Acta Psychologica Sinica ›› 2021, Vol. 53 ›› Issue (5): 456-468.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00456
• Reports of Empirical Studies • Previous Articles Next Articles
SONG Xiyan, CHENG Yahua, XIE Zhouxiutian, GONG Nanyan, LIU Lei()
Received:
2020-08-04
Published:
2021-05-25
Online:
2021-03-29
Contact:
LIU Lei
E-mail:liulei@nbu.edu.cn
Supported by:
SONG Xiyan, CHENG Yahua, XIE Zhouxiutian, GONG Nanyan, LIU Lei. (2021). The influence of anger on delay discounting: The mediating role of certainty and control. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 53(5), 456-468.
Add to citation manager EndNote|Ris|BibTeX
URL: https://journal.psych.ac.cn/acps/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2021.00456
Figure 1. The logarithm k0 of the delay discounting rate under different emotion conditions Note. The smaller the mean, the more likely it was to delay gratification; error bars depicted standard errors; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. The same as below.
Mediation path | Estimated value | 95% CI | |
---|---|---|---|
low | High | ||
Using the control group as reference: | |||
Anger → certainty-control → delay discounting | -0.184a | -0.295 | -0.074 |
Anger → delay discounting | -0.013 | -0.098 | 0.073 |
Fear → certainty-control → delay discounting | 0.396a | 0.294 | 0.498 |
Fear → delay discounting | 0.093 | -0.016 | 0.203 |
Pleasure → certainty-control → delay discounting | -0.141a | -0.242 | -0.039 |
Pleasure → delay discounting | 0.003 | -0.106 | 0.113 |
Table 1 An analysis of the mediating effect of certainty-control on delay discounting
Mediation path | Estimated value | 95% CI | |
---|---|---|---|
low | High | ||
Using the control group as reference: | |||
Anger → certainty-control → delay discounting | -0.184a | -0.295 | -0.074 |
Anger → delay discounting | -0.013 | -0.098 | 0.073 |
Fear → certainty-control → delay discounting | 0.396a | 0.294 | 0.498 |
Fear → delay discounting | 0.093 | -0.016 | 0.203 |
Pleasure → certainty-control → delay discounting | -0.141a | -0.242 | -0.039 |
Pleasure → delay discounting | 0.003 | -0.106 | 0.113 |
[1] | Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173-1182. |
[2] | Benzion, U., Rapoport, A., & Yagil, J. (1989). Discount rates inferred from decisions: An experimental study. Management Science, 35(3), 259-385. |
[3] |
Berns, G. S., Laibson, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2007). Intertemporal choice-toward an integrative framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(11), 482-488.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.011 URL pmid: 17980645 |
[4] | Bodenhausen, G. V., Sheppard, L. A., & Kramer, G. P. (1994). Negative affect and social judgment: The differential impact of anger and sadness. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24(1), 45-62. |
[5] |
Brown, R. L. (2016). The influence of stressor exposure and psychosocial resources on the age-anger relationship: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Aging and Health, 28(8), 1465-1487.
doi: 10.1177/0898264315624900 URL pmid: 26823387 |
[6] |
Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is an approach-related affect: Evidence and implications. Psychological Bulletin, 135(2), 183-204.
doi: 10.1037/a0013965 URL pmid: 19254075 |
[7] | Casey, B. J., Somerville, L. H., Gotlib, I. H., Ayduk, O., Franklin, N. T., Askren, M. K., … Shoda, Y. (2011). Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratification 40 years later. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(36), 14998-15003. |
[8] | Chen, H., Ng, S., & Rao, A. R. (2005). Cultural differences in consumer impatience. Journal of Marketing Research, 42(3), 291-301. |
[9] | Clark, M. S., Pataki, S. P., & Carver, V. H. (1996). Some thoughts and findings on self-presentation of emotions in relationships. In G. J. O. Fletcher, & J. Fitness (Eds.), Knowledge structures in close relationships: A social psychological approach (pp. 247-274). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. |
[10] | Clore, G. L., Gasper, K., & Garvin, E. (2001). Affect as information. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Handbook of affect and social cognition (pp. 121-144). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. |
[11] | Commons, M. L., Mazur, J. E., Nevin, J. A., & Rachlin, H. (Eds.). (1987). Quantitative analyses of behavior, Vol. 5. The effect of delay and of intervening events on reinforcement value. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. |
[12] | de Hooge, I. E. (2017). Combining emotion appraisal dimensions and individual differences to understand emotion effects on gift giving. Journal of Behavioral Decision-making, 30(2), 256-269. |
[13] | Ding, R. Y., Wang, F. X., Niu, D., & Li, B. J. (2014). The effect of emotions associated with certainty (happiness, anger) and uncertainty (sadness) on trust. Journal of Psychological Science, 37(5), 1092-1099. |
[14] | Druckman, J. N., & McDermott, R. (2008). Emotion and the framing of risky choice. Political Behavior, 30(3), 297-321. |
[15] | Fang, C. H., Song, X. Y., Xu, S. P., Cheng, Y. H., Chen, C. Y., & Liu, L. (2019). The differential effects of disgust and fear on intertemporal choice: An ERP study. Journal of Psychological Science, 42(6), 1305-1311. |
[16] |
Figner, B., Knoch, D., Johnson, E. J., Krosch, A. R., Lisanby, S. H., Fehr, E., & Weber, E. U. (2010). Lateral prefrontal cortex and self-control in intertemporal choice. Nature Neuroscience, 13(5), 538-539.
doi: 10.1038/nn.2516 URL pmid: 20348919 |
[17] |
Fischhoff, B., Gonzalez, R. M., Lerner, J. S., & Small, D. A. (2005). Evolving judgments of terror risks: Foresight, hindsight, and emotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 11(2), 124-139.
URL pmid: 15998184 |
[18] | Frederick, S., Loewenstein, G., & O’Donoghue, T. (2002). Time discounting and time preference: A critical review. Journal of Economic Literature, 40(2), 351-401. |
[19] |
Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2008). Approach-motivated positive affect reduces breadth of attention. Psychological Science, 19(5), 476-482.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02112.x URL pmid: 18466409 |
[20] | Gable, P. A., & , Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The motivational dimensional model of affect: Implications for breadth of attention, memory, and cognitive categorisation. Cognition and Emotion, 24(2), 322-337. |
[21] | Gehring, W. J., & Willoughby, A. R. (2002). The medial frontal cortex and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science, 295(5563), 2279-2282. |
[22] |
Gino, F., Brooks, A. W., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2012). Anxiety, advice, and the ability to discern: Feeling anxious motivates individual to seek and use advice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(3), 497-512.
URL pmid: 22121890 |
[23] |
Green, L., & Myerson, J. (2004). A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. Psychological Bulletin, 130(5), 769-792.
URL pmid: 15367080 |
[24] |
Guan, S. C., Cheng, L., Fan, Y., & Li, X. C. (2015). Myopic decisions under negative emotions correlate with altered time perception. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 468.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00468 URL pmid: 25941508 |
[25] | Han, S., Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2007). Feelings and consumer decision-making: The appraisal-tendency framework. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 17(3), 158-168. |
[26] |
Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F., & Rangel, A. (2009). Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science, 324(5927), 646-648.
doi: 10.1126/science.1168450 URL pmid: 19407204 |
[27] |
Kirby, K. N., & Maraković, N. N. (1996). Delay-discounting probabilistic rewards: Rates decrease as amounts increase. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 3(1), 100-104.
doi: 10.3758/BF03210748 URL pmid: 24214810 |
[28] | Knutson, B. (1996). Facial expressions of emotion influence interpersonal trait inferences. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 20(3), 165-182. |
[29] | Laibson, D. (2001). A cue-theory of consumption. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116(1), 81-119. |
[30] |
Leith, K. P., & Baumeister, R. F. (1996). Why do bad moods increase self-defeating behavior? Emotion, risk taking, and self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(6), 1250-1267.
URL pmid: 8979390 |
[31] |
Lench, H. C., Flores, S. A., & Bench, S. W. (2011). Discrete emotions predict changes in cognition, judgment, experience, behavior, and physiology: A meta-analysis of experimental emotion elicitations. Psychological Bulletin, 137(5), 834-855.
doi: 10.1037/a0024244 URL pmid: 21766999 |
[32] |
Lerner, J. S., Gonzalez, R. M., Small, D. A., & Fischhoff, B. (2003). Effects of fear and anger on perceived risks of terrorism: A national field experiment. Psychological Science, 14(2), 144-150.
doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01433 URL pmid: 12661676 |
[33] | Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2000). Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion specific influences on judgement and choice. Cognition and Emotion, 14(4), 473-494. |
[34] |
Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 146-159.
doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.81.1.146 URL pmid: 11474720 |
[35] |
Lerner, J. S., Li, Y., & Weber, E. U. (2013). The financial costs of sadness. Psychological Science, 24(1), 72-79.
doi: 10.1177/0956797612450302 URL pmid: 23150274 |
[36] | Lerner, J. S., & Tiedens, L. Z. (2006). Portrait of the angry decision maker: How appraisal tendencies shape anger's influence on cognition. Journal of Behavioral Decision-making, 19(2), 115-137. |
[37] | Li, J. Z., Li, S., & Liu. H. (2011). How has the Wenchuan Earthquake influenced people's intertemporal choices? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 41(11), 2739-2752. |
[38] | Li, X. M., & Xie, J. (2012). The influence mechanism of incidental emotions on choice deferral. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 44(12), 1641-1650. |
[39] | Liberman, N., Sagristano, M. D., & Trope, Y. (2002). The effect of temporal distance on level of mental construal. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38(6), 523-534. |
[40] | Liu, L., Su, T., Peng, J., Guo, Y. Q., & Feng, T. Y. (2014). The cognitive and neural mechanism of the delay discounting: From the trait and state perspectives. Advances in Psychological Science, 22(7), 1047-1061. |
[41] | Liu, L., & Suo, T. (2018). The Influence of emotion on delayed discounting: Status quo, mechanism and prospect. Journal of Psychological Science, 41(3), 553-558. |
[42] |
Luhmann, C. C., Chun, M. M., Yi, D. J., Lee, D., & Wang, X. (2008). Neural dissociation of delay and uncertainty in intertemporal choice. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28(53), 14459-14466.
URL pmid: 19118180 |
[43] |
Luo, S., Ainslie, G., & Monterosso, J. (2014). The behavioral and neural effect of emotional primes on intertemporal decisions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(3), 283-291.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nss132 URL pmid: 23160811 |
[44] |
Mitchell, S. H., & Wilson, V. B. (2010). The subjective value of delayed and probabilistic outcomes: Outcome size matters for gains but not for losses. Behavioural Processes, 83(1), 36-40.
doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2009.09.003 URL pmid: 19766702 |
[45] | Read, D., Olivola, C. Y., & Hardisty, D. J. (2017). The value of nothing: Asymmetric attention to opportunity costs drives intertemporal decision-making. Management Science, 63(12), 4277-4297. |
[46] | Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(3), 513-523. |
[47] |
She, S., Ma, C., & Wu, D. D. (2010). General probability-time tradeoff and intertemporal risk-value model. Risk Analysis, 30(3), 421-431.
doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2009.01344.x URL pmid: 20136745 |
[48] | She, S. X., Chen, Y., Chen, J., & Yang, F. (2017). The effect of fear on intertemporal choice: An experiment based on recalling emotion. Psychological Exploration, 37(6), 543-548. |
[49] | She, S. X., Zheng, X. W., Zhou, J., & Yang, S. S. (2016). Does fear increase impatience in inter — temporal choice? Evidence from an experiment. Psychological Exploration, 36(1), 25-30. |
[50] | Small, D. A., & Lerner, J. S. (2008). Emotional policy: Personal sadness and anger shape judgments about a welfare case. Political Psychology, 29(2), 149-168. |
[51] |
Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(4), 813-838.
URL pmid: 3886875 |
[52] | Solomon, R. C. (1990). A passion for justice. New York: Addison-Wesley. |
[53] |
Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., & Fong, G. T. (2005). Establishing a causal chain: Why experiments are often more effective than mediational analyses in examining psychological processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(6), 845-851.
doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.6.845 URL pmid: 16393019 |
[54] | Suo, T., Gu, B. B., Zhang, C. Y., & Liu, L. (2018). The effect of ego depletion on different self-control-trait individual in intertemporal choices. Journal of Psychological Science, 41(6), 1443-1449. |
[55] | Tavris, C. (1989). Anger: The misunderstood emotion. New York: Touchstone. |
[56] |
Tiedens, L. Z. (2001). Anger and advancement versus sadness and subjugation: The effect of negative emotion expressions on social status conferral. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(1), 86-94.
URL pmid: 11195894 |
[57] |
Tiedens, L. Z., & Linton, S. (2001). Judgment under emotional certainty and uncertainty: The effects of specific emotions on information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 973-988.
doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.81.6.973 URL pmid: 11761319 |
[58] | Todd, A. R., Forstmann, M., Burgmer, P., Brooks, A. W., & Galinsky, A. D. (2015). Anxious and egocentric: How specific emotions influence perspective taking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(2), 374-391. |
[59] | Wang, P., & Liu, Y. F. (2009). The effect of mood on intertemporal choice. Journal of Psychological Science, 32(6), 1318-1320. |
[60] | Whitson, J. A., Galinsky, A. D., & Kay, A. (2015). The emotional roots of conspiratorial perceptions, system justification, and belief in the paranormal. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 56, 89-95. |
[61] | Wiltermuth, S. S., & Tiedens, L. Z. (2011). Incidental anger and the desire to evaluate. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 116(1), 55-65. |
[62] |
Winterich, K. P., Han, S., & Lerner, J. S. (2010). Now that I’m sad, it’s hard to be mad: The role of cognitive appraisals in emotional blunting. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(11), 1467-1483.
doi: 10.1177/0146167210384710 URL pmid: 20876386 |
[63] |
Xu, L. J., Liang, Z. Y., Wang, K., Li, S., & Jiang, T. Z. (2009). Neural mechanism of intertemporal choice: From discounting future gains to future losses. Brain Research, 1261, 65-74.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.12.061 URL pmid: 19185567 |
[64] | Yang, Z. N., Gu, Z. B., Wang, D. J., Tan, X. Y., & Wang, X. M. (2017). The effect of anger and sadness on prosocial decision-making: The role of the interpersonal attribution of responsibility. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 49(3), 393-403. |
[65] | Zauberman, G., Kim, B. K., Malkoc, S. A., & Bettman, J. R. (2009). Discounting time and time discounting: Subjective time perception and intertemporal preferences. Journal of Marketing Research, 46(4), 543-556. |
[66] | Zhao, J. L., Kirwen, N., Johnson, J., & Vigo, R. (2017). Anger and intertemporal choice: The behavioral approach system and the interactive effects of trait and state anger. Personality and Individual Differences, 110, 60-64. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||