心理学报 ›› 2025, Vol. 57 ›› Issue (6): 1070-1082.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2025.1070 cstr: 32110.14.2025.1070
收稿日期:
2024-07-19
发布日期:
2025-04-15
出版日期:
2025-06-25
通讯作者:
崔芳, E-mail: cuifang0826@gmail.com基金资助:
SONG Ru, WU Jun, LIU Caixia, LIU Jie, CUI Fang()
Received:
2024-07-19
Online:
2025-04-15
Published:
2025-06-25
摘要:
在道德情境中, 个体因不同的先前经历而对同一道德事件的理解和判断常常存在差异, 难以实现真正的“旁观者清”。本研究通过激活不同道德角色的视角, 运用事件相关电位技术探讨先前经历如何影响视角选择, 从而调节个体的道德判断及其神经基础。研究结果显示, 启动接受者视角使道德判断更加严苛, 而启动决策者视角则使判断更为宽松。此外, 随着决策者从不道德决策中获得的收益降低, 不同视角对道德判断的调节作用逐渐减弱。在神经层面, 启动不同道德角色视角影响了道德判断的早期加工和情绪唤起, 具体表现为决策者视角诱发了更大的N1和P2成分, 而接受者视角则引发了更大的与预期违背相关的FRN成分。这表明, 先前的道德经历显著影响个体在作为旁观者时的道德判断偏好, 主要通过调节对他人道德决策的早期加工过程来实现。
中图分类号:
宋茹, 吴珺, 刘彩霞, 刘洁, 崔芳. (2025). 旁观者清?道德情景中不同角色视角的启动调节第三方道德判断. 心理学报, 57(6), 1070-1082.
SONG Ru, WU Jun, LIU Caixia, LIU Jie, CUI Fang. (2025). The influence of moral role-based perspectives on moral judgments of third-party bystanders. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 57(6), 1070-1082.
变量 | 决策者视角(n = 30) | 接受者视角(n = 30) | 控制组(n = 30) | 差异检验 |
---|---|---|---|---|
年龄(岁) | 21.27 ± 2.08 | 20.43 ± 1.77 | 20.03 ± 1.73 | F = 3.4, p = 0.05 |
性别(女/男) | 15/15 | 16/14 | 15/15 | |
IRI-观点采择 | 17.37 ± 3.53 | 18 ± 3.26 | 17.93 ± 3.15 | F = 0.33, p = 0.72 |
IRI-想象 | 22.2 ± 4.48 | 22.53 ±3.73 | 20.87 ±4.18 | F = 1.36, p = 0.26 |
IRI-共情关切 | 22.33 ± 2.96 | 22.03 ± 3.05 | 21.43 ± 3.11 | F = 0.68, p = 0.51 |
IRI-个人痛苦 | 16.27 ± 2.74 | 16.2 ± 4.36 | 15.37 ± 3.56 | F = 0.58, p = 0.56 |
DMSQ-移情内疚 | 29.83 ± 5.12 | 29.97 ± 5.82 | 29.63 ± 5.08 | F = 0.03, p = 0.97 |
DMSQ-惩罚倾向 | 20.53 ± 4.95 | 22.58 ± 5.83 | 20.63 ± 5.06 | F = 0.75, p = 0.47 |
DMSQ-移情烦扰 | 22.4 ± 4.34 | 21.03 ± 5.27 | 22.43 ± 5.11 | F = 0.79, p = 0.46 |
DMSQ-觉察频率 | 17.4 ± 4.56 | 18.33 ± 4.98 | 16.87 ± 4.14 | F = 0.79, p = 0.46 |
DMSQ-共感想象 | 12.47 ± 5.19 | 12.73 ± 3.92 | 13.03 ± 3.58 | F = 0.13, p = 0.88 |
表1 三组被试的人口统计学特征(M ± SD)
变量 | 决策者视角(n = 30) | 接受者视角(n = 30) | 控制组(n = 30) | 差异检验 |
---|---|---|---|---|
年龄(岁) | 21.27 ± 2.08 | 20.43 ± 1.77 | 20.03 ± 1.73 | F = 3.4, p = 0.05 |
性别(女/男) | 15/15 | 16/14 | 15/15 | |
IRI-观点采择 | 17.37 ± 3.53 | 18 ± 3.26 | 17.93 ± 3.15 | F = 0.33, p = 0.72 |
IRI-想象 | 22.2 ± 4.48 | 22.53 ±3.73 | 20.87 ±4.18 | F = 1.36, p = 0.26 |
IRI-共情关切 | 22.33 ± 2.96 | 22.03 ± 3.05 | 21.43 ± 3.11 | F = 0.68, p = 0.51 |
IRI-个人痛苦 | 16.27 ± 2.74 | 16.2 ± 4.36 | 15.37 ± 3.56 | F = 0.58, p = 0.56 |
DMSQ-移情内疚 | 29.83 ± 5.12 | 29.97 ± 5.82 | 29.63 ± 5.08 | F = 0.03, p = 0.97 |
DMSQ-惩罚倾向 | 20.53 ± 4.95 | 22.58 ± 5.83 | 20.63 ± 5.06 | F = 0.75, p = 0.47 |
DMSQ-移情烦扰 | 22.4 ± 4.34 | 21.03 ± 5.27 | 22.43 ± 5.11 | F = 0.79, p = 0.46 |
DMSQ-觉察频率 | 17.4 ± 4.56 | 18.33 ± 4.98 | 16.87 ± 4.14 | F = 0.79, p = 0.46 |
DMSQ-共感想象 | 12.47 ± 5.19 | 12.73 ± 3.92 | 13.03 ± 3.58 | F = 0.13, p = 0.88 |
变量 | 决策者视角(n = 28) | 接受者视角(n = 26) | 差异检验 |
---|---|---|---|
年龄(岁) | 21.18 ± 2.21 | 20.57 ± 2.02 | F = 1.08, p = 0.30 |
性别(女/男) | 13/15 | 13/13 | |
IRI-观点采择 | 17.89 ± 3.09 | 17.65 ± 3.60 | F = 0.07, p = 0.79 |
IRI-想象 | 20.96 ± 4.33 | 21.23 ± 5.23 | F = 0.04, p = 0.84 |
IRI-共情关切 | 21.21 ± 3.66 | 20.69 ± 3.75 | F = 0.27, p = 0.61 |
IRI-个人痛苦 | 14.86 ± 3.93 | 15.42 ± 4.73 | F = 0.23, p = 0.63 |
DMSQ-移情内疚 | 27.21 ± 6.80 | 27.62 ± 5.56 | F = 0.06, p = 0.81 |
DMSQ-惩罚倾向 | 19.43 ± 5.98 | 19.81 ± 5.86 | F = 0.06, p = 0.81 |
DMSQ-移情烦扰 | 21.86 ± 5.49 | 21.46 ± 5.28 | F = 0.07, p = 0.79 |
DMSQ-觉察频率 | 17.11 ± 4.93 | 16.77 ± 3.48 | F = 0.08, p = 0.77 |
DMSQ-共感想象 | 12.82 ± 3.50 | 12.27 ± 3.71 | F = 0.32, p = 0.57 |
表2 两组被试的人口统计学特征(M ± SD)
变量 | 决策者视角(n = 28) | 接受者视角(n = 26) | 差异检验 |
---|---|---|---|
年龄(岁) | 21.18 ± 2.21 | 20.57 ± 2.02 | F = 1.08, p = 0.30 |
性别(女/男) | 13/15 | 13/13 | |
IRI-观点采择 | 17.89 ± 3.09 | 17.65 ± 3.60 | F = 0.07, p = 0.79 |
IRI-想象 | 20.96 ± 4.33 | 21.23 ± 5.23 | F = 0.04, p = 0.84 |
IRI-共情关切 | 21.21 ± 3.66 | 20.69 ± 3.75 | F = 0.27, p = 0.61 |
IRI-个人痛苦 | 14.86 ± 3.93 | 15.42 ± 4.73 | F = 0.23, p = 0.63 |
DMSQ-移情内疚 | 27.21 ± 6.80 | 27.62 ± 5.56 | F = 0.06, p = 0.81 |
DMSQ-惩罚倾向 | 19.43 ± 5.98 | 19.81 ± 5.86 | F = 0.06, p = 0.81 |
DMSQ-移情烦扰 | 21.86 ± 5.49 | 21.46 ± 5.28 | F = 0.07, p = 0.79 |
DMSQ-觉察频率 | 17.11 ± 4.93 | 16.77 ± 3.48 | F = 0.08, p = 0.77 |
DMSQ-共感想象 | 12.82 ± 3.50 | 12.27 ± 3.71 | F = 0.32, p = 0.57 |
[31] | Liu Z., Zhang H., Wei L., & Ge X. (2022). Moral Chameleons: The positive association between materialism and self-interest-triggered moral flexibility. Journal of Research in Personality, 100, 104268. |
[32] |
Ma Q., Hu Y., Jiang S., & Meng L. (2015). The undermining effect of facial attractiveness on brain responses to fairness in the Ultimatum Game: An ERP study. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 9, 77.
doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00077 pmid: 25805967 |
[33] | Malle B. (2021). Moral judgments. Annual Review of Psychology, 72, 293-318. |
[34] |
Miltner W. H. R., Braun C. H., & Coles M. G. H. (1997). Event-related brain potentials following incorrect feedback in a time-estimation task: Evidence for a “generic” neural system for error detection. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(6), 788-798.
doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.6.788 pmid: 23964600 |
[35] |
Pfattheicher S., Sassenrath C., & Keller J. (2019). Compassion magnifies third-party punishment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 117(1), 124-41.
doi: 10.1037/pspi0000165 pmid: 30945902 |
[36] |
Pletti C., Decety J., & Paulus M. (2019). Moral identity relates to the neural processing of third-party moral behavior. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 14(4), 435-445.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsz016 pmid: 30855686 |
[37] | Rong X., Sun B. H., Huang X. Z., Cai M. Y., & Li W. J. (2010). Reliabilities and validities of Chinese version of interpersonal reactivity index. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 18(2), 158-160. |
[戎幸, 孙炳海, 黄小忠, 蔡旻颖, 李伟健. (2010). 人际反应指数量表的信度和效度研究. 中国临床心理学杂志, 18(2), 158-160.] | |
[38] |
Sambrook T. D., & Goslin J. (2015). A neural reward prediction error revealed by a meta-analysis of ERPs using great grand averages. Psychological Bulletin, 141(1), 213-235.
doi: 10.1037/bul0000006 pmid: 25495239 |
[39] | Sarlo M., Lotto L., Manfrinati A., Rumiati R., Gallicchio G., & Palomba D. (2012). Temporal dynamics of cognitive- emotional interplay in moral decision-making. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 1018-1029. |
[40] | Schein C., & Gray K. (2017). The theory of dyadic morality: Reinventing moral judgment by redefining harm. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 22(1), 32-70. |
[41] | Volz L. J., Welborn B. L., Gobel M. S., Gazzaniga M. S., & Grafton S. T. (2017). Harm to self outweighs benefit to others in moral decision making. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(30), 7963-7968. |
[1] |
Adams G., & Inesi E. (2016). Impediments to forgiveness: Victim and transgressor attributions of intent and guilt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111 (6), 866-881.
pmid: 27537273 |
[2] | Bandura A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Journal of Applied Psychology, 12(1), 169. |
[3] | Bartels D. M., Bauman C. W., Cushman F. A., Pizarro D. A., & McGraw A. P. (2015). Moral judgment and decision making. In G. Keren & G. Wu (Eds.), The Wiley Blackwell handbook of judgment and decision making (pp. 478-515). Wiley-Blackwell. |
[4] |
Bellebaum C., Polezzi D., & Daum I. (2010). It is less than you expected: The feedback-related negativity reflects violations of reward magnitude expectations. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3343-3350.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.023 pmid: 20655319 |
[5] | Bocian K., Cichocka A., & Wojciszke B. (2021). Moral tribalism: Moral judgments of actions supporting ingroup interests depend on collective narcissism. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 93, 104098. |
[6] | Cameron C. D., Conway P., & Scheffer J. A. (2022). Empathy regulation, prosociality, and moral judgment. Current Opinion in Psychology, 44, 188-195. |
[7] |
Chen A., Xu P., Wang Q., Luo Y., Yuan J., Yao D., & Li H. (2008). The timing of cognitive control in partially incongruent categorization. Human Brain Mapping, 29, 1028-1039.
pmid: 17894393 |
[8] |
Chen J., Yuan J., Feng T., Chen A., Gu B., & Li H. (2011). Temporal features of the degree effect in self-relevance: Neural correlates. Biological Psychology, 87(2), 290-295.
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.03.012 pmid: 21470572 |
[9] | Chugh D., Bazerman M. H., & Banaji M. R. (2005). Bounded ethicality as a psychological barrier to recognizing conflicts of interest. In D. A. Moore, D. M. Cain, G. Loewenstein, & M. H. Bazerman (Eds.), Conflicts of interest: Challenges and solutions in business, law, medicine, and public policy (pp. 74-95). Cambridge University Press. |
[10] |
Cowell J., & Decety J. (2015). Precursors to morality in development as a complex interplay between neural, socioenvironmental, and behavioral facets. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(41), 12657-12662.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1508832112 pmid: 26324885 |
[11] | Cushman F., Young L., & Hauser M. (2006). The role of conscious reasoning and intuitions in moral judgment: Testing three principles of harm. Cognition, 104(2), 293-304. |
[12] | Davis M. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85. |
[13] | DeScioli P., Massenkoff M., Shaw A., Petersen M. B., & Kurzban R. (2014). Equity or equality? Moral judgments follow the money. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1797), 20142112. |
[14] | Faul F., Erdfelder E., Lang A.-G., & Buchner A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavior, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191. |
[15] | Fehr E., Bernhard H., & Rockenbach B. (2008). Egalitarianism in young children. Nature, 454, 1079-1083. |
[16] | Frisch L., Kneer M., Krueger J., & Ullrich J. (2021). The effect of outcome severity on moral judgement and interpersonal goals of perpetrators, victims, and bystanders. European Journal of Social Psychology, 51(7), 1158-1171. |
[17] | Gan T., Zhang Y., Zhang L., & Gu R. (2022). Neural sensitivity to helping outcome predicts helping decision in real life. Neuropsychologia, 173, 108291. |
[18] |
Gehring W., & Willoughby A. (2002). The medial frontal cortex and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science, 295(5563), 2279-2282.
pmid: 11910116 |
[19] |
Geng X. W., Fang, J. R., Han, Y. F., Li, Z. Q., Zhao M., & Yang Y. (2019). The influence of moral relativism and disgust on moral intuitive judgment. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 51(4), 517-526.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2019.00517 |
[耿晓伟, 房津如, 韩彦芳, 李中权, 赵蜜, 杨烨. (2019). 道德相对主义和厌恶情绪对道德直觉判断的影响. 心理学报, 4(51), 517-526.] | |
[20] | Gold N., Pulford B. D., & Colman A. M. (2015). Do as I say, don’t do as I do: Differences in moral judgments do not translate into differences in decisions in real-life trolley problems. Journal of Economic Psychology, 40, 50-61. |
[21] | Greene J. (2003). From neural “is” to moral “ought”: What are the moral implications of neuroscientific moral psychology? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(10), 846-849. |
[22] | Gui D.-Y., Gan T., & Liu C. (2015). Neural evidence for moral intuition and the temporal dynamics of interactions between emotional processes and moral cognition. Social Neuroscience, 11(4), 380-394. |
[23] |
Haidt J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814-834.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.4.814 pmid: 11699120 |
[42] | Wu Y., Leliveld M. C., & Zhou X. (2011). Social distance modulates recipient’s fairness consideration in the dictator game: An ERP study. Biological Psychology, 88(2), 253-262. |
[43] |
Yoder K. J., & Decety J. (2014). Spatiotemporal neural dynamics of moral judgment: A high-density ERP study. Neuropsychologia, 60, 39-45.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.05.022 pmid: 24905282 |
[44] |
You T. T., Zhang L. P., Qi G. M., & Long C. Q. (2023). Opportunity (in)equity affects outcome evaluation at an early cognitive stage. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 55(12), 1997-2012.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01997 |
[尤婷婷, 张利平, 祁国梅, 龙长权. (2023). 机会公平在早期加工阶段影响个体实际结果的评价. 心理学报, 55(12), 1997-2012.]
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2023.01997 |
|
[45] | Zheng X. J., & Cen G. Z. (2008). A research on college students' dispositional moral sensitivity structure. Journal of Psychological Science, 5, 1026-1030. |
[郑信军, 岑国桢. (2008). 大学生倾向性道德敏感的结构研究. 心理科学, 5, 1026-1030.] | |
[46] | Zhong C., Ku G., Lount R., & Murnighan J. (2010). Compensatory ethics. Journal of Business Ethics, 92(3), 323-339. |
[47] |
Zhu R., Wu H., Xu Z., Tang H., Shen X., Mai X., & Liu C. (2019). Early distinction between shame and guilt processing in an interpersonal context. Social Neuroscience, 14(1), 53-66.
doi: 10.1080/17470919.2017.1391119 pmid: 29016239 |
[24] |
Hajcak G., Moser J. S., Holroyd C. B., & Simons R. F. (2006). The feedback-related negativity reflects the binary evaluation of good versus bad outcomes. Biological Psychology, 71(2), 148-154.
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.04.001 pmid: 16005561 |
[25] | He Y., Gu R., Deng G., Lin Y., Gan T., Cui F., … Luo Y.-J. (2024). Psychological and brain responses to artificial intelligence’s violation of community ethics. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 27(8), 562-570. |
[26] |
Holroyd C. B., & Coles M. G. H. (2002). The neural basis of human error processing: Reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 109(4), 679-709.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.109.4.679 pmid: 12374324 |
[27] | Hu X., & Mai X. (2021). Social value orientation modulates fairness processing during social decision-making: Evidence from behavior and brain potentials. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16(7), 670-682. |
[28] | Huang Y., Hu P., & Deng H. (2023). Empathic concern induction modulates behavioral ratings and neural responses to harm-related moral judgment: An event-related potentials study. Behavioural Brain Research, 446, 114397. |
[29] |
Kaltwasser L., Hildebrandt A., Wilhelm O., & Sommer W. (2016). Behavioral and neuronal determinants of negative reciprocity in the ultimatum game. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(10), 1608-1617.
doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw069 pmid: 27261490 |
[30] | Liu M., Zhou J., Liu Y., & Liu S. (2022). The impact of social comparison and (un)fairness on upstream indirect reciprocity: Evidence from ERP. Neuropsychologia, 177, 108398. |
[1] | 范伟, 杨颖, 郭希亚, 林卓铭, 钟毅平. 不道德行为中道德标准对自我欺骗的影响: 来自ERP的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(8): 1414-1436. |
[2] | 焦丽颖, 李昌锦, 陈圳, 许恒彬, 许燕. 当AI“具有”人格:善恶人格角色对大语言模型道德判断的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(6): 929-946. |
[3] | 谢晓玲, 潘文谊, 张纯纯, 林静远, 李红. 连续反馈影响主导感的心理与脑电机制[J]. 心理学报, 2025, 57(3): 380-397. |
[4] | 石荣, 刘昌, 唐慧琳, 郝俊懿, 沈汪兵. 自发的善行:加工模式和情境紧急性影响亲社会行为[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(9): 1239-1251. |
[5] | 刘梦颖, 蒋婧怡, 杨依琳, 江波, 黄建平. 古典美还是表现美:摆盘美学影响健康饮食决策的计算与神经机制[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(8): 1061-1075. |
[6] | 豆艳, 李晶. 多对象情境中一级视觉视角采择的自发性表现[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(4): 421-434. |
[7] | 张锋, 皮瑜, 李小保. 个体与集体时间自我评价:来自行为和ERP的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(4): 447-457. |
[8] | 王婷, 赵梁佛, 杨金朋, 张丹丹, 雷震. 分配意图与上行间接互惠:来自行为与ERP的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(12): 1788-1799. |
[9] | 吴珺, 李晚晨, 姚晓欢, 刘洁, 崔芳. 友善重要, 还是公平重要?亲社会性与公平性调节复杂道德判断[J]. 心理学报, 2024, 56(11): 1541-1555. |
[10] | 张文芸, 卓诗维, 郑倩倩, 关颖琳, 彭微微. 自闭特质对疼痛共情的影响:疼痛负性情绪和认知的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(9): 1501-1517. |
[11] | 钟毅平, 牛娜娜, 范伟, 任梦梦, 李梅. 动作自主性与社会距离对主动控制感的影响:来自行为与ERPs的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(12): 1932-1948. |
[12] | 李梅, 李琎, 张冠斐, 钟毅平, 李红. 承诺水平与社会距离对信任投资的影响:来自行为与ERPs的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(11): 1859-1871. |
[13] | 覃慧怡, 丁丽洪, 段威, 雷旭. 脑电的重测信度:在多项静息态和任务态实验中的对比[J]. 心理学报, 2023, 55(10): 1587-1596. |
[14] | 占友龙, 肖啸, 谭千保, 李琎, 钟毅平. 声誉关注与社会距离对伤害困境中道德决策的影响:来自行为与ERPs的证据[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(6): 613-627. |
[15] | 李建花, 解佳佳, 庄锦英. 生理周期对情景记忆的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2022, 54(5): 466-480. |
阅读次数 | ||||||
全文 |
|
|||||
摘要 |
|
|||||