与健康研究所

2河南大学经济学院, 开封 475004

The near-miss effect in gambling games

SUO Tao,1, YANG Yan2, FAN Zeng1, WANG Mengmeng1, ZHAO Guoxiang,1

1 Institute of Psychology and Behavior, Institute of cognition, Brain and Health, College of Education and Science, Henan University

2 School of Economics, Henan University, Kaifeng 475004, China

通讯作者: * 赵国祥, E-mail:zhaogx@henu.edu.cn** 索涛, E-mail:suotao810815@163.com

收稿日期: 2017-09-22   网络出版日期: 2018-09-15

基金资助: 国家自然科学基金青年项目(31400908)
中国博士后科学基金面上项目(2014M551995)
河南省哲学社科规划项目(2015BJY007)
河南省高等学校重点科研项目(16A190001)

Received: 2017-09-22   Online: 2018-09-15

Fund supported: (31400908)
(2014M551995)
(2015BJY007)
(16A190001)

摘要

near-miss效应是指在赌博中, 与一般的输钱和赢钱相比, “几乎赢(near-miss)”的输钱会诱发个体更高的生理唤醒和更强的赌博动机, 从而导致个体持续赌博的一种现象, 是导致赌博成瘾的主要诱因之一。针对这种现象的研究范式大致有三种:老虎机/类老虎机任务、轮盘任务和刮刮乐彩票任务。这种现象的理论解释目前主要有认知曲解假说、控制幻觉理论和受挫假说。near-miss效应的脑机制和病理研究才刚刚起步, 所涉及到的脑功能区域主要包括脑岛、腹侧纹状体等。未来的研究应在near-miss效应发生机制的理论模型建构、研究范式多样化、研究技术多模态化、病理机制和临床干预等方面进一步展开。

关键词: near-miss效应; 认知曲解假说; 控制幻觉理论; 受挫假说; 脑岛; 腹侧纹状体

Abstract

The near-miss effect refers to a phenomenon that near-miss losses can elicit individuals’ higher physiological arousal and stronger gambling motivation than full-miss losses and winnings, which could lead to gambling persistence of gamblers. Researches on the near-miss effect have important theoretical value for understanding the cognitive and neural mechanisms of dynamic outcome evaluation in decision-making processes and shedding light on the mechanisms of pathological gambling. Studies have found convergent evidence that near-miss losses have motivationally enhancing properties. However, the cognitive and neural mechanisms of the near-miss effect are still under debate. In addition, the role that the near-miss effect played in pathological gambling is still unclear. At present, there are three main theoretical explanations for the near-miss effect: the cognitive misrepresentation hypothesis, the illusion of control theory, and the frustration hypothesis. The neural correlates of the near-miss effect mainly involve the insula and ventral striatum. Future researches should further explore the theoretical models of the near-miss effect, improve the experimental paradigm, various research methods, and examine the effect in pathological populations.

Keywords: near-miss effect; the cognitive misrepresentation hypothesis; the illusion of control theory; the frustration hypothesis; insula; ventral striatum

PDF (1068KB) 元数据 多维度评价 相关文章 导出 EndNote| Ris| Bibtex  收藏本文

本文引用格式

索涛, 杨雁, 范增, 王梦梦, 赵国祥. 赌博游戏中的near-miss效应 . 心理科学进展, 2018, 26(9): 1689-1699 doi:10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.01689

SUO Tao, YANG Yan, FAN Zeng, WANG Mengmeng, ZHAO Guoxiang. The near-miss effect in gambling games. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(9): 1689-1699 doi:10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.01689

1 引言

赌博游戏是人们日常生活中普遍流行的一类娱乐性风险决策活动, 但部分个体过度沉溺于这类风险游戏, 导致消极的病态赌博行为, 对个人、家庭甚至社会产生极其负面的影响, 比如, 个体身心紊乱, 生活质量低下; 家庭支离破碎, 债台高筑; 社会风气败坏, 违法犯罪率剧增(Goudriaan, Oosterlaan, de Beurs, & Van den Brink et al., 2004; Hodgins, Stea, & Grant, 2011; van Holst, van den Brink, Veltman, & Goudriaan., 2010)。许多不同类型的赌博研究一致发现一种能诱发赌徒长时持续下赌注进而导致成瘾的现象:near-miss效应(Reid, 1986; Barton et al., 2017; Côté, Caron, Aubert, Desrochers, & Ladouceur, 2003)。near-miss效应是指在赌博过程中, 与一般的输钱和赢钱相比, “差点赢(near-miss)”的输钱结果会诱发个体更高的生理唤醒和更强行为动机, 从而导致个体持续赌博游戏行为的一种现象(Barton et al., 2017; Billieux, Van der Linden, Khazaal, Zullino, & Clark, 2012; Chase & Clark 2010; Clark, Crooks, Clarke, Aitken, & Dunn, 2012; Clark, Lawrence, Astley-Jones, & Gray, 2009; Griffiths,1991; Larche, Musielak, & Dixon, 2017; Reid, 1986)。这种动机效应在反馈结果评价时诱发个体产生持久赌博动机和行为中发挥着重要的作用, 是病态赌博和游戏成瘾的主要诱因之一(Barton et al., 2017)。最近几年, 这种效应已成为国外赌博研究领域关注的一个热点, 但国内关于这方面的研究寥寥无几。研究这种现象对于进一步探讨动态结果评价的认知过程和神经机制具有重要的理论价值, 对于理清病理性赌博成瘾的发生机制和探讨赌博成瘾的诊疗途径具有一定的实践意义。本文首先阐述了near-miss效应研究的常见范式, 接着简述了研究者们对这一现象的不同理论解释, 然后概述了关于这种现象的脑功能成像和脑电研究, 最后基于这几个方面, 针对当前研究存在的不足和问题, 对未来的研究方向进行了展望。

2 near-miss效应的研究范式

研究赌博中near-miss效应最常用的实验范式主要有三种:老虎机/类老虎机任务、轮盘任务和刮刮乐彩票任务。

老虎机任务(the slot machine task, SMT)/类老虎机任务(the liking-SMT)。老虎机也叫吃角子机或单臂强盗(one-arm bandit), 是一种在赌场和娱乐场所最常见的赌博机器, 因最早筹码上有老虎图案而得名。最早的商业老虎机由美国的查理·费(Charlie Fey)于1895年发明, 由内部的三个转轴(里面有不同的图案)、一个投币槽和外部控制机器转动的手柄组成。老虎机的基本玩法是把硬币或代币投入投币槽后拉动手柄或启动旋转按钮, 转轴转动, 每个转轴里的图案开始随机变化, 当机器停止转动时如果机器界面出现特定的图标组合(比如三个转轴在赔付线上图标相同)就会赢钱, 否则输掉赌注, 由于操作简便且能以小博大, 很快风靡全球, 成为许多酒吧、赌场、游戏厅, 甚至零售店的娱乐赌具。

早期赌博研究采用的研究任务大多是让被试在真实的老虎机上进行赌博, 然后考察被试对不同反馈结果的行为反应和生理反应(Dixon & Schreiber, 2004; Reid, 1986; Harrigan, 2009)。但由于真实老虎机任务存在诸多不可控因素, 且研究数据记录不便, 因此随后大多数研究采用了电脑可视化的三转轴老虎机任务(Belisle & Dixon 2016; Dymond et al., 2014; Habib, & Dixon, 2010; MacLin, Dixon, Daugherty, & Small, 2007; Sharman, Aitken, & Clark, 2015; Shao, Read, Behrens, & Rogers, 2013; Worhunsky, Malison, Rogers, & Potenza, 2014)。在这个任务中, 当三个转轴停止转动时, 在前两个转轴中停在赔付线(pay-line)上的图符一样, 而在第三个转轴中这个图符刚刚停在紧挨着赔付线上方或下方时(见图1A)就是near-miss结果。后来一些研究(Clark et al., 2009; Alicart, Cucurell, Masherrero, & Marcopallarés, 2015; Sescousse et al., 2016)把老虎机任务进一步简化为双转轴模式(两个转轴和一个赔付线), 首先由参与者在左转轴的众多图符中选中一个停在赔付线上, 然后按按钮后右转轴快速转动一段时间后随机停下, 当右转轴中停在赔付线上的图符与左转轴中选中的图符相同时参与者赢钱, 反之输钱。当右转轴停下时紧挨赔付线上一个或下一个图符与左转轴选中的图符相同时的输钱就是near-miss结果(见图1B)。

图1

图1   常见的研究near-miss效应的实验任务(A图源于Dixon et al., 2011; B图源于Clark et al., 2009; C图源于Wu, van Dijk, Li, Aitken, & Clark, 2017; D图源于Stange, Graydon, & Dixon, 2016)


轮盘任务。轮盘(Rapid Roulette)也是赌场常见的一种赌具。轮盘由转轮和赌注图案两部分组成, 式样有两种:一种只有一个赌注图案, 轮盘设于一端; 另一种是转轮在中间和两边各设一图案。庄家宣布开赌后, 参与者开始下赌注, 下注的位置由自己选择。转轮向逆时针方向转动, 然后庄家把一个小球放在微凸的轮盘面上以顺时针方向旋动, 在这个过程中, 参与者可不断下注, 待小球转速下降, 落入轮盘上任何两个金属间隔之间, 每个间隔上面标注的颜色数字就是赌徒赢钱的号码。早期的赌博研究都是采用这种真实的赌博轮盘任务。为了更方便操控自变量和克服研究中额外变量的干扰, 后来的轮盘赌博研究(Jessup, & O'Doherty, 2011; Ulrich, & Hewig, 2014; Wu et al., 2017)大多都采用了简化版的幸运轮任务(wheel- of-fortune task) (见图1C), 整个轮盘仅有一个分成不同区域的轮盘面和一个指针组成。简化的幸运轮盘任务有两种类型:一种是先固定指针在任意位置, 然后让轮盘快速转起来一段时间, 当轮盘停下来时, 指针指向的数字号码就是参与者输赢钱的数量; 另一种是轮盘固定不动, 让指针快速转动一段时间, 然后指针慢慢停下来, 指针指向的数字号码就是参与者输赢钱的数量。在这个任务中, near-miss结果则是指那些指针指向十分接近赢钱(尤其是大赢)的位置但没有赢钱时的结果。

刮刮乐彩票任务。还有一部分博彩研究(Griffiths, 1995; Lole, Gonsalvez, Barry, & De Blasio, 2013; Stange, Graydon, & Dixon, 2016, 2017; Stange, Grau, Osazuwa, Graydon, & Dixon, 2017)采用了模拟刮刮乐彩票(Scratch card games)的任务(见图1D), 每次向参与者呈现若干个(一般3~7个)并排的隐藏符号(比如, 动物图形、水果图形、字母等), 参与者先选择不同的赌注, 然后依次呈现或同时呈现这些符号, 当所有符号都相同时能赢最多的钱就是full-win结果; 当只有一个符号和其余符号不同时, 只能赢很少部分钱或未赢钱或输钱的结果就是near-miss结果; 当两个以上符号和其余符号不同时未能赢钱就是full-loss结果。

3 near-miss效应的理论解释

近30年来的研究发现, near-miss效应是病态赌博产生的主要诱因之一。但关于这种效应的发生机制, 也即near-miss结果如何导致个体产生更强的持续赌博动机, 研究者提出了不同的理论解释。

3.1 认知曲解假说(the cognitive misrepresentation hypothesis, CMH)

Griffiths (1991)认为, near-miss结果更加接近于能赢钱的结果, 使个体增强赢钱预期, 进一步提高个体的唤醒水平, 导致认知曲解(cognitive misrepresentation), 使得个体误以为这种输钱结果更接近于赢钱而不是输钱, 时间久之, 这种表面类似赢钱的结果会强化个体进一步误以为他们不是经常输钱而是经常几乎要赢钱。Dixon和Schreiber (2004)的一项研究中让被试评定near-miss结果更类似赢钱还是输钱, 结果表明near-miss更类似于赢钱(也见Habib, & Dixon 2010)。同时, 一些动物研究也一致发现, 同能产生奖赏的结果一样, near-miss结果也能增强老鼠的奖赏预期, 增强老鼠持续赌博的动机。Peters, Hunt和Harper (2010)为老鼠设计了一个与老虎机类似的程序, 只要老鼠使转轮上的图标匹配就能得到食物奖赏, 他们发现老鼠也会把near-miss结果当作一种食物奖赏的强化物。他们认为, 老鼠能把类老虎机上意味着赢得食物奖赏的符号与食物相联系而成为获取食物的条件强化物, 同样near-miss结果由于与能获取食物的结果在视觉上具有相似性, 因此导致老鼠也把near-miss结果作为食物获取的条件强化物。这也与Winstanley, Cocker和Rogers (2011)的研究结果一致, 他们让老鼠通过操控灯泡亮熄数量来诱发不同性质的结果。研究发现, near-miss结果能引起多巴胺水平的提高, 增强老鼠的奖赏预期, 促使老鼠持续按键反应(Winstanley et al., 2011)。

3.2 控制幻觉理论(the illusion of control theory, ICT)

Clark等(2009)认为, 如果认知曲解假说解释恰当, near-miss输钱结果应该像赢钱一样使个体感到高兴而不像一般输钱那样使个体感到不高兴。于是, 在他们的研究中, 让被试对老虎机赌博中遇到的不同类型结果(如win、full-loss、near-miss loss)进行情绪评定, 结果发现一般的赢钱被评价为比较高兴, 但near-miss输钱结果被评价为不高兴, 甚至比一般的输钱更不高兴(也见Qi, Ding, Song, & Yang, 2011; 索涛, 冯廷勇, 贾世伟, 李红, 2009)。因此, Clark等(2009)反对认知曲解假说, 提出控制幻觉理论来解释这种现象。他们认为near-miss结果的物理属性会引起控制幻觉, 能影响随后的赌博决策是因为这些结果的出现被赌徒错误地解释为赌博技能提升的信号, 因而产生了赌博中的控制幻觉。他们认为, 在真实技能游戏中, 尽管缺乏相关强化物, 但near-miss结果能传递技能提高的信息, 例如, 在篮球比赛中, 投3分球时能砸中球框所表现出的个体技能远远比其投空球的技能要高; 但在像彩票抽奖或老虎机这样的背景下, 赢钱由几率决定, 此时这个逻辑就不成立, 但是一些赌徒还是错误地认定near-miss结果的出现意味着他们赌博技能的提高, 随后更有可能赢大钱(也见Clark, Liu, McKavanagh, Garrett, Dunn, Aitken, 2013)。Billieux等(2012)也发现, 技能倾向的赌博认知会导致near-miss结果增强继续赌博的动机。他们推测, near-miss结果可能通过传递技能提高的信息, 进而增强继续赌博动机。

3.3 受挫假说(the frustration hypothesis, FH)

Clark等(2009)发现, 尽管near-miss结果诱发了不高兴的情绪, 甚至比一般的输钱更强烈, 但near-miss结果激活的脑区与赢钱激活的脑区相重叠。针对Clark等(2009)研究中行为结果和脑功能激活结果存在的矛盾, 大量研究(Dixon et al., 2011; Dixon et al., 2013; Larche et., 2017)发现, 输钱会使人产生挫折感, near-miss输钱使被试产生挫折感的强度比一般输钱更强, 且相应的各种生理反应更强。因此, 也有人试图提出一种受挫假说来解释这种现象(Dixon et al., 2011; Dixon, MacLaren, Jarick, Fugelsang, & Harrigan, 2013)。这种解释指出, 相比一般输钱, near-miss输钱结果使个体产生更复杂的受挫情绪, 这种情绪状态更会激发个体通过进一步下注赢钱捞本以挽回当前尴尬的局面(Stange, Graydon, Dixon, 2016, 2017; Stange, Grau, Osazuwa, Graydon, & Dixon, 2017)。最近一些生理研究证实了这一看法(Dixon et al., 2011; Dixon et al., 2013)。

纵观以上三种理论解释, 最终可归结为结果评价理论解释的两大争论:强化学习理论(the reinforcement-learning theory, Holroyd & Coles, 2002)和情感动机假说(the motivational/affective hypothesis, Gehring & Willoughby, 2002 )。前两者解释都认为near-miss输钱通过增强奖赏预期提升个体持续赌博动机, 它们都属于强化学习理论的范畴, 但二者的具体分歧在于:认知曲解假说把near-miss输钱结果直接看作和赢钱一样的奖赏强化物, 从而增强个体的奖赏预期; 而控制幻觉理论则认为near-miss输钱能引起自我技能的提升, 进而增强个体的奖赏预期。但受挫假说则认为near-miss输钱会导致令人不快的复杂挫折情绪, 为了尽快改变这种不快的状态, 继而激发个体持续赌博的动机, 这属于情感动机假说的范畴。

值得注意的是, 最近一些研究(Clark et al., 2012; Liu et al., 2007; Liu, Hairston, Schrier, & Fan, 2011; Qi et al., 2011; Sharman, & Clark, 2016; Wu et al., 2017; 索涛等, 2009)也认为反事实思维和后悔情绪(Byrne, 2016; Epstude, & Roese, 2008; Roese, 1997; Roese, & Epstude, 2017)在near-miss效应产生中可能起着重要的影响作用, 相比一般的输钱, near-miss输钱诱发的反事实思维和后悔情绪增强个体的控制幻觉, 从而产生更强烈的奖赏预期, 进而引起持续赌博的强烈动机和行为。

4 near-miss效应的脑神经机制研究

4.1 功能脑成像研究

由于near-miss结果的特殊性(感觉上非常接近赢钱但实质上输钱), 因此在决策结果反馈加工阶段可能会引起更加复杂的脑活动。最近, 脑功能成像(fMRI)研究采用不同的实验范式考察了赌博任务中near-miss效应涉及到的脑功能区, 这些研究主要涉及到包括腹侧纹状体(ventral striatum, VS)、前脑岛(anterior insula, AI)、前额叶(prefrontal cortex,包括内侧前额叶、眶额叶)、前扣带回(anterior cingulated cortex, ACC)、额下回(inferior frontal gyrus, IFG)、杏仁核(amygdala)、顶下叶(inferior parietal lobule, IPL)、下丘脑(bilateral inferior thalamic)在内的从大脑前额到顶部以及旁边缘系统等多巴胺奖赏系统功能区(Barton et al., 2017; Clark et al., 2009; Clark et al., 2014; Dymond et al., 2014; Habib & Dixon, 2010; Liu et al., 2011; Sescousse et al., 2016; Shao et al., 2013; van Holst, Chase, & Clark, 2014; Worhunsky et al., 2014)。尽管这些不同实验范式的fMRI研究并没有得出完全一致的结论, 但这些研究涉及到的脑区域最多的是腹侧纹状体和脑岛。

腹侧纹状体由伏隔核(nucleus accumbens, NAcc)和嗅结节(olfactory tubercle, OT)组成, 是基底核的一部分。作为多巴胺神经奖赏系统的重要组成部分, 在奖赏信息(大小、效价、凸显性、意义等)加工和学习、动机产生和决策中发挥着多重作用(Liu et al., 2011; Murty, Stanek, & Heusser, 2013; Tremblay, Worbe, & Hollerman, 2009; Zaehle et al., 2013; Zeighami, & Moustafa, 2015)。near-miss效应的fMRI研究发现near-miss输钱和赢钱一样激活了腹侧纹状体(Clark et al., 2009; Chase, & Clark, 2010; Sescousse et al., 2016; Shao et al., 2013; van Holst et al., 2014), 但near-miss输钱激活的腹侧纹状体活动没有赢钱激活的强烈(Clark et al., 2009; Shao et al., 2013)。而且Chase和Clark (2010)还发现, near-miss结果激活的腹侧纹状体活动强度能被赌徒的赌瘾严重程度所预测, 赌瘾越严重, 腹侧纹状体活动越强烈; 同时他们也发现, 与健康控制组相比, 问题赌徒的赢钱和near-miss结果诱发的腹侧纹状体反应活动更加强烈(也参见Sescousse et al., 2016; van Holst et al., 2014)。因此, 他们认为, 赌徒错误地把赌博中near-miss结果感知为更加接近赢钱, 进而增强了奖赏系统中多巴胺的传输, 进一步导致赌博紊乱加重。

脑岛是唯一完全隐藏在脑组织内的高级脑叶。脑岛由额下回后部、颞叶和顶叶形成的岛盖(Operculum)环绕覆盖, 隐藏于侧裂内, 以外侧裂为中心, 大体上呈倒三角锥形。它通过上纵束、钩状束、额枕束及前联合与额叶、颞叶、顶叶、枕叶以及包括前扣带皮层、腹内侧前额叶皮层、杏仁核和腹侧纹状体等在内的其他边缘系统相联(李健, 戴西件, 廖婷, 龚洪翰, 2015; Namkung, Kim, & Sawa, 2017)。脑岛大致可分为前脑岛与后脑岛, 前脑岛主要接收和整合来自前扣带皮层, 腹内侧前额叶皮层, 杏仁核和腹侧纹状体等与之相连的边缘区域的认知、情绪和动机方面的信息; 后部主要接收顶叶、枕叶和颞叶皮层输入的前庭系统和视觉运动等信息, 同时还通过丘脑接收来自脊髓和脑干的上行躯体内脏感觉信息(Namkung et al., 2017)。大量的脑结构和功能神经成像研究表明, 脑岛是多种信息整合的共同核心体, 在内感性、自我意识、共情、公平感、道德认知加工、反事实思维、情绪体验、动机、各类成瘾、决策和推理加工等诸多心理过程中发挥着重要的作用(Downar, Blumberger, & Daskalakis., 2016; Goodkind, et al., 2015; Namkung et al., 2017)。早期的near-miss效应的fMRI研究发现, 与一般输钱(full-loss)相比, near-miss输钱也明显激活了前脑岛, 同时near-miss诱发的前脑岛活动强度与被试的赌博相关认知评分(the gambling-related cognitions scale, GRCS)以及near-miss结果诱发其持续赌博的动机强度都呈正相关(Clark et al., 2009; Chase, & Clark, 2010), 这表明个体的赌博错误认知越严重, 持续赌博的动机越强烈, 前脑岛的激活就越强烈。van Holst等(2014)研究也表明, near-miss结果诱发的双侧脑岛活动强度与问题赌博的严重性密切相关。Sescousse等(2016)也发现, 无论是正常控制组还是病理性赌徒, 与full-miss结果相比, near-miss结果在双侧脑岛激活更强的BOLD信号。还有, Dymond等(2014)不但发现near-miss结果和赢钱一样激活脑岛和右侧额下回, 同时与full-loss结果相比, near-miss结果明显激活前额叶、右脑岛、额下回、额中回和双侧下丘脑等。Dymond等(2014)还发现, near-miss结果在脑岛和右侧眶额叶皮层(OFC)激发的θ波段(4~7 Hz)震荡与赌博成瘾的严重程度呈正相关。θ波段增强可能反映了对反馈结果与相关任务的主动认知与情感加工(Christie & Tata, 2009; Doñamayor, Marco- Pallarés, Heldmann, Schoenfeld, & Münte., 2011)。另外, Naqvi, Rudrauf, Damasio, 和 Bechara (2007)发现, 与大脑其他部位受损的病人相比, 脑岛受损的病人能很快停止吸烟, 不会对吸烟长期渴求。动物研究(Contreras, Ceric, & Torrealba, 2007)也表明, 脑岛钝化能使有安非他明(苯异丙胺)体验的老鼠降低对安非他明的渴求行为。 由此推断, 脑岛在near-miss效应产生中的作用很可能与它在调节个体对毒品(Naqvi & Bechara, 2009)、食物(Tang, Fellows, Small, & Dagher, 2012)以及其它成瘾(Clark & Limbrick- Oldfield, 2013; Hommer, Bjork, & Gilman., 2011)等的主观渴求中的作用一致。最值得注意的是, Clark等(2014)进一步对比了脑岛、腹内侧前额叶(ventromedial prefrontal cortex, VMPFC)和杏仁核受损的三组病人对near-miss结果的行为反应, 结果发现脑岛受损病人的near-miss效应消失, 而其他两组病人的near-miss效应表现正常。这直接证实了脑岛在near-miss效应的产生中发挥着关键的作用。

另外, Habib和Dixon (2010)的研究发现, 相对一般输钱, near-miss只激活了顶下叶(inferior parietal lobule, IPL); Worhunsky等(2014)发现near-miss结果增强了枕叶(occipital lobule)、后扣带回(posterior cingulate cortex, PCC)、顶下叶(inferior parietal lobule, IPL)、顶上叶(superior parietal lobule, SPL)的活动。

总之, 考虑到near-miss结果诱发的生理唤醒(Clark et al., 2012; Dixon, MacLaren, Jarick, Fugelsang, & Harrigan., 2013)和前脑岛的内感性以及在情绪、认知和动机方面的整合功能(Namkung et al., 2017), 由此我们推断, 脑岛在near-miss效应中很可能通过整合来自腹侧纹状体的奖赏加工(Liu et al., 2011)、前扣带回的预期冲突(Clark et al., 2009; Holroyd et al., 2004)、眶额叶的反事实思维加工和情绪体验(Dymond et al., 2014; Chua, Gonzalez, Taylor, Welsh, & Liberzon, 2009; Hoeck et al., 2013; Levens et al., 2014)等多个脑区域的信息, 进而产生明确的外显性动机和强烈的欲望, 促使个体持续赌博行为。

4.2 脑电研究

由于事件相关电位(ERP)技术在高时间分辨率上的优势,一些研究(Alicart et al., 2015; Dymond et al., 2014; Lole et al., 2013; Lole, Gonsalvez, & Barry, 2015; Luo, Wang, & Qu, 2011; Qi et al., 2011; Ulrich, & Hewig, 2014; 索涛等, 2009)也利用ERP技术探讨了near-miss效应的电生理时间进程。尽管这些研究得出的结论并不完全一致, 部分甚至相反, 但这些研究都涉及到决策评价中常见的两个特异性脑电成分:反馈负波(feedback-related negativity, FRN)和P300 (Gehring & Willoughby, 2002; Hajcak, Moser, Holroyd, & Simons, 2007; Holroyd, & Coles, 2002; Nieuwenhuis, Holroyd, Mol, & Coles, 2004; Nieuwenhuis, Aston-Jones, & Cohen, 2005)。

FRN是由反馈结果呈现后250~300 ms内诱发的一个特定负波, 其在大脑头皮前中部波幅最大。源定位分析表明, FRN的发生源位于ACC或内侧前额叶(medial frontal cortex, MFC) (Gehring, & Willoughby, 2002; Holroyd & Coles, 2002)。对FRN的理论解释具有代表性的是强化学习理论(the reinforcement-learning theory)和情感动机假说(the motivational/affective hypothesis)。强化学习理论(Holroyd & Coles, 2002)认为, FRN是中脑多巴胺信号对ACC产生影响的结果, 不利的预期错误信号(比如, 行为结果比预期的不好)引起多巴胺系统相位活动减弱, 这些信息传输到ACC就产生了较大的FRN。Wu和Zhou (2009)进一步认为, “预期错误”不仅包括结果效价(例如, 输赢、正误)上的预期, 而且也应包括那些结果是否符合事先确定的且无效价的预期, 且预期偏离越大, 产生的FRN越大。情感动机假说(Gehring & Willoughby, 2002)认为, FRN本身并不反映行为评价或预期错误监测的认知加工, 而是反映了对不利结果引起的动机情感的评估加工。由于在赌博游戏中参与者的最终意图是为了赢更多的钱, 所以当输钱结果出现时意味着预期偏离, 因此相比赢钱会诱发更大的FRN, 这个结果已经得到大量研究一致证实(Nieuwenhuis et al., 2004)。由于near-miss输钱结果的特殊性, 尽管实质上输钱, 但个体总会把它错误地知觉为非常接近赢钱, 根据强化学习理论, near-miss结果诱发的FRN波幅应该介于赢钱和完全输钱(full-miss)之间。Luo等(2011)Lole等(2013)的研究(Lole et al., 2013; Luo et al., 2011)证实了这一推论。但另一些研究(Kreussel, Hewig, Kretschmer, Hecht, Coles, & Miltner, 2013; Ulrich, & Hewig, 2014)却发现, near-miss输钱诱发的FRN波幅反而比full-miss输钱和赢钱诱发的都大, 这似乎和动机情感假说一致, near-miss输钱引起更大的情感动机, 进而表现出来FRN波幅更大。还有一些研究(Alicart et al., 2015; Qi et al., 2011; 索涛等, 2009)报告near-miss输钱结果与full-miss输钱结果诱发的FRN并没有明显差异。这些研究结果用强化学习理论和情感动机假说都无法解释。

P300是反馈结果呈现后300~600 ms出现的一种多源性正波, 是大脑多个皮层联合活动的结果, 在注意、记忆、情绪和决策等研究领域备受关注(Polich, 2012; Nieuwenhuiset al., 2005)。在决策结果评价中P300很可能反映了个体对反馈刺激功能意义的评价 (Nieuwenhuis et al., 2005; Wu & Zhou, 2009)。Yeung和Sanfey (2004)认为P300是多重评价体系的产物, 既可能反映认知评价也可能反映结果评价中诱发的情绪加工。索涛等(2009)最早在一项类老虎机任务中发现了near-miss结果的P300效应, near-miss输钱诱发的P300波幅明显比full-loss输钱诱发的小。随后Ulrich和Hewig (2014)的研究证实了这一结果。但Qi等(2011)Alicart等(2015)的研究却发现, 赢钱诱发的P300波幅最大, near-miss输钱诱发的次之, full-loss输钱诱发的最小。另外, Luo等(2011)Lole等(2013, 2015)的研究仅仅发现赢钱诱发的P300波幅最大, 但near-miss输钱和full-loss输钱诱发的P300波幅没有明显差异。

值得注意的是, 最近也有研究探讨了near-miss效应的EEG频谱震荡(Alicart et al., 2015; Dymond et al., 2014), 尤其θ波段(4~8 Hz)与near-miss效应密切相关。Alicart等(2015)发现, 与赢钱一样, near-miss输钱诱发的θ波段(4~8 Hz)、α波段(9~13 Hz)、低β波段(15~22 Hz)和β-γ波段(25~35 Hz)的震荡都比full-loss输钱结果诱发的大。Dymond等(2014)的脑磁图(MEG)研究也发现, near-miss输钱结果在脑岛和右侧眶额叶皮层激发的θ波段(4~7 Hz)振荡增强。而且, 这些θ波段(4~7 Hz)震荡与赌博成瘾的严重程度呈正相关。这些发现表明near-miss效应的产生和脑岛和眶额叶皮层的θ波振荡密切相关。根据以往研究(Bernat, Malone, Williams, Patrick, & Iacono, 2007; Cavanagh & Frank, 2014), θ波振荡与奖赏加工、选择性注意、行为调节、认知控制、工作记忆和学习等认知加工密切相关。因此, 能体现预期偏离的凸显性结果也会引起θ波振荡(Bunzeck, Guitart-Masip, Dolan, Düzel, 2011; Cavanagh, Frank, Klein, & Allen, 2010; Mas- Herrero & Marco-Pallarés, 2014), near-miss结果的凸显性属性必将引起个体的选择性注意和情感动机, 进而对随后的行为和策略进行调整和改变(Schultz, 2006)。

5 未来研究的展望

尽管近年来一些学者从行为反应、神经生理和脑成像层面对near-miss效应进行了初步研究, 基于以上的国内外研究动态分析, 我们发现关于这种效应的研究在以下几个方面仍需要进一步开展:

(1) near-miss效应发生机制的理论模型建构。尽管不少研究一致发现near-miss结果能增强个体的生理唤醒水平和进一步不理性赌博的动机和行为, 对这种动机效应的发生机制提出了不同的看法(Griffiths, 1991; Clark et al., 2009; Dixon et al., 2011; Dixon et al., 2013; Reid, 1986), 但至今并未达成一致, 且这些研究并未从动态结果评价的角度对near-miss结果评价的认知过程进行详细的探讨和阐述。最近一些研究强调了反事实思维在near-miss效应产生中的作用(Sharman, & Clark, 2016; Wu et al., 2017)。反事实思维的研究(Roese, 1997; Roese, & Epstude, 2017)表明, 实际结果接近预期结果的程度会明显影响反事实思维和决策后情绪(如后悔), 比如, 与迟到1个小时相比, 迟到5分钟而未赶上航班会诱发更强的反事实思维和后悔情绪。另外, 反事实思维和后悔情绪会诱发个体产生尽快获得机会重新行动的动机, 以至于摆脱当前不利的局面(Roese, 1997)。比如, 考试规定60分及格, 59分会使考生产生若能再给一次补考机会就能通过考试的强烈动机, 这和赌博中near-miss结果诱发的继续赌博以便捞本的动机非常类似。还有研究(Roese, & Olson, 1995)发现,行为责任感和可控感的提高会明显增强反事实思维的强度。另外, 行为结果对个体的意义也会调节结果评价的认知过程(Schultz, 2006)。因此, 我们推测反事实思维在这种动机效应的产生中可能起着关键作用, 且伴随的后悔情绪、个体行为责任感和控制感以及赌博结果的意义等内、外因素在调控这种效应强度中也可能起着重要的影响作用。基于以上分析, 我们构建了near-miss效应发生机制的理论模型, 如图2。为了验证这个理论模型的恰当性, 以后的研究可结合这个模型对涉及到的这些认知影响因素逐一加以探讨。

图2

图2   near-miss效应的产生机制设想图


(2)实验研究范式需进一步整合和发展。以前研究多采用两轮或多轮老虎机和类老虎机任务(见图1), 在这类任务中, 参与者先从多个赌注图标中选出一个, 随后若干组轮子开始旋转, 当各组轮子都停止时其上面的图标都和选中的一样时才能赢钱, 否则输钱。这样的任务可能会引入混淆变量:赌注图标的选取会使参与者在持续赌博中产生赌徒谬误或热手谬误, 影响随后的行为动机, 进而会和near-miss诱发的动机效应相混淆。同时, 这些任务在操作行为控制感方面也存在一定的局限性。另外, 最近有研究也发现一些手机游戏(比如, 糖果传奇)中也存在near-miss效应, 导致个体沉溺于手机游戏不可自拔(Larche et al., 2017)。因此, 随后的研究应从不同的角度设计多种能诱发near-miss结果的博彩游戏任务, 比如, 从结果的时间、空间、数字的接近性等多角度设计实验任务。这样, 可以使用不同实验范式对同一现象进行研究, 以便互相验证所得结果的可靠性。

(3)利用多模态脑成像技术进一步探讨near-miss效应的发生脑神经网络机制。基于前述, 尽管大量的研究采用fMRI、ERP等脑功能成像技术研究了这种现象的脑神经机制, 但这些研究得出的结果并不完全一致, 甚至相反。例如, 一些研究发现脑岛和腹侧纹状体在near-miss效应产生中发挥着重要的作用, 但也有研究发现眶额叶、扣带回、额下回和顶叶等诸多脑区也涉入其中。这些脑区如何关联和交互作用, 至今也并不十分清楚。因此, 未来的研究需要结合像fMRI、MEG等多种脑功能成像技术对near-miss效应的脑神经网络机制进行深入的拓展与探讨。另外, 脑电方面的研究仍需要进一步发展。例如, Suo等(2009)和Qi等(2011)的研究仅发现near-miss效应的P300效应(也见Alicart et al., 2015; Ulrich, & Hewig, 2014), 但Luo等(2011)Lole等(2013)的研究却仅发现near-miss效应的FRN效应。未来的脑电研究应采用高级的EEG频谱分析技术对比和探讨不同实验范式下EEG信号差异及其原因。

(4) near-miss效应的病理和临床研究进一步深入化。对near-miss效应脑神经机制及其在赌博成瘾中作用的研究可以发掘用于诊断病态赌博成瘾的行为指标、电生理指标以及脑功能信号, 同时可以为病态赌博成瘾的干预与矫正提供新的途径。尽管已有fMRI研究探讨了病态赌博群体和正常群体在near-miss效应上的行为与脑神经活动差异(Chase, & Clark, 2010; Sescousse et al., 2016; van Holst et al., 2014), 但关于near-miss效应的病理成瘾的临床研究才刚刚起步, near-miss效应与赌博成瘾之间的关系还不甚清楚。因此, 利用ERP、fMRI等多模态技术探讨病态赌博成瘾中near-miss效应的病理机制仍是今后研究的一个重要方向。还有, Nastally和Dixon (2012)发现, 临床干预能明显减弱病理性赌徒的near-miss效应, 病理性赌徒接受正念训练(mindfulness exercises)后自我报告near-miss结果接近赢钱的程度明显降低, near-miss效应明显减弱, 赌博成瘾的程度明显缓解。因此, 以研究near-miss效应的病理机制和干预方法为契机, 探究各类行为和生理刺激方法对病理性赌瘾的临床干预也是未来研究非常值得关注的一个方向。

参考文献

李健, 戴西件, 廖婷, 龚洪翰 . ( 2015).

岛叶解剖及功能区域分化和整合的研究进展

实用放射学杂志,31( 6), 1026-1028

DOI:10.3969/j.issn.1002-1671.2015.06.037      URL     [本文引用: 1]

岛叶是人大脑的第五叶,属于旁边缘系统的一部分,是唯一隐藏在脑组织内的高级脑叶。因其位置深在,周边血 管密集,岛叶肿瘤病变的外科手术比较困难。岛叶具有多种功能,包括社会情绪、感觉运动、认知、嗅味觉及不同功能的整合等。然而,岛叶不同亚区代表的功能不 尽相同,国内外对其功能的研究也较少,相关功能机制也一直在研究与探索之中。因此,全面了解岛叶的解剖结构及功能显得至关重要。本文综合国内外相关研究, 就此作一综述。

索涛, 冯廷勇, 贾世伟, 李红 . ( 2009).

决策失利后情绪的接近性效应与ERP证据

中国科学C辑: 生命科学, 39( 6), 611-620.

[本文引用: 5]

Alicart H., Cucurell D., Mas-Herrero E., & Marco-Pallaré J. ( 2015).

Human oscillatory activity in near-miss events

Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10( 10), 1405-1412.

DOI:10.1093/scan/nsv033      URL     PMID:25809401      [本文引用: 7]

Near-miss events are situations in which an action yields a negative result but is very close to being successful. They are known to influence behavior, especially in gambling scenarios. Previous neuroimaging studies have described an 'anomalous' activity of brain reward areas following these events. The goal of the present research was to study electrophysiological correlates of near-misses in the expectation and outcome phases. Electroencephalography was recorded while participants were playing a simplified version of a slot machine. Four possible outcomes (gain, near-miss, loss and no-information) were presented in a pseudorandom order to ensure fixed proportions. Results from the time-frequency analysis for the theta (4-8 Hz), alpha (9-13 Hz), low beta (15-22 Hz) and beta-gamma (25-35 Hz) frequency-bands presented larger power increases for wins and near-misses compared with losses. In the anticipation phase, power changes were lower than in the resolution phase. The current results are in agreement with previous studies showing that near-miss events recruit brain areas of the reward network. Likewise, the oscillatory activity in near-misses is very similar to the one elicited in the gain condition. In addition, present findings suggest that oscillatory activity in the expectation phase does not play a crucial role in near-miss events.

Barton K. R., Yazdani Y., Ayer N., Kalvapalle S., Brown S., Stapleton J., .. Harrigan K. A. ( 2017).

The effect of losses disguised as wins and near misses in electronic gaming machines: A systematic review

Journal of Gambling Studies, 33( 4), 1241-1260.

[本文引用: 4]

Belisle, J., & Dixon, M. R. ( 2016).

Near misses in slot machine gambling developed through generalization of total wins

Journal of Gambling Studies, 32( 2), 689-706.

DOI:10.1007/s10899-015-9554-x      URL     PMID:26018845      [本文引用: 1]

The purpose of the present study was to evaluate the development of the near miss effect in slot machine gambling as a product of stimulus generalization from total wins. The study was conducted across two experiments. Twelve college students participated in the first experiment, which demonstrated that greater post-reinforcement pauses followed losing outcomes that were formally similar to total wins, relative to losing outcomes that were formally dissimilar [ F (5, 7)02=025.24, p 02=02.025] along a generalization gradient ( R 2 02=02.96). Additionally, 11 out of 12 participants showed greater response latencies following near-misses than following total wins. Thirteen college students participated in the second experiment, which demonstrated that symbols that more saliently indicated a loss resulted in lower response latencies than functionally equivalent but visually dissimilar losing symbols [ F (3, 10)02=0215.50, p 02=02.01]. A generalization gradient was observed across winning symbols ( R 2 02=02.98), and an inverse of the gradient observed across winning symbols was observed across symbols that were the least formally similar ( R 2 02=02.69). The present study replicates and extends previous research on near misses in slot machine gambling, and provides discussion around the clinical utility of such findings on the prevention of problem gambling.

Bernat E. M., Malone S. M., Williams W. J., Patrick C. J., & Iacono W. G. ( 2007).

Decomposing delta, theta, and alpha time-frequency ERP activity from a visual oddball task using PCA

. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 64( 1), 62-74.

DOI:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.07.015      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Billieux J., Van der Linden M., Khazaal Y., Zullino D., & Clark L. ( 2012).

Trait gambling cognitions predict near-miss experiences and persistence in laboratory slot machine gambling

British Journal of Psychology, 103( 3), 412-427.

[本文引用: 2]

Bunzeck N., Guitart-Masip M., Dolan R.J., & Düzel E. ( 2011).

Contextual novelty modulates the neural dynamics of reward anticipation

Journal of Neuroscience, 31( 36), 12816-12822.

DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0461-11.2011      URL     PMID:3192314      [本文引用: 1]

We investigated how rapidly the reward-predicting properties of visual cues are signaled in the human brain and the extent these reward prediction signals are contextually modifiable. In a magnetoencephalography study, we presented participants with fractal visual cues that predicted monetary rewards with different probabilities. These cues were presented in the temporal context of a preceding novel or familiar image of a natural scene. Starting at 100 ms after cue onset, reward probability was signaled in the event-related fields (ERFs) over temporo-occipital sensors and in the power of theta (5-8 Hz) and beta (20-30 Hz) band oscillations over frontal sensors. While theta decreased with reward probability beta power showed the opposite effect. Thus, in humans anticipatory reward responses are generated rapidly, within 100 ms after the onset of reward-predicting cues, which is similar to the timing established in non-human primates. Contextual novelty enhanced the reward anticipation responses in both ERFs and in beta oscillations starting at 100 ms after cue onset. This very early context effect is compatible with a physiological model that invokes the mediation of a hippocampal-VTA loop according to which novelty modulates neural response properties within the reward circuitry. We conclude that the neural processing of cues that predict future rewards is temporally highly efficient and contextually modifiable.

Byrne, R. M. J. ( 2016).

Counterfactual thought

Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 135-157.

[本文引用: 1]

Cavanagh, J. F., & Frank, M. J. ( 2014).

Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control

Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18( 8), 414-421.

DOI:10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.012      URL     PMID:4112145      [本文引用: 1]

Recent advancements in cognitive neuroscience have afforded a description of neural responses in terms of latent algorithmic operations. However, the adoption of this approach to human scalp electroencephalography (EEG) has been more limited, despite the ability of this methodology to quantify canonical neuronal processes. Here, we provide evidence that theta band activities over the midfrontal cortex appear to reflect a common computation used for realizing the need for cognitive control. Moreover, by virtue of inherent properties of field oscillations, these theta band processes may be used to communicate this need and subsequently implement such control across disparate brain regions. Thus, frontal theta is a compelling candidate mechanism by which emergent processes, such as ‘cognitive control’, may be biophysically realized.

Cavanagh J. F., Frank M. J., Klein T. J., & Allen, J. J. B. ( 2010).

Frontal theta links prediction errors to behavioral adaptation in reinforcement learning

Neuroimage, 49( 4), 3198-3209.

DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.080      URL     PMID:19969093      [本文引用: 1]

Investigations into action monitoring have consistently detailed a frontocentral voltage deflection in the event-related potential (ERP) following the presentation of negatively valenced feedback, sometimes termed the feedback-related negativity (FRN). The FRN has been proposed to reflect a neural response to prediction errors during reinforcement learning, yet the single-trial relationship between neural activity and the quanta of expectation violation remains untested. Although ERP methods are not well suited to single-trial analyses, the FRN has been associated with theta band oscillatory perturbations in the medial prefrontal cortex. Mediofrontal theta oscillations have been previously associated with expectation violation and behavioral adaptation and are well suited to single-trial analysis. Here, we recorded EEG activity during a probabilistic reinforcement learning task and fit the performance data to an abstract computational model (Q-learning) for calculation of single-trial reward prediction errors. Single-trial theta oscillatory activities following feedback were investigated within the context of expectation (prediction error) and adaptation (subsequent reaction time change). Results indicate that interactive medial and lateral frontal theta activities reflect the degree of negative and positive reward prediction error in the service of behavioral adaptation. These different brain areas use prediction error calculations for different behavioral adaptations, with medial frontal theta reflecting the utilization of prediction errors for reaction time slowing (specifically following errors), but lateral frontal theta reflecting prediction errors leading to working memory-related reaction time speeding for the correct choice.

Chase, H. W., & Clark, L. ( 2010).

Gambling severity predicts midbrain response to near-miss outcomes

Journal of Neuroscience, 30( 18), 6180-6187.

DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5758-09.2010      URL     PMID:20445043      [本文引用: 5]

Abstract Gambling is a common recreational activity that becomes dysfunctional in a subset of individuals, with DSM "pathological gambling" regarded as the most severe form. During gambling, players experience a range of cognitive distortions that promote an overestimation of the chances of winning. Near-miss outcomes are thought to fuel these distortions. We observed previously that near misses recruited overlapping circuitry to monetary wins in a study in healthy volunteers (Clark et al., 2009). The present study sought to extend these observations in regular gamblers and relate brain responses to an index of gambling severity. Twenty regular gamblers, who varied in their involvement from recreational playing to probable pathological gambling, were scanned while performing a simplified slot machine task that delivered occasional monetary wins, as well as near-miss and full-miss nonwin outcomes. In the overall group, near-miss outcomes were associated with a significant response in the ventral striatum, which was also recruited by monetary wins. Gambling severity, measured with the South Oaks Gambling Screen, predicted a greater response in the dopaminergic midbrain to near-miss outcomes. This effect survived controlling for clinical comorbidities that were present in the regular gamblers. Gambling severity did not predict win-related responses in the midbrain or elsewhere. These results demonstrate that near-miss events during gambling recruit reward-related brain circuitry in regular players. An association with gambling severity in the midbrain suggests that near-miss outcomes may enhance dopamine transmission in disordered gambling, which extends neurobiological similarities between pathological gambling and drug addiction.

Christie, G. J., & Tata, M. S. ( 2009).

Right frontal cortex generates reward-related theta-band oscillatory activity

NeuroImage, 48( 2), 415-422.

DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.076      URL     PMID:19591949      [本文引用: 1]

When participants in a gambling game are given feedback as to whether they won or lost the previous bet, a series of stereotypical brain electrical responses can be observed in the electroencephalogram (EEG) and the stimulus-locked Event-Related Potential (ERP). These include the Feedback-Related Mediofrontal Negativity (FRN), a posterior P300, and a feedback-induced increase in power at the theta (4 to 8 Hz) band over frontal scalp. Although the generators of the FRN and P300 have been studied previously, little is known about the generator of feedback-induced theta. We employed a gambling game in which participants chose either high-risk/high-reward or low-risk/low-reward bets to investigate these feedback-related responses. The FRN was not modulated by the riskiness of the bet, but both P300 and feedback-induced theta were of greater amplitude following high- relative to low-risk bets. Using a bilateral multi-source Beamformer approach, we localized the induced theta-band responses following wins and losses to partially overlapping regions in the right medial frontal cortex, possibly including the Anterior Cingulate. Using a dipole-fitting approach, we found that the generators of feedback-induced theta are anatomically distinct from those of the FRN and P300.

Chua H. F., Gonzalez R., Taylor S. F., Welsh R. C., & Liberzon I. ( 2009).

Decision-related loss: Regret and disappointment

Neuroimage, 47( 4), 2031-2040.

DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.006      URL     PMID:19524050      [本文引用: 1]

Both affective neuroscience and decision science focus on the role of emotions in decisions. Regret and disappointment are emotions experienced with negative decision outcomes. The present research examines the neural substrates of regret and disappointment as well as the role of regret and disappointment in decision making. Experiment 1 compared the subjective experience of regret and disappointment. Participants selected one of two gambles and received different types of feedback during the outcome phase. Despite identical nominal losses, regret induced a more intense dislike of the outcomes and a stronger desire to switch choices than disappointment. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, Experiment 2 examined the neural correlates of regret and disappointment. Both regret and disappointment activated anterior insula and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex relative to fixation, with greater activation in regret than in disappointment. In contrast to disappointment, regret also showed enhanced activation in the lateral orbitofrontal cortex. These findings suggest that regret and disappointment, emotions experienced during decision-related loss, share a general neural network but differ in both the magnitude of subjective feelings and with regret activating some regions with greater intensity.

Clark L., Crooks B., Clarke R., Aitken M. R., & Dunn B. D. ( 2012).

Physiological responses to near-miss outcomes and personal control during simulated gambling

Journal of Gambling Studies, 28( 1), 123-137.

DOI:10.1007/s10899-011-9247-z      URL     PMID:21516368      [本文引用: 3]

AbstractNear-miss outcomes during gambling are non-win outcomes that fall close to a pay-out. While objectively equivalent to an outright miss, near-misses motivate ongoing play and may therefore be implicated in the development of disordered gambling. Given naturalistic data showing increases in heart rate (HR) and electrodermal activity (EDA) during periods of real gambling play, we sought to explore the phasic impact of win, near-miss and full-miss outcomes on physiological arousal in a controlled laboratory environment. EDA and HR were monitored as healthy, student participants (n02=0233) played a simulated slot-machine task involving unpredictable monetary wins. A second gambling distortion, perceived personal control, was manipulated within the same task by allowing the participant to select the play icon on some trials, and having the computer automatically select the play icon on other trials. Near-misses were rated as less pleasant than full-misses. However, on trials that involved personal choice, near-misses produced higher ratings of ‘continue to play’ than full-misses. Winning outcomes were associated with phasic EDA responses that did not vary with personal choice. Compared to full-misses, near-miss outcomes also elicited an EDA increase, which was greater on personal choice trials. Near-misses were also associated with greater HR acceleration than other outcomes. Near-miss outcomes are capable of eliciting phasic changes in physiological arousal consistent with a state of subjective excitement, despite their objective non-win status.

Clark L., Lawrence A. J., Astley-Jones F., & Gray N. ( 2009).

Gambling near-misses enhance motivation to gamble and recruit win-related brain circuitry

Neuron, 61( 3), 481-490.

DOI:10.1016/j.neuron.2008.12.031      URL     PMID:19217383      [本文引用: 13]

“Near-miss” events, where unsuccessful outcomes are proximal to the jackpot, increase gambling propensity and may be associated with the addictiveness of gambling, but little is known about the neurocognitive mechanisms that underlie their potency. Using a simplified slot machine task, we measured behavioral and neural responses to gambling outcomes. Compared to “full-misses,” near-misses were experienced as less pleasant, but increased desire to play. This effect was restricted to trials where the subject had personal control over arranging their gamble. Near-miss outcomes recruited striatal and insula circuitry that also responded to monetary wins; in addition, near-miss-related activity in the rostral anterior cingulate cortex varied as a function of personal control. Insula activity to near-misses correlated with self-report ratings as well as a questionnaire measure of gambling propensity. These data indicate that near-misses invigorate gambling through the anomalous recruitment of reward circuitry, despite the objective lack of monetary reinforcement on these trials.

Clark, L., & Limbrick-Oldfield, E. H. ( 2013).

Disordered gambling: a behavioral addiction

Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23( 4), 655-659.

DOI:10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.004      URL     PMID:23375671      [本文引用: 1]

Developments in psychiatry have ratified the existence of behavioral addictions, that certain activities such as gambling or video-game play may be considered addictive in the absence of exogenous (i.e. drug-induced) stimulation of brain reinforcement circuitry. This article describes recent advances in understanding the neurobiological basis of behavioral addiction, with a focus on pathological gambling as the prototypical disorder. We describe positron emission tomography (PET) studies characterizing dopaminergic transmission, and functional imaging studies of reward processing and gambling-related cognitive distortions. The current evidence not only indicates changes in pathological gamblers in core circuitry implicated in drug addiction, but also highlights some subtle differences. Behavioral addictions can also provide experimental traction on distinguishing vulnerability markers for addictions from the active detrimental effects of chronic drug use.

Clark L., Liu R., McKavanagh R., Garrett A., Dunn B. D., & Aitken, M. R. F. ( 2013).

Learning and Affect Following Near‐Miss Outcomes in Simulated Gambling

Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 26( 5), 442-450.

DOI:10.1002/bdm.1774      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Gambling near-misses are non-rewarded events that resemble a winning configuration. Past research using slot machines has shown that moderate rates of near-misses increase gambling persistence, but the mechanisms supporting this persistence are unclear. One hypothesis is that near-misses are mistakenly interpreted as signals of skill acquisition, supporting learning and fuelling the ‘illusion of control’. A slot machine simulation was administered to 60 volunteers, with ratings of the perceived chances of winning, pleasure and motivation to play following particular outcomes. Psychophysiological measures (electrodermal activity and heart rate) were taken, and gambling persistence was measured after 30 trials. Near-misses were similar to full-miss outcomes in that they were regarded as unpleasant. However, near-misses were akin to win outcomes in that they increased motivations to play and electrodermal activity. Learning was evidenced by the expectancy of winning increasing following wins and decreasing after losses. Although there was no overall change in expectancy of winning after near-misses across all participants, those subjects reporting a greater increase in the expectancy of winning following a near-miss showed more persistent play, consistent with the learning hypothesis. Greater heart rate acceleration following near-misses was also associated with persistence. We also observed differential effects of near-misses where the reel stopped either side of the winning position (‘payline’): motivational effects were restricted to near-misses stopping before the payline, whereas near-misses that stopped after the payline were primarily aversive. The payline effects are not predicted by the learning hypothesis and may indicate an affective component to near-misses, possibly linked to counterfactual processing. Copyright 08 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Clark L., Studer B., Bruss J., Tranel D., & Bechara A. ( 2014).

Damage to insula abolishes cognitive distortions during simulated gambling

PNAS, 111( 16), 6098-6103.

DOI:10.1073/pnas.1322295111      URL     [本文引用: 2]

Gambling is a naturalistic example of risky decision-making. During gambling, players typically display an array of cognitive biases that create a distorted expectancy of winning. This study investigated brain regions underpinning gambling-related cognitive distortions, contrasting patients with focal brain lesions to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), insula, or amygdala ("target patients") against healthy comparison participants and lesion comparison patients (i.e., with lesions that spare the target regions). A slot machine task was used to deliver near-miss outcomes (i.e., nonwins that fall spatially close to a jackpot), and a roulette game was used to examine the gambler's fallacy (color decisions following outcome runs). Comparison groups displayed a heightened motivation to play following near misses (compared with full misses), and manifested a classic gambler's fallacy effect. Both effects were also observed in patients with vmPFC and amygdala damage, but were absent in patients with insula damage. Our findings indicate that the distorted cognitive processing of near-miss outcomes and event sequences may be ordinarily supported by the recruitment of the insula. Interventions to reduce insula reactivity could show promise in the treatment of disordered gambling.

Contreras M., Ceric F., & Torrealba F. ( 2007).

Inactivation of the interoceptive insula disrupts drug craving and malaise induced by lithium

Science, 318( 5850), 655-658.

DOI:10.1126/science.1145590      URL     PMID:17962567      [本文引用: 1]

Addiction profoundly alters motivational circuits so that drugs become powerful reinforcers of behavior. The interoceptive system continuously updates homeostatic and emotional information that are important elements in motivational decisions. We tested the idea that interoceptive information is essential in drug craving and in the behavioral signs of malaise. We inactivated the primary interoceptive cortex in amphetamine-experienced rats, which prevented the urge to seek amphetamine in a place preference task. Interoceptive insula inactivation also blunted the signs of malaise induced by acute lithium administration. Drug-seeking and malaise both induced Fos expression, a marker of neuronal activation, in the insula. We conclude that the insular cortex is a key structure in the perception of bodily needs that provides direction to motivated behaviors.

Côté D., Caron A., Aubert J., Desrochers V., & Ladouceur R. ( 2003).

Near wins prolong gambling on a video lottery terminal

. Journal of Gambling Studies, 19( 4), 433-438.

DOI:10.1023/A:1026384011003      URL     PMID:14634302      [本文引用: 1]

Abstract The purpose of the present study was to evaluate whether near wins can prolong gambling activity on a video lottery terminal. In a three-reel game, near wins were operationally defined as two identical symbols followed by a third different symbol. Players in an experimental condition were exposed to 27% near wins in a series of continuous losses, whereas players in a control group were exposed to none. Participants played as long as they wished, and received real money for their wins. The results showed that players in the near win condition played 33% more games than did the control group. The results of this study suggest that near wins can be added to the list of factors that may motivate people to gamble despite the probability of monetary loss.

Dixon M. J., Harrigan K. A., Jarick M., MacLaren V., Fugelsang, J. A. & Sheepy, E. ( 2011).

Psychophysiological arousal signatures of near-misses in slot machine play

International Gambling Studies, 11( 3), 393-407.

DOI:10.1080/14459795.2011.603134      URL     [本文引用: 5]

Near-misses in slot machines resemble jackpot wins but fall just short (e.g. two red sevens on the payline and a third just above). These outcomes have been shown to be subjectively arousing outcomes that promote further slot machine play. We assessed the psychophysical responses of 65 participants to wins, losses and near-misses while playing a slot machine simulator. Skin conductance responses were significantly larger for near-misses than either wins or losses. Similarly, heart rate deceleration was significantly larger for near-misses than either wins or losses. These arousal responses were not mediated by players' problem gambling status near-misses generated large arousal responses even among novice players. We propose that these arousal patterns are due to the frustration of just missing a big win. This research shows the psychophysiological responses triggered by near-misses, and furthers our understanding of how near-misses promote further play.

Dixon M. J., MacLaren V., Jarick M., Fugelsang J. A., & Harrigan K. A. ( 2013).

The frustrating effects of just missing the jackpot: Slot machine near-misses trigger large skin conductance responses, but no post-reinforcement pauses

Journal of Gambling Studies, 29( 4), 661-674.

[本文引用: 5]

Dixon, M. R., & Schreiber, J. E. ( 2004).

Near-miss effects on response latencies and win estimations of slot machine players

The Psychological Record, 54( 3), 335-348.

DOI:10.1007/BF03395477      URL     [本文引用: 2]

The present study examined the degree to which slot machine near-miss trials, or trials that displayed 2 of 3 winning symbols on the payoff line, affected response times and win estimations of 12 recreational slot machine players. Participants played a commercial slot machine in a casino-like laboratory for course extra-credit points. Videotaped sessions were later coded to assess trial types and participant response latencies and win estimations for each participant. Results show that all 12 participants emitted verbalizations suggesting near-miss trials were of a higher approximation to a win than non-near-miss losing trials. Of the 12 participants 8 also demonstrated higher response latencies following losing trials than following winning trials. Variations across participants response latencies were attributed to the presence of and response to near miss trials. The implications of the near-miss on game preference, resistance to extinction, and the development of a behavioral treatment for pathological gamblers are discussed.

Doñamayor N., Marco-Pallarés J., Heldmann M., Schoenfeld M. A., & Münte T. F. ( 2011).

Temporal dynamics of reward processing revealed by magnetoencephalography

Human Brain Mapping, 32( 12), 2228-2240.

DOI:10.1002/hbm.21184      URL     PMID:21305665      [本文引用: 1]

<P>Monetary gains and losses in gambling situations are associated with a distinct electroencephalographic signature: in the event-related potentials (ERPs), a mediofrontal feedback-related negativity (FRN) is seen for losses, whereas oscillatory activity shows a burst of in the -range for losses and in the -range for gains. We used whole-head magnetoencephalography to pinpoint the magnetic counterparts of these effects in young healthy adults and explore their evolution over time. On each trial, participants bet on one of two visually presented numbers (25 or 5) by button-press. Both numbers changed color: if the chosen number turned green (red), it indicated a gain (loss) of the corresponding sum in Euro cent. For losses, we found the magnetic correlate of the FRN extending between 230 and 465 ms. Source localization with low-resolution electromagnetic tomography indicated a first generator in posterior cingulate cortex with subsequent activity in the anterior cingulate cortex. Importantly, this effect was sensitive to the magnitude of the monetary loss (25 cent > 5 cent). Later activation was also found in the right insula. Time-frequency analysis revealed a number of oscillatory components in the theta, alpha, and high-beta/low-gamma bands associated to gains, and in the high-beta band, associated to the magnitude of the loss. All together, these effects provide a more fine-grained picture of the temporal dynamics of the processing of monetary rewards and losses in the brain. Hum Brain Mapp, 2011. 2011 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.</P>

Downar J., Blumberger D. M., & Daskalakis Z. J. ( 2016).

The neural crossroads of psychiatric illness: an emerging target for brain stimulation

Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20( 2), 107-120.

DOI:10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.007      URL     PMID:26655436      [本文引用: 1]

The structural and functional correlates of most major psychiatric disorders are becoming increasingly well characterized, owing to expanding databases of neuroimaging studies and new quantitative meta-analytic techniques. A ‘common core’ of areas is affected across most categories of psychiatric illness; key nodes include the dACC and anterior insula. This common core corresponds well to a one of the resting-state networks of the brain, known as the aCIN or ‘salience’ network; standing in a ‘crossroads’ position in the network architecture of the brain, it is active during behavioral self-control, emotion regulation, and social cognition. The common core network may be amenable to therapeutic brain stimulation via repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS), transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), and deep brain stimulation (DBS). Its nodes may represent a promising target for future studies of therapeutic brain stimulation.

Dymond S., Lawrence N. S., Dunkley B. T., Yuen K. S. L., Hinton E. C., Dixon M. R., .. Singh K.D. ( 2014).

Almost winning: Induced MEG theta power in insula and orbitofrontal cortex increases during gambling near-misses and is associated with BOLD signal and gambling severity

NeuroImage, 91, 210-219.

DOI:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.019      URL     [本文引用: 8]

Epstude, K., & Roese, N. J. ( 2008).

The functional theory of counterfactual thinking

Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12( 2), 168-192.

DOI:10.1177/1088868308316091      URL     PMID:2408534      [本文引用: 1]

Thinking about what might have been ounterfactual thinking s a common feature of the mental landscape. Key questions about counterfactual thinking center on why and how they occur and what downstream cognitive and behavioral outcomes they engender. The functional theory of counterfactual thinking aims to answer these and other questions by drawing connections to goal cognition and by specifying distinct functions that counterfactuals may serve, including preparing for goal pursuit and regulating affect. Since the publication of our last theoretical statement ( Epstude & Roese, 2008 ), numerous lines of empirical evidence support, or are rendered more readily understandable, when glimpsed through the lens of the functional theory. However, other lines of evidence have called into question the very basis of the theory. We integrate a broad range of findings spanning several psychological disciplines so as to present an updated version of the functional theory. We integrate findings from social psychology, cognitive neuroscience, developmental psychology, clinical psychology, and health psychology that support the claim that episodic counterfactual thoughts are geared mainly toward preparation and goal striving and are generally beneficial for individuals. Counterfactuals may influence behavior via either a content-specific pathway (in which the counterfactual insight informs behavior change) or a content-neutral pathway (in which the negative affect from the counterfactual motivates generic behavior change). Challenges to the functional theory of counterfactual thinking center on whether counterfactuals typically cohere to a structural form amenable to goal striving and whether behavioral consequences are mainly dysfunctional rather than functional. Integrating both supporting and challenging evidence, we offer a new theoretical synthesis intended to clarify the literature and guide future research in multiple disciplines of psychology.

Gehring, W. J., & Willoughby, A. R. ( 2002).

The medial frontal cortex and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses

Science, 295( 5563), 2279-2282.

DOI:10.1126/science.1066893      URL     [本文引用: 4]

Goodkind M., Eickhoff S. B., Oathes D. J., Jiang Y., Chang A., Jones-Hagata L. B., … Etkin A. ( 2015).

Identification of a common neurobiological substrate for mental illness

JAMA psychiatry, 72( 4), 305-315.

DOI:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2206      URL     PMID:25651064      [本文引用: 1]

ImportancePsychiatric diagnoses are currently distinguished based on sets of specific symptoms. However, genetic and clinical analyses find similarities across a wide variety of diagnoses, suggesting that a common neurobiological substrate may exist across mental illness.ObjectiveTo conduct a meta-analysis of structural neuroimaging studies across multiple psychiatric diagnoses, followed by parallel analyses of 3 large-scale healthy participant data sets to help interpret structural findings in the meta-analysis.Data SourcesPubMed was searched to identify voxel-based morphometry studies through July 2012 comparing psychiatric patients to healthy control individuals for the meta-analysis. The 3 parallel healthy participant data sets included resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging, a database of activation foci across thousands of neuroimaging experiments, and a data set with structural imaging and cognitive task performance data.Data Extraction and SynthesisStudies were included in the meta-analysis if they reported voxel-based morphometry differences between patients with an Axis I diagnosis and control individuals in stereotactic coordinates across the whole brain, did not present predominantly in childhood, and had at least 10 studies contributing to that diagnosis (or across closely related diagnoses). The meta-analysis was conducted on peak voxel coordinates using an activation likelihood estimation approach.Main Outcomes and MeasuresWe tested for areas of common gray matter volume increase or decrease across Axis I diagnoses, as well as areas differing between diagnoses. Follow-up analyses on other healthy participant data sets tested connectivity related to regions arising from the meta-analysis and the relationship of gray matter volume to cognition.ResultsBased on the voxel-based morphometry meta-analysis of 193 studies comprising 15 892 individuals across 6 diverse diagnostic groups (schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, depression, addiction, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and anxiety), we found that gray matter loss converged across diagnoses in 3 regions: the dorsal anterior cingulate, right insula, and left insula. By contrast, there were few diagnosis-specific effects, distinguishing only schizophrenia and depression from other diagnoses. In the parallel follow-up analyses of the 3 independent healthy participant data sets, we found that the common gray matter loss regions formed a tightly interconnected network during tasks and at resting and that lower gray matter in this network was associated with poor executive functioning.Conclusions and RevelanceWe identified a concordance across psychiatric diagnoses in terms of integrity of an anterior insula/dorsal anterior cingulate ased network, which may relate to executive function deficits observed across diagnoses. This concordance provides an organizing model that emphasizes the importance of shared neural substrates across psychopathology, despite likely diverse etiologies, which is currently not an explicit component of psychiatric nosology.

Goudriaan A. E., Oosterlaan J., de Beurs E., & Van den Brink, W. ( 2004).

Pathological gambling: a comprehensive review of biobehavioral findings

Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 28( 2), 123-141.

DOI:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.001      URL     PMID:15172761      [本文引用: 1]

In this review, findings of biobehavioral research into pathological gambling (PG) are discussed, focusing on neuropsychological, psychophysiological, neuroimaging, neurochemical and genetic studies. Neuropsychological studies indicate deficiencies in certain executive functions. Psychophysiological studies indicate that arousal in PG is of importance when reward is present. Neuroimaging studies point to abnormalities in brain functioning. Recent research into the neurochemistry of PG indicates that abnormalities exist in different neurotransmitter systems. Finally, genetic studies indicate the existence of abnormal dopamine receptor genes in PG. Methodological and theoretical factors that may explain discrepancies between studies include differences in screening and assessment, heterogeneity of gambling problems and different underlying cognitive or motivational mechanisms. Results from the PG studies fit in with recent theoretical models of addiction and PG, which stress the involvement of brain reward pathways, neurotransmitter abnormalities, the frontal cortex and the psychophysiological stress system. A framework for future studies is suggested, indicating the need for studies that integrate knowledge from different research areas, and that employ stricter diagnostic screening methods and inclusion of clinical control groups.

Griffiths, M. ( 1991).

Psychobiology of the near-miss in fruit machine gambling

The Journal of psychology, 125( 3), 347-357.

DOI:10.1080/00223980.1991.10543298      URL     PMID:1880756      [本文引用: 3]

Explanations involving the etiology of have tended to emphasize psychosocial factors. However, the possibility that psychobiological factors may be important in the of should not be ruled out. Psychobiological approaches are becoming ever more prominent with the three main lines of research being (a) a search for a physiological disposition and/or underlying biological substrate in pathological gamblers, (b) an examination of the role of arousal in gambling, and (c) speculation about endorphin-related explanations. The data from questionnaires and interviews with fruit machine gamblers suggest that both physiological and cognitive factors (e.g., the psychology of the near-miss) may be important in the explanation of excessive fruit machine gambling. Thus, if a gambler becomes physiologically aroused when he or she wins or nearly wins, it will stimulate further play, here termed the psychobiology of the near-miss.

Griffiths, M. ( 1995).

Scratch-card gambling: A potential addiction?

Education and Health, 13( 2), 17-20.

[本文引用: 1]

Habib, R., & Dixon, M. R. ( 2010).

Neurobehavioral evidence for the ‘‘near-miss’’ effect in pathological gamblers

Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 93( 3), 313-328.

DOI:10.1901/jeab.2010.93-313      URL     PMID:2861872      [本文引用: 4]

The purpose of this translational study was twofold: (1) to contrast behavioral and brain activity between pathological and nonpathological gamblers, and (2) to examine differences as a function of the outcome of the spin of a slot machine, focusing predominately on the “Near-Miss”—when two reels stop on the same symbol, and that symbol is just above or below the payoff line on the third reel. Twenty-two participants (11 nonpathological; 11 pathological) completed the study by rating the closeness of various outcomes of slot machine displays (wins, losses, and near-misses) to a win. No behavioral differences were observed between groups of participants, however, differences in brain activity were found in the left midbrain, near the substantia nigra and ventral tegmental area (SN / VTA). Near-miss outcomes uniquely activated brain regions associated with wins for the pathological gamblers and regions associated with losses for the nonpathological gamblers. Thus, near-miss outcomes on slot machines may contain both functional and neurological properties of wins for pathological gamblers. Such a translational approach to the study of gambling behavior may be considered an example that gives life to B. F. Skinner's conceptualization of the physiologist of the future.

Hajcak G., Moser J. S., Holroyd C. B., & Simons R. F. ( 2007).

It’s worse than you thought: The feedback negativity and violations of reward prediction in gambling tasks

. Psychophysiology, 44( 6), 905-912.

DOI:10.1111/psyp.2007.44.issue-6      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Harrigan, K. A. ( 2009).

Slot machines: Pursuing responsible gaming practices for virtual reels and near misses

International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 7( 1), 68-83.

DOI:10.1007/s11469-007-9139-8      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Since 1983, slot machines in North America have used a computer and virtual reels to determine the odds. Since at least 1988, a technique called clustering has been used to create a high number of near misses, failures that are close to wins. The result is that what the player sees does not represent the underlying probabilities and randomness, and this misrepresented outcome will have some effect on the player perceptions of the game, which may lead directly to classical and operant conditioning, the frustration effect, the perception of early wins, illusion of control, biased evaluation of outcomes, entrapment, and irrational thinking. We use transcripts of Nevada hearings to show that the initial proponents understood that virtual reels and near misses may have a detrimental psychological effect on the player. We conclude by suggesting that jurisdictions should consider the historical facts and research presented in this paper when pursuing responsible gaming practices for slot machines.

Hodgins D. C., Stea J. N., & Grant J. E. ( 2011).

Gambling disorders

The Lancet, 378( 9806), 1874-1884.

[本文引用: 1]

Hoeck N. V., Ma N., Ampe L., Baetens K., Vandekerckhove M., & Overwalle F. V. ( 2013).

Counterfactual thinking: an fMRI study on changing the past for a better future

Social Cognitive & Affective Neuroscience, 8( 5), 556-564.

DOI:10.1093/scan/nss031      URL     PMID:22403155      [本文引用: 1]

Abstract Recent studies suggest that a brain network mainly associated with episodic memory has a more general function in imagining oneself in another time, place or perspective (e.g. episodic future thought, theory of mind, default mode). If this is true, counterfactual thinking (e.g. 'If I had left the office earlier, I wouldn't have missed my train.') should also activate this network. Present functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study explores the common and distinct neural activity of counterfactual and episodic thinking by directly comparing the imagining of upward counterfactuals (creating better outcomes for negative past events) with the re-experiencing of negative past events and the imagining of positive future events. Results confirm that episodic and counterfactual thinking share a common brain network, involving a core memory network (hippocampal area, temporal lobes, midline, and lateral parietal lobes) and prefrontal areas that might be related to mentalizing (medial prefrontal cortex) and performance monitoring (right prefrontal cortex). In contrast to episodic past and future thinking, counterfactual thinking recruits some of these areas more strongly and extensively, and additionally activates the bilateral inferior parietal lobe and posterior medial frontal cortex. We discuss these findings in view of recent fMRI evidence on the working of episodic memory and theory of mind.

Holroyd, C. B., & Coles, M. G. H. ( 2002).

The neural basis of human error processing: Reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity

Psychological Review, 109( 4), 679-709.

DOI:10.1037/0033-295X.109.4.679      URL     [本文引用: 4]

Holroyd C. B., Nieuwenhuis S., Yeung N., Nystrom L., Mars R. B., Coles M. G. H., & Cohen J. D. ( 2004).

Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex shows fMRI response to internal and external error signals

Nature neuroscience, 7( 5), 497-498.

DOI:10.1038/nn1238      URL     PMID:15097995      [本文引用: 1]

In our event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiment, participants learned to select between two response options by trial-and-error, using feedback stimuli that indicated monetary gains and losses. The results of the experiment indicate that error responses and error feedback activate the same region of dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, suggesting that this region is sensitive to both internal and external sources of error information.

Hommer D. W., Bjork J. M., & Gilman J. M. ( 2011).

Imaging brain response to reward in addictive disorders

Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1216( 1), 50-61.

DOI:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05898.x      URL     PMID:21272010      [本文引用: 1]

We compare the evidence from human neuroimaging studies for and against two of the major hypotheses of how alterations in the brain's reward system underlie addiction. One of these, the impulsivity hypothesis, proposes that addiction is characterized by excessive sensitivity to reward combined with a failure of inhibition. The other, the reward-deficiency hypothesis, proposes that addicted individuals have a reduced response to nondrug rewards that leads them to seek drugs in preference to more socially acceptable goals. Positron emission tomographic (PET) studies of dopamine receptor density and dopamine release strongly support the reward-deficiency hypothesis, while the more recent and numerous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies of goal-directed behavior provide both support and contradiction for each of the hypotheses. Differences in the time scale on which PET and fMRI make measurements probably account for differences in results, at least in part. It is likely that aspects of brain function described by both the impulsivity and reward-deficiency hypotheses contribute to the pathophysiology of addiction.

Jessup, R. K., & O'Doherty, J. P. ( 2011).

Human dorsal striatal activity during choice discriminates reinforcement learning behavior from the gambler's fallacy

Journal of Neuroscience, 31( 17), 6296-6304.

[本文引用: 1]

Kreussel L., Hewig J., Kretschmer N., Hecht H., Coles M. G. H., & Miltner, W. H. R. ( 2013).

How bad was it? Differences in the time course of sensitivity to the magnitude of loss in problem gamblers and controls

Behavioural Brain Research, 247, 140-145.

DOI:10.1016/j.bbr.2013.03.024      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Larche C. J., Musielak N., & Dixon M. J. ( 2017).

The Candy Crush Sweet Tooth: How ‘Near-misses’ in Candy Crush Increase Frustration, and the Urge to Continue Gameplay

Journal of gambling studies, 33( 2), 599-615.

[本文引用: 3]

Levens S. M., Larsen J. T., Bruss J., Tranel D., Bechara A., & Mellers B. A. ( 2014).

What might have been? The role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and lateral orbitofrontal cortex in counterfactual emotions and choice

Neuropsychologia, 54, 77-86.

DOI:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.10.026      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Liu X., Hairston J., Schrier M., & Fan J. ( 2011).

Common and distinct networks underlying reward valence and processing stages: a meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies

. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35( 5), 1219-1236.

[本文引用: 4]

Lole L., Gonsalvez C. J., & Barry R. J. ( 2015).

Reward and punishment hyposensitivity in problem gamblers: A study of event-related potentials using a principal components analysis

Clinical Neurophysiology, 126( 7), 1295-1309.

DOI:10.1016/j.clinph.2014.10.011      URL     [本文引用: 2]

Lole L., Gonsalvez C. J., Barry R. J., & De Blasio, F. M. ( 2013).

Can event-related potentials serve as neural markers for wins, losses, and near-wins in a gambling task? A principal components analysis

International Journal of Psychophysiology, 89( 3), 390-398.

[本文引用: 6]

Luo Q., Wang Y., & Qu C. ( 2011).

The near-miss effect in slot-machine gambling: modulation of feedback-related negativity by subjective value.

Neuroreport, 22(#18), 989-993.

DOI:10.1097/WNR.0b013e32834da8ae      URL     [本文引用: 5]

MacLin O. H., Dixon M. R., Daugherty D., & Small S. L. ( 2007).

Using a computer simulation of three slot machines to investigate a gambler’s preference among varying densities of near-miss alternatives

Behavior Research Methods, 39( 2), 237-241.

[本文引用: 1]

Mas-Herrero, E., & Marco-Pallarés, J. ( 2014).

Frontal theta oscillatory activity is a common mechanism for the computation of unexpected outcomes and learning rate

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26( 3), 447-458.

DOI:10.1162/jocn_a_00516      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Murty V. P., Stanek J. K., & Heusser A. C. ( 2013).

Representations of distinct salience signals in the nucleus accumbens

Journal of Neuroscience, 33( 39), 15319-15320.

DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3002-13.2013      URL     PMID:24068797      [本文引用: 1]

During perinatal development, corticospinal tract (CST) projections into the spinal cord help refine spinal circuitry. Although the normal developmental processes that are controlled by the arrival of corticospinal input are becoming clear, little is known about how perinatal cortical damage impacts specific aspects of spinal circuit development, particularly the inhibitory microcircuitry that regulates spinal reflex circuits. In this study, we sought to determine how ischemic cortical damage impacts the synaptic attributes of a well-characterized population of inhibitory, GABAergic interneurons, called GABApre neurons, which modulates the efficiency of proprioceptive sensory terminals in the sensorimotor reflex circuit. We found that putative GABApre interneurons receive CST input and, using an established mouse model of perinatal stroke, that cortical ischemic injury results in a reduction of CST density within the intermediate region of the spinal cord, where these interneurons reside. Importantly, CST alterations were restricted to the side contralateral to the injury. Within the synaptic terminals of the GABApre interneurons, we observed a dramatic upregulation of the 65-isoform of the GABA synthetic enzyme glutamic acid decarboxylase (GAD65). In accordance with the CST density reduction, GAD65 was elevated on the side of the spinal cord contralateral to cortical injury. This effect was not seen for other GABApre synaptic markers or in animals that received sham surgery. Our data reveal a novel effect of perinatal stroke that involves severe deficits in the architecture of a descending spinal pathway, which in turn appear to promote molecular alterations in a specific spinal GABAergic circuit.

Namkung H., Kim S. H., & Sawa A. ( 2017).

The insula: an underestimated brain area in clinical neuroscience, psychiatry, and neurology

Trends in Neurosciences, 40( 4), 200-207.

DOI:10.1016/j.tins.2017.02.002      URL     [本文引用: 4]

Naqvi, N. H., & Bechara, A. ( 2009).

The hidden island of addiction: the insula

Trends in neurosciences, 32( 1), 56-67.

DOI:10.1016/j.tins.2008.09.009      URL     PMID:3698860      [本文引用: 1]

Most prior research on the neurobiology of addiction has focused on the role of subcortical systems, such as the amygdala, the ventral striatum and mesolimbic dopamine system, in promoting the motivation to seek drugs. Recent evidence indicates that a largely overlooked structure, the insula, plays a crucial part in conscious urges to take drugs. The insula has been highlighted as a region that integrates interoceptive (i.e. bodily) states into conscious feelings and into decision-making processes that involve uncertain risk and reward. Here, we propose a model in which the processing of the interoceptive effects of drug use by the insula contributes to conscious drug urges and to decision-making processes that precipitate relapse.

Naqvi N. H., Rudrauf D., Damasio H., & Bechara A. ( 2007).

Damage to the insula disrupts addiction to cigarette smoking

Science, 315( 5811), 531-534.

DOI:10.1126/science.1135926      URL     PMID:17255515      [本文引用: 1]

A number of brain systems have been implicated in addictive behavior, but none have yet been shown to be necessary for maintaining the addiction to cigarette smoking. We found that smokers with brain damage involving the insula, a region implicated in conscious urges, were more likely than smokers with brain damage not involving the insula to undergo a disruption of smoking addiction, characterized by the ability to quit smoking easily, immediately, without relapse, and without persistence of the urge to smoke. This result suggests that the insula is a critical neural substrate in the addiction to smoking.

Nastally, B. L., & Dixon, M. R. ( 2012).

The effect of a brief acceptance and commitment therapy intervention on the near-miss effect in problem gamblers

The Psychological Record, 62( 4), 677-690.

DOI:10.1007/BF03395828      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Nieuwenhuis S., Aston-Jones G., & Cohen J. D. ( 2005).

Decision making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system

Psychological Bulletin, 131( 4), 510-532.

DOI:10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.510      URL     PMID:16060800      [本文引用: 3]

Psychologists and neuroscientists have had a long-standing interest in the P3, a prominent component of the event-related brain potential. This review aims to integrate knowledge regarding the neural basis of the P3 and to elucidate its functional role in information processing. The authors review evidence suggesting that the P3 reflects phasic activity of the neuromodulatory locus coeruleus-norepinephrine (LC-NE) system. They discuss the P3 literature in the light of empirical findings and a recent theory regarding the information-processing function of the LC-NE phasic response. The theoretical framework emerging from this research synthesis suggests that the P3 reflects the response of the LC-NE system to the outcome of internal decision-making processes and the consequent effects of noradrenergic potentiation of information processing.

Nieuwenhuis S., Holroyd C. B., Mol N., & Coles, M. G. H. ( 2004).

Reinforcement-related brain potentials from medial frontal cortex: Origins and functional significance

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 28( 4), 441-448.

DOI:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.05.003      URL     [本文引用: 2]

Peters H., Hunt M., & Harper D. ( 2010).

An animal model of slot machine gambling: The effect of structural characteristics on response latency and persistence

Journal of Gambling Studies, 26( 4), 521-531.

DOI:10.1007/s10899-010-9183-3      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Polich, J. ( 2012).

Neuropsychology of P300

In E. S. Kappenman, & S. J. Luck (Eds.), Oxford handbook of event-related potential components (pp. 159-188). Oxford Universiy Press.

[本文引用: 4]

Qi S., Ding C., Song Y., & Yang D. ( 2011).

Neural correlates of near-misses effect in gambling

Neurosci. Lett., 493( 3), 80-85.

DOI:10.1016/j.neulet.2011.01.059      URL     PMID:21315803      [本文引用: 3]

The present study investigated the cognitive and neural mechanisms underlying the gambling near-miss effect by measuring event-related-potentials. Using a simple gambling task, we measured behavioral response and electrophysiological activity of gambling outcomes. Self-rating results showed that when compared to full-miss outcome, near-miss outcome were rated as less pleasant, but yielded higher motivation to play. Whereas the feedback-related negativity (FRN) amplitude did not reflect the motivation rating differences between near-miss and full-miss, the P300 amplitude mirrored the motivation rating differences between near-miss and full-miss, with larger amplitudes for near-miss outcomes. Dipole source analysis of the difference wave (near-miss minus full-miss) indicated that two generators of the P300, localized in the putamen and orbitofrontal cortex, might be involved in motivational evaluation and regret, respectively. Our findings indicated that the near-miss effect stems from sources: higher levels of motivation and the presence of regret, caused by counterfactual thinking.

Reid, R. L. ( 1986).

The psychology of the near miss

Journal of Gambling Behaviour, 2( 1), 32-39.

DOI:10.1007/BF01019932      URL     [本文引用: 4]

Near misses are widely believed to encourage future play, even in games of chance where the probability of winning remains constant from trial to trial. Some commercial gambling systems, particularly instant lotteries and slot machines, are contrived to ensure a higher frequency of near misses than would be expected by chance alone. Theoretical interpretations and relevant experiments are examined. A distinction is drawn between possible short-term and longer-term effects of manipulating the rate of occurrence of near misses.

Roese, N. J. ( 1997).

Counterfactual thinking

Psychological Bulletin, 121( 1), 133-148.

DOI:10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.133      URL     [本文引用: 3]

Roese, N. J., & Epstude, K. ( 2017).

The functional theory of counterfactual thinking: New evidence, new challenges, new insights

Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 56, 1-79.

URL     [本文引用: 2]

Advances in Experimental Social Psychology continues to be one of the most sought after and most often cited series in this field. Containing contributions of major empirical and theoretical interest, this series represents the best and the brightest in new research, theory, and practice in social psychology. *One of the most well-received and credible series in social psychology *Chapters spanning such diverse areas such as goal achievement, interracial relations, and self defense *An excellent resource for researchers, librarians, and academics

Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. ( 1995).

Outcome controllability and counterfactual thinking

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21( 6), 620-628.

DOI:10.1177/0146167295216008      URL     [本文引用: 1]

ABSTRACT The impact of outcome controllability on the direction of counterfactual thoughts (reconstructions of past outcomes based on "might have been"alternatives) was examined in two laboratory experiments. Counterfactual direction reflects the distinction between upward counterfactuals (focusing on how things could have been better) and downward counterfactuals (focusing on how things could have been worse). Previous research has shown that upward counterfactuals are more frequent after failure, even though consideration of downward counterfactuals is affectively self-enhancing. Two studies showed that outcome controllability affects counterfactual direction: Upward counterfactuals were more frequent following controllable outcomes, whereas downward counterfactuals were more frequent following uncontrollable outcomes. Paralleling past research, upward counterfactuals were more frequent after failure, whereas downward counterfactuals were more frequent after success. These findings are consistent with an emerging functional theory of counterfactual thinking.

Schultz, W. ( 2006).

Behavioral theories and the neurophysiology of reward

Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 87-115.

DOI:10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070229      URL     [本文引用: 7]

Sescousse G., Janssen L. K., Hashemi M. M., Timmer M. H., Geurts D. E., & ter Huurne, N. P., et al. ( 2016).

Amplified striatal responses to near-miss outcomes in pathological gamblers

Neuropsychopharmacology, 41( 10), 2614-262.

DOI:10.1038/npp.2016.43      URL     PMID:27006113      [本文引用: 4]

Neuropsychopharmacology, the official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, publishing the highest quality original research and advancing our understanding of the brain and behavior.

Shao R., Read J., Behrens T. E. J., & Rogers R. D. ( 2013).

Shifts in reinforcement signalling while playing slot-machines as a function of prior experience and impulsivity

Translational Psychiatry, 3, e213.

[本文引用: 1]

Sharman S., Aitken M. R., & Clark L. ( 2015).

Dual effects of ‘losses disguised as wins’ and near-misses in a slot machine game

International Gambling Studies, 15( 2), 212-223.

DOI:10.1080/14459795.2015.1020959      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Individually, both near-misses and losses disguised as wins (LDWs) have been seen to exert pro-motivational effects on gambling. However, it is not clear whether both structural characteristics are effective within the same game. Participants (n = 40) played a slot machine simulation. The simulation delivered near-misses, wins and 'full-misses'. Half the participants also received LDWs that occurred independently of the outcomes on the payline. Valence and motivation ratings were collected after each round. Results showed that the LDW group reported increased valence ratings compared to the no-LDW group. Within the LDW group, trials with LDWs also resulted in increased enjoyment compared to trials without LDWs. We distinguished near-misses falling either side of the payline. Near-misses before the payline (NMB) were rated as more motivational than near-misses after the payline (NMA), whereas NMAs were rated as more aversive than NMBs. These differences between the two near-miss types were exacerbated by LDWs. Results demonstrate LDWs increase the trial-by-trial enjoyment of non-win outcomes. The motivational and hedonic effects of near-misses differed for events either side of the payline, and these differences were exaggerated by the presence of LDWs. Thus, near-misses can retain their effectiveness in complex forms of gambling that also deliver LDWs.

Sharman, S., & Clark, L. ( 2016).

Mixed emotions to near-miss outcomes: A psychophysiological study with facial electromyography

Journal of Gambling Studies, 32( 3), 823-834.

DOI:10.1007/s10899-015-9578-2      URL     PMID:26482890      [本文引用: 3]

Near-misses occur across many forms of gambling and are rated as unpleasant while simultaneously increasing the motivation to continue playing. On slot machines, the icon position relative to the payline moderates the effects of near-misses, with near-misses before the payline increasing motivation, and near-misses after the payline being rated as aversive. Near-misses are also known to increase physiological arousal compared to full-misses, but physiological measures to date have not been able to dissociate positive and negative emotional responses. The present study measured facial electromyography at the corrugator (brow) and zygomaticus (cheek) sites, as well as electrodermal activity (), following gambling outcomes on a two-reel slot machine simulation in 77 novice gamblers. Behavioral data was collected using trial-by-trial ratings of motivation and valence. Wins were rated as more pleasant and increased motivation to continue playing, compared to non-win outcomes. Wins were also accompanied by increased and zygomaticus activity. Near-misses after the payline were rated as more aversive than other non-wins, and this was accompanied by increased and zygomaticus activity. Near-misses before the payline increased motivation to continue playing, and were accompanied by increased . Thus, both subjective and physiological responses to near-misses differ for events falling either side of the payline. The 'near-miss effect' is not a unitary phenomenon. Facial EMG has differential sensitivity to positive and negative valence and may be a useful measure for future studies of gambling .

Stange M., Grau M., Osazuwa S., Graydon C., & Dixon M. J. ( 2017).

Reinforcing small wins and frustrating near-misses: Further investigation into scratch card gambling

Journal of Gambling Studies, 33( 1), 47-63.

[本文引用: 2]

Stange M., Graydon C., & Dixon M. J. ( 2016).

“I was that close”: Investigating players’ reactions to losses, wins, and near-misses on scratch cards

Journal of Gambling Studies, 32( 1), 187-203.

[本文引用: 3]

Stange M., Graydon C., & Dixon M. J. ( 2017).

Increased urge to gamble following near-miss outcomes may drive purchasing behaviour in scratch card gambling

Journal of Gambling Studies, 33( 3), 867-879.

[本文引用: 1]

Tang D. W., Fellows L. K., Small D. M., & Dagher A. ( 2012).

Food and drug cues activate similar brain regions: a meta-analysis of functional MRI studies

Physiology & Behavior, 106( 3), 317-324.

DOI:10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.03.009      URL     PMID:22450260      [本文引用: 1]

78 Food cues trigger hunger and feeding, and smoking cues trigger craving and relapse in smokers. 78 Responses in the appetitive brain regions to food and smoking cues have not been adequately compared. 78 Comparing food and smoking cue response will clarify mechanisms of natural and drug reward systems. 78 Using meta-analyses, we found that food and smoking cues activate comparable brain networks. 78 This is consistent with the theory that cues are conditioned incentives.

Tremblay L., Worbe Y. , & Hollerman, J. R.( 2009) .

The ventral striatum: a heterogeneous structure involved in reward processing, motivation and decision-making

In Handbook of Reward and Decision Making. (pp 51-77). New York: Academic Press.

[本文引用: 1]

Ulrich, N., & Hewig, J. ( 2014).

A miss is as good as a mile? Processing of near and full outcomes in a gambling paradigm

Psychophysiology, 51( 9), 819-823.

DOI:10.1111/psyp.12232      URL     PMID:24806789      [本文引用: 9]

Studies investigating the feedback-related negativity (FRN) and the following near misses, full misses, and wins have yielded inconsistent results. Furthermore, the results were likely confounded by an oddball effect due to the probabilities of the different outcomes. We introduced a fourth outcome (narrow win), which allows for balanced outcome probabilities and thus rules out potential oddball effects. We measured the FRN and as well as subjective ratings while participants were gambling on a wheel of fortune. The FRN was larger following misses compared to wins and larger following near compared to full outcomes. For the , we observed a larger positivity following wins compared to misses and full compared to near outcomes. These findings further corroborate that near and full outcomes are processed as distinct events even though they result in the same monetary outcomes.

van Holst R. J., Chase H. W., & Clark L. ( 2014).

Striatal connectivity changes following gambling wins and near-misses: Associations with gambling severity

NeuroImage: Clinical, 5, 232-239.

[本文引用: 2]

van Holst R. J., van den Brink W., Veltman D. J., & Goudriaan A. E. ( 2010).

Why gamblers fail to win: a review of cognitive and neuroimaging findings in pathological gambling

Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34( 1), 87-107.

Winstanley C. A., Cocker P. J., & Rogers R. D. ( 2011).

Dopamine modulates reward expectancy during performance of a slot machine task in rats: Evidence for a ‘near-miss’ effect

Neuropsychopharmacology, 36, 913-925.

DOI:10.1038/npp.2010.230      URL     [本文引用: 4]

Worhunsky P. D., Malison R. T., Rogers R. D., & Potenza M. N. ( 2014).

Altered neural correlates of reward and loss processing during simulated slot-machine fMRI in pathological gambling and cocaine dependence

Drug and alcohol dependence, 145, 77-86.

[本文引用: 4]

Wu Y., van Dijk E., Li H., Aitken M., & Clark L. ( 2017).

On the counterfactual nature of gambling near-misses: an experimental study

Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 30( 4), 855-868.

DOI:10.1002/bdm.2010      URL     PMID:5638081      [本文引用: 1]

Research on gambling near‐misses has shown that objectively equivalent outcomes can yield divergent emotional and motivational responses. The subjective processing of gambling outcomes is affected substantially by close but non‐obtained outcomes (i.e. counterfactuals). In the current paper, we investigate how different types of near‐misses influence self‐perceived luck and subsequent betting behavior in a wheel‐of‐fortune task. We investigate the counterfactual mechanism of these effects by testing the relationship with a second task measuring regret/relief processing. Across two experiments (Experiment 1,n=6551; Experiment 2,n=65104), we demonstrate that near‐wins (neutral outcomes that are close to a jackpot) decreased self‐perceived luck, whereas near‐losses (neutral outcomes that are close to a major penalty) increased luck ratings. The effects of near‐misses varied by near‐missposition(i.e. whether the spinner stopped just short of, or passed through, the counterfactual outcome), consistent with established distinctions between upward versus downward, and additive versus subtractive, counterfactual thinking. In Experiment 1, individuals who showed stronger counterfactual processing on the regret/relief task were more responsive to near‐wins and near‐losses on the wheel‐of‐fortune task. The effect of near‐miss position was attenuated when the anticipatory phase (i.e. the spin and deceleration) was removed in Experiment 2. Further differences were observed within the objective gains and losses, between “clear” and “narrow” outcomes. Taken together, these results help substantiate the counterfactual mechanism of near‐misses. 08 2017 The Authors Journal of Behavioral Decision Making Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

Wu, Y., & Zhou, X. L. ( 2009).

The P300 and reward valence, magnitude, and expectancy in outcome evaluation

Brain Research, 1286, 114-122.

DOI:10.1016/j.brainres.2009.06.032      URL     PMID:19539614      [本文引用: 1]

The P300 in event-related potentials (ERPs) has been implicated in outcome evaluation and reward processing, but it is controversial as to what aspects of reward processing it is sensitive. This study manipulated orthogonally reward valence, reward magnitude, and expectancy towards reward magnitude in a monetary gambling task and observed both the valence and the magnitude effects on the P300, but only when the amount of reward was expected on the basis of a previous cue. The FRN (feedback-related negativity), defined as the mean amplitudes of ERP responses to the loss or the gain outcome in the 250 350 ms time window post-onset of feedback, was found to be sensitive not only to reward valence, but also to expectancy towards reward magnitude and reward magnitude, with the violation of expectancy and the small magnitude eliciting more negative-going FRN. These findings demonstrate that while the FRN may function as a general mechanism that evaluates whether the outcome is consistent or inconsistent with expectation, the P300 is sensitive to a later, top-down controlled process of outcome evaluation, into which factors related to the allocation of attentional resources, including reward valence, reward magnitude, and magnitude expectancy, come to play.

Yeung, N., & Sanfey, A. G. ( 2004).

Independent coding of reward magnitude and valence in the human brain

Journal of Neuroscience, 24( 28), 6258-6264.

DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4537-03.2004      URL     PMID:15254080      [本文引用: 2]

Abstract Previous research has shown that two components of the event-related brain potential, the P300 and feedback negativity, are sensitive to information about rewards and penalties. The present study investigated the properties of these components in a simple gambling game that required participants to choose between cards that were unpredictably associated with monetary gains and losses of variable magnitude. The aim was to determine the sensitivity of each component to two critical features of reward stimuli: magnitude (small or large) and valence (win or loss). A double dissociation was observed, with the P300 sensitive to reward magnitude but insensitive to reward valence and the feedback negativity showing the opposite pattern, suggesting that these two fundamental features of rewarding stimuli are evaluated rapidly and separately in the human brain. Subsequent analyses provided additional evidence of functional dissociations between the feedback negativity and P300. First, the P300 (but not the feedback negativity) showed sensitivity to the reward value of alternative, nonselected stimuli. Second, individual differences in the amplitude of the feedback negativity correlated with individual differences in risk-taking behavior observed after monetary losses, whereas individual differences in P300 amplitude were related to behavioral adjustments observed in response to alternative, unchosen outcomes.

Zaehle T., Bauch E. M., Hinrichs H., Schmitt F. C., Voges J., Heinze H. J., & Bunzeck N. ( 2013).

Nucleus accumbens activity dissociates different forms of salience: evidence from human intracranial recordings

Journal of Neuroscience, 33( 20), 8764-8771.

DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5276-12.2013      URL    

Zeighami, Y., & Moustafa, A. A. ( 2015).

Differential functions of ventral and dorsal striatum

Brain, 138( 10), e381.

DOI:10.1093/brain/awv085      URL     PMID:25833821      [本文引用: 1]

No abstract available.

/


版权所有 © 《心理科学进展》编辑部
地址:北京市朝阳区林萃路16号院 
邮编:100101 
电话:010-64850861 
E-mail:jinzhan@psych.ac.cn
备案编号:京ICP备10049795号-1 京公网安备110402500018号

本系统由北京玛格泰克科技发展有限公司设计开发
51La