ISSN 0439-755X
CN 11-1911/B
主办:中国心理学会
   中国科学院心理研究所
出版:科学出版社

心理学报, 2018, 50(6): 622-636 doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.00622

研究报告

虚拟仿真场景中威胁性视觉刺激搜索的注意偏向效应 *

袁小钧, 崔晓霞, 曹正操, 阚红, 王晓, 汪亚珉,

首都师范大学心理学院, 北京市“学习与认知”重点实验室, 北京 100048

Attentional bias towards threatening visual stimuli in a virtual reality-based visual search task

YUAN Xiaojun, CUI Xiaoxia, CAO Zhengcao, KAN Hong, WANG Xiao, WANG Yamin,

Beijing Key Laboratory of “Learning & Cognition”, School of Psychology, Capital Normal University, Beijing 100048, China

通讯作者: 汪亚珉, E-mail:wangym@cnu.edu.cn

收稿日期: 2016-11-28   网络出版日期: 2018-06-01

基金资助: 北京市教育委员会面上项目.  KM201610028018
北京市教育委员会市属高校创新能力提升计划资助项目资助.  TJSH20161002801

Received: 2016-11-28   Online: 2018-06-01

Fund supported: .  KM201610028018
.  TJSH20161002801

摘要

自Öhman, Flykt和Esteves (2001)完成威胁性刺激引起注意偏向的经典实验后, 有不少实验重复了这一发现。进化心理学家据此将人对蛇与蜘蛛的注意偏向反应解释为早期人类适应野外环境的结果。然而已有实验中既没有野外环境, 也没有视觉搜索时的光流动态信息, 实验的生态效度存疑。本研究利用新近发展的虚拟现实技术, 模拟早期人类生存的丛林环境, 让被试以完全浸入式的方式来搜索丛林草地中的威胁性刺激(蛇、蜘蛛), 结果发现被试搜索威胁性刺激确实比非威胁性刺激(蘑菇、花、松鼠、蝉)要快。并且, 三维空间数据显示, 被试确认威胁性刺激的空间距离要显著远于非威胁性刺激。这是首次从时间与空间两个维度上揭示了人对威胁性刺激搜索的注意偏向加工。实验结果为威胁性刺激加工的进化心理学解释提供了新的事实依据。此外, 利用虚拟现实技术提高实验的生态效度的尝试也获得了初步进展。

关键词: 威胁性刺激 ; 注意偏向 ; 视觉搜索 ; 虚拟现实 ; 生态效度

Abstract

Ever since Öhman, Flykt and Esteves published their classic study in 2001, many researchers have replicated their findings regarding attentional bias to threatening stimuli in visual perception research. Based on these findings, psychologists proposed a promising theory called predatory fear, in which the attentional bias to threatening animals is interpreted as evolutionarily adaptive behavior of early mammals and the ancestors of modern humans. However, from an evolutionary perspective, the lack of ecological validity of existing experiments inevitably attenuated the interpretation. The present study aimed to fill the gaps by repeating the classic work in a virtual reality environment.

A virtual reality grove was created with the Virtools virtual reality engine, in which jungles, trees, flowers, and weeds were arranged in the form of a wild grass field. The virtual reality grove was presented with an Oculus Rift DK 2 helmet. Forty participants were instructed to navigate along a path in the grove and search for threatening or non-threatening target stimuli. 3D models of a snake, a spider, a flower, a mushroom, a cicada, and a squirrel were used as stimuli in the search task, among which snake and spider were considered threatening stimuli. All the stimuli were shown in yellow and were assessed by twenty participants not included in the forty participants in search task to ensure they were of similar salience.

To examine attentional bias to threatening stimuli, two experiments were conducted in the same visual search task as reported by Öhman et al. In Experiment 1, as in Öhman et al., the snake or the spider was selected as a target stimulus, and thirteen copies of the flower or the mushroom were used as distracting stimuli, or other combinations of these. Twenty participants were individually presented with the virtual grove and instructed to passively wander along the path in the jungle to search for target stimuli. A fixed camera was set at a uniform speed to simulate the navigation in visual search task. Given that searching for animals took less time than searching for plants ( Soares et al, 2009), flowers and mushrooms were replaced with cicadas and squirrels in Experiment 2. The other twenty participants repeated the experiment procedure. In addition to response time (RT), response distance (RD) was also computed as a compensatory index.

In Experiment 1, the results of RTs revealed that the searching for threatening stimuli (snake and spider) is faster than searching for non-threatening stimuli (mushroom, flower). The RD values showed that participants found the threatening stimuli when they were farther away than the non-threatening stimuli. In Experiment 2, the same results were found even when the distracting stimuli were all animals. The RTs and RDs both confirmed that participants were better at finding snakes and spiders than finding flowers, mushrooms, cicadas, and squirrels.

The total results supported the hypothesis of predatory fear was relatively soundly and the attentional bias to threatening animals, especially snake and spider, was found to be likely to be caused by predatory fear as part of human cognition. These findings provide new evidence for the hypothesis of predatory fear from an evolutionary perspective. In addition, virtual reality was proven to be a suitable technique for assessing the ecological validity of psychological experiments.

Keywords: threatening stimuli ; attentional bias ; visual search ; virtual reality ; ecological validity ; response distance

PDF (5374KB) 元数据 多维度评价 相关文章 导出 EndNote| Ris| Bibtex  收藏本文

本文引用格式

袁小钧, 崔晓霞, 曹正操, 阚红, 王晓, 汪亚珉. 虚拟仿真场景中威胁性视觉刺激搜索的注意偏向效应 *. 心理学报[J], 2018, 50(6): 622-636 doi:10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.00622

YUAN Xiaojun, CUI Xiaoxia, CAO Zhengcao, KAN Hong, WANG Xiao, WANG Yamin. Attentional bias towards threatening visual stimuli in a virtual reality-based visual search task. Acta Psychologica Sinica[J], 2018, 50(6): 622-636 doi:10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.00622

1 引言

威胁性刺激引起注意偏向一直备受心理学家关注。根据注意的认知资源理论, 认知系统内存在着一个负责资源分配的机制, 这一机制是受意识控制的, 可以把认知资源灵活地分配到重要的刺激上。同时, 人类的认知系统内还存在着一个评价环境重要性的系统, 重要的刺激会优先进入到这个系统, 获得进一步加工(彭聃龄, 2012)。因此, 这些占有认知资源比较多、优先加工的刺激会引起注意偏向。

进化心理学认为, 对威胁性刺激的偏向加工是进化而来的。人类已经进化了对远古危险物的害怕倾向, 形成了一些生存适应器, 这些适应器会处理危及我们祖先生命的各种问题。当我们在一个开阔的地方四处张望时, 我们的信息加工机制会让我们觉察到“暗藏的敌人”, 我们的注意会自动地偏向这些危险物。与现代环境中的危险物相比, 人们更容易对远古环境中就存在的危险物表现出威胁反应。比如说, 人大多害怕蛇, 但很少害怕汽车, 尽管汽车每年夺走很多人的生命。对此, 进化心理学的解释是蛇是人类进化过程中一直存在的危险物(Buss, 1999)。这种威胁不仅反应在情绪上, 还反映在注意偏向上。因此, 威胁性刺激更容易引起注意偏向。

在已有的威胁性刺激导致注意偏向的研究中, 最常用的研究范式主要有5种:视觉搜索范式(Öhman, Flykt, & Esteves, 2001)、情绪Stroop范式(Janczyk, Ausgst, & Kunde, 2014)、线索-靶子范式(Stormark, Nordly, & Hugdahl, 1995)、点探测范式(Mogg, Bradley, & Hallowell, 1994)、情绪条件反射范式(Stormark & Hugdahl, 1996), 其中视觉搜索范式是使用最为广泛的一种范式。在早期Öhman和Flykt等(2001)的威胁性刺激搜索实验中, 研究者以蛇、蜘蛛、蘑菇、花的图片作为实验材料, 要求被试在一些与威胁无关的图片(花、蘑菇)中搜索与威胁相关的图片(蛇、蜘蛛), 或在一些与威胁相关的图片(蛇、蜘蛛)中搜索与威胁无关的图片(花、蘑菇)。结果发现在与威胁无关物体中搜索与威胁相关的目标的速度要显著快于在与威胁相关的物体中搜索与威胁无关的目标, 并且不受目标物所在位置以及分心物数目的影响。此后, Mogg和Bradley (2006)使用视觉搜索范式研究了不同高低水平蜘蛛威胁在不同暴露时间下的注意偏向。实验时给被试成对呈现蜘蛛和猫的图片, 呈现时间分3种, 分别为200 ms、500 ms、2000 ms。研究结果发现, 当呈现时间为200 ms时, 高水平蜘蛛威胁组对蜘蛛的注意偏向比低水平组要大。随着暴露时间的增加, 高水平组注意偏向降低。在更长暴露时间时, 这种注意偏向差异则不再显著。这一研究表明, 威胁性刺激对注意的捕获效应主要发生在早期的快速视觉信息加工阶段。王福兴、李文静、颜志强、段朝辉和李卉(2015)使用眼动技术来研究幼儿对威胁性刺激物——蛇的注意偏向, 结果也发现, 无论是幼儿还是成人对蛇的首次注视时间都比注视花朵的时间更短。LoBue (2010)使用视觉搜索范式研究了儿童和成人对蜘蛛的注意偏向。该实验要求被试在蘑菇或蟑螂的图片中搜索蜘蛛, 或者是反过来进行搜索。研究结果表明, 无论是儿童还是成人, 搜索蜘蛛的速度都比其他两种要快, 并且搜索蘑菇和蟑螂差异不显著, 后续的电生理研究也得到了类似的结果(LoBue & Rakison, 2013; Thrasher & LoBue, 2016)。Dudeney, Sharpe和Hunt (2015)对焦虑儿童的研究也发现, 与正常儿童相比, 焦虑儿童在注意搜索中能更快地搜索到威胁性刺激。

除了蛇与蜘蛛外, 负性情绪面孔也常被用作威胁性刺激, 研究者们以面孔为刺激材料的研究得到了同样的结论。Hansen和Hansen (1988)最早以面孔图片为刺激材料来研究情绪刺激的注意偏向。该实验要求被试在一个2 × 2的矩阵中搜索面孔, 结果表明, 被试在快乐面孔中搜索愤怒面孔较快, 而在愤怒面孔里搜索快乐面孔较慢。他认为愤怒面孔属于威胁性刺激, 这种刺激能够捕获注意, 因而在快乐面孔中的愤怒面孔更容易找到, 而快乐面孔很容易被忽视。他们把这种现象称为凸显效应(pop- out effect)。Öhman, Lundqvist和Esteves (2001)使用与Hansen不同的面孔图片来研究情绪刺激对注意的影响。Öhman和Lundqvist等的实验采用的是图示面孔, 而不是真实面孔。他认为在Hansen的实验中, 被试之所以搜索愤怒面孔快, 是因为愤怒面孔的异质性更大, 而图示面孔是从真实面孔中抽取出来的, 同质性更高。实验结果表明, 在快乐面孔中搜索愤怒面孔要显著地快于在愤怒面孔中搜索快乐面孔, 这和Hansen的实验结果是一致的。也有一些对焦虑个体的研究, 得到了类似的结果。与搜索微笑面孔相比, 高特质焦虑个体搜索愤怒面孔要更快, 而且识别愤怒面孔也快于微笑面孔(Matsumoto, 2010)。此外, 有关威胁性刺激的认知神经机制研究也表明, 威胁性信息导致注意偏向主要发生在视觉信息加工的早期阶段, 这一阶段的加工是一种自动化的加工, 主要依赖于低频轮廓信息(LeDoux, 2000; Vuilleumier, Armony, Driver, & Dolan, 2003; van Le et al., 2013)。

现有研究在不同被试身上以不同的刺激材料重复出了同样的结果, 对这一结果, 研究者们采用环境适应这一进化心理学的观点予以解释(Öhman & Mineka, 2001; Öhman, 2009; Öhman, Soares, Juth, Lindström, & Esteves, 2012; Soares, Lindström, Estevens, & Öhman, 2014)。然而, 要证实威胁性刺激导致注意偏向的进化论解释还有一个非常重要的问题没有解决, 那就是已有实验的生态效度不高。既然人对威胁性刺激的注意偏向加工是适应环境的结果, 那么环境信息与搜索时的光流信息就很重要。现有实验都是通过威胁性刺激图片的呈现来设计的, 没有提供早期人类搜索蛇或蜘蛛时的必要环境信息。如果不提供必要的环境信息, 现有的注意偏向结果也可能解释为新异刺激导致的效应(Peira, Golkar, Öhman, Anders, & Wiens, 2012)。另外, 真实环境中人在搜索威胁性刺激时是一种身体不断接近的过程, 威胁物就算是蛰伏的, 相应的外形颜色特征也会随着接近过程中的光流信息的变化而获得监测。在知觉理论中, 吉布森早就提出并强调光流(optic flow)信息在视知觉加工中的重要作用, 他认为利用环境提供的光学分布、结构级差、光流模式、功能承受性和共振等信息可以解释视知觉(Gibson, 1979; Lee & Kalmus, 1980; Sun, Carey, & Goodale, 1992)。在真实的视觉搜索中, 物体远近及角度变化带来的动态信息对理解视觉搜索加工的认知机制很重要, 而威胁性刺激呈现的情境信息(比如远古人类面对的丛林草地环境)对检验已有研究的生态效度同样重要。显然, 无论是环境信息还是光流信息, 已有研究都没有提供。

受过去的实验技术限制, 已有的视觉搜索研究中几乎找不到生态效度好的实验数据。随着计算机信息技术的进步, 新近快速发展起来的虚拟现实技术为解决这一问题提供了可能。虚拟现实技术是利用计算机创设和体验虚拟环境的一种仿真系统, 这一系统的最大特征是沉浸感强, 能够给使用者带来身临其境的感觉。就光流信息而言, 虚拟现实场景中的漫游就是典型的模拟了现实场景中相对运动而导致的物体的表面、边缘等处的光流信息, 这种光流信息本质上体现的是一种似动运动信息。利用虚拟现实技术, 心理学实验不仅可以有仿真的刺激情境, 还可以提供远近角度上的光流动态变化信息, 还原进化心理学家假设的远古人类在丛林或草丛中随时搜索发现蛇与蜘蛛的真实场景。

基于以上的分析, 本研究利用虚拟现实技术, 模拟早期人类生存的丛林场景, 让被试以浸入式体验的方式执行Öhman和Flykt等(2001)设计的威胁性刺激的视觉搜索任务, 探讨威胁性刺激引起注意偏向的认知机制。将虚拟现实技术应用于视觉搜索实验范式的优势在于, 一是提供刺激的仿真性情境信息, 二是提供刺激的光流信息, 三是提供反应时之外的反应距离信息。反应时之外的反应距离信息在二维呈现的传统实验里几乎无法测试, 但是在虚拟仿真的三维场景中就可以很容易地观测被试做搜索反应时的空间距离。这种反应距离的测量既是反应时或正确率数据的有力补充, 又是进一步了解注意偏向的认知机制的新的有效指标。根据已有二维呈现的实验研究结论, 我们预期在三维虚拟仿真场景中威胁性刺激将同样导致注意偏向反应, 同时反应距离信息能进一步证实这种注意偏向反应。由此, 我们假设, 相比于非威胁性刺激, 被试会在更远的距离下做出对威胁性刺激的搜索确认反应。

此外, 越来越多的研究表明, 动物的搜索要比植物的搜索快(Soares, Esteves, Lundqvist, & Öhman, 2009; Soares, 2012)。在Öhman和Flykt等(2001)的经典研究中, 威胁性刺激是蜘蛛与蛇, 而非威胁性刺激是蘑菇与花, 因此在验证威胁性刺激注意偏向时有必要排除威胁性刺激与非威胁性刺激在类型属性上的这种差异。为此, 本研究进一步在虚拟现实情境中比较了威胁性刺激蛇和蜘蛛与非威胁性刺激松鼠和蝉在丛林搜索中的差异, 从而进一步验证威胁性刺激的注意偏向现象, 为威胁性刺激注意加工的进化心理学解释提供最重要的实验证据。

为验证上述观点, 本研究设计了两个实验, 实验1是设计构建野外丛林虚拟仿真环境, 以在较好的生态效度情况下重复Öhman和Flykt等(2001)的经典实验。实验2则是在实验1的基础上将非威胁性刺激替换成松鼠和蝉, 再次验证威胁性刺激的注意偏向加工。

2 方法

实验1将探讨被试在虚拟仿真的丛林搜索实验中, 搜索威胁性刺激是否快于非威胁性刺激, 以演示经典的威胁性刺激注意偏向加工现象。实验2拟将进一步探讨同属动物的威胁性与非威胁性刺激是否仍然存在威胁性刺激的注意偏向加工现象。实验1与实验2的结果一起为威胁性刺激的注意偏向加工的进化心理学解释提供证据支持。

2.1 被试

招募大学生被试共89名(男45名)。实验1共有59人参与。其中10名(男5名)参加了刺激材料的评定实验, 24名(男12名)填写了虚拟现实场景的体验问卷, 20名(男10名)参加了视觉搜索实验, 5名(男3名)参加了数据剔除标准的评定测验。实验2共有30人参与。其中10名(男5名)参加了刺激材料的评定实验, 20名(男10名)参加了视觉搜索实验。所有被试视力或矫正视力正常, 均为右利手, 身体心理健康, 无精神病史, 无任何恐惧症。被试自愿参加实验, 实验完成后获得一定报酬。

2.2 材料

2.2.1 实验用虚拟仿真场景

实验场景包括三部分材料, 第一部分为实验刺激材料, 包括威胁性刺激(蛇与蜘蛛)与非威胁性刺激(蘑菇、花、松鼠、蝉)的仿真模型; 第二部分为丛林草地自然场景, 包括多种草、花、树、石头等; 第三部分为实验交互控制材料, 包括进入丛林入口处的一扇门以及第一人称虚拟角色(在实验中这一角色是隐藏的, 被试不可见)。丛林草地场景与门如图1所示。

图1

图1   视觉搜索实验中所使用的仿真丛林场景

注:彩图见电子版, 下同


2.2.2 视觉搜索实验中的刺激物

搜索实验所用刺激物分别为蛇、蜘蛛、花、蘑菇、松鼠、蝉六种。对于刺激物的大小, 由于很难找到精确的尺度来衡量, 所以我们采用了比较相对体积的方法, 这种方法确保了这6种目标物的尺寸的大小接近。对于刺激物的颜色, 这6种模型采用了同样的颜色贴图。6种刺激物的大小及颜色对比如图2所示。

图2

图2   搜索实验中所用刺激物模型及颜色对比

注:依次为蛇、蜘蛛、花、蘑菇、松鼠、蝉, 其中花、蘑菇与蛇、蜘蛛匹配作为实验1的刺激材料, 松鼠、蝉与蛇、蜘蛛匹配作为实验2的材料。


2.2.3 丛林草地中间的小径设置

为了保证被试以相同的条件进行视觉搜索, 我们在丛林草地中间设置了一条人为踩踏出的略呈“S”型的小道, 被试通过摄像机以直线在小路上行走, 沿小道两边布置目标刺激物(威胁性或非威胁性目标刺激)放置点, 同时在草地上点缀多种野花、杂树、石头等必要自然物。为了方便记录, 路的右边记为位置1到位置7, 路的左边记为位置11到位置17, 如图3左侧所示。需要说明的是, 路径两边的各点分布并不是等距离选取的, 这是防止被试出现搜索猜测。

图3

图3   丛林草地上的路径及对所搜索目标物(蘑菇)进行评定时的3种条件

注:固定路径摄像机行走的是中间的直线, 在小径的任意行走点上, 被试看到目标物的视线与固定路径之间的夹角为α


2.2.4 实验前的评定实验所用材料

评定的材料是从创设的虚拟仿真场景中对4种目标模型的截图, 显示器分辨率为1024 × 768。我们分别从3个不同距离进行截图, 一个是肯定可以看到目标物时, 截图大小为65 × 65像素; 一个是基本可以看到目标物时, 截图大小为50 × 50像素; 一个是隐约可以看到目标物时, 截图大小为30 × 30像素。其中肯定可以看到目标物时对所有位置进行截图, 共有56张图片; 基本可以看到目标物时从位置3开始截图, 共有40张图片; 隐约可以看到目标物时从位置5开始截图, 共有24张图片, 3种截图如图3右侧所示。

2.2.5 实验中所用材料的制作软件信息

实验中所用的图形场景制作软件主要有3个:3dsmax9, Adobe Photoshop CS 5及Virtools 5.0。3dsmax9 (Autodesk, San Rafael, California, U.S.)主要用于制作和修改模型, 为了使后期计算机运行起来更流畅, 在制作和修改模型时, 在保证逼真的前提下, 尽量减少模型的面数。Adobe Photoshop CS5 (Adobe Systems Inc, SAN jose, California, U.S.)主要用于对模型贴图的修改。Virtools 5.0虚拟现实游戏引擎(Dassault Systèmes, Vélizy, Villacoublay Cedex, France)用于创设野外丛林场景。我们将3dsmax中做成的模型导入进来, 在Virtools 5.0中整合成完整场景, 然后进行交互设计和编程, 实现被试能够在场景中进行固定路径的视觉搜索。

2.3 设计

实验分为实验1与实验2, 每个实验包括两个部分, 第一部分为虚拟现实实验场景的真实感体验评定和视觉搜索目标物物理属性差异的评定, 第二部分是正式虚拟现实场景中的视觉搜索实验。

虚拟现实场景的体验评定采用国外较为常用的IGROUP问卷(Schubert, Friedmann, & Regenbrecht, 2001; Felnhofer et al., 2015)。视觉搜索目标物的物理属性差异评定是通过一个简单的选择反应时实验来完成的。物理属性差异评定采用4(刺激物模型:蛇、蜘蛛、花、蘑菇) × 14(位置:虚拟场景中的14个位置)被试内设计, 刺激物采用电脑图片呈现方式, 记录被试的反应时和错误率。

正式的视觉搜索实验为2(刺激类型:威胁性刺激、非威胁性刺激) × 14(位置:虚拟场景中的14个位置)被试内设计, 记录反应时、错误率以及被试做反应时的反应距离。反应时为从门开到被试按键反应的时间, 错误率为反应错误试次所占该条件总试次数的百分比。对于反应距离的定义, 我们将反应距离与传统实验的反应时(Response Time)相对应。具体做法为, 首先, 我们将被试在三维场景中做反应时摄像机的位置(即反应位置)距离目标位置的连线, 然后, 将反应位置距离目标位置对摄像机行走路径做垂点的连线, 最后, 两条连线做夹角, 夹角的正切值即为反应距离(Response Distance), 如图4中的tanα。这样一来, 反应距离包含反应位置与目标位置的距离信息及测量视角, 故而是一种更为全面的指标。图4示意了反应距离的计算方式。在Virtools软件中, 固定路径漫游中的某时刻点的坐标无法获取, 程序是封装模块, 除非进行非常底层的二次开发。实际上, 固定路径漫游是匀速运动, 我们根据被试的反应时进行事后计算, 相应的位置坐标同样可以得到。因此, 本研究中就以某个目标位置(比如位置4)搜索时的平均反应时RT为基础(即摄像机行走SOA (图4中的2D子图)所用的时间)进行计算。程序中最初设定的路径起点O到终点E的总时间为Ttotal, 将RT与Ttotal相比就可以得到A点的坐标。图中B点为该目标位置向摄像机路径做垂线的垂点, 目标位置、摄像机位置、垂点构成三角形, 计算现tanα值, 以此值作为RD的指标。

图4

图4   反应距离计算示意图


特别要说明, 实验中采用了我们实验室开发的计时器来完成的, 它是基于CPU时钟来记录的, 比较精确。如果采用Virtools软件本身提供的记时器, 由于它是基于刷新率来记录时间的, 所以在加载高清场景时, 刷新率不容易稳定, 记时精度也不够高, 值得注意。

我们在设计时也考虑了光流信息。光流信息包含了被观察物体的运动信息, 也包含有关景物三维结构的丰富信息, 我们在设计时通过虚拟现实技术展现了搜索物体相对运动的信息。摄像机在场景中移动, 搜索的物体距离摄像机越来越近, 其相对摄像机向摄像机反方向移动。也包含了搜索场景的信息。前人因技术限制没有考虑光流信息, 我们通过该技术提供了视觉搜索的光流信息, 更好地为威胁场景中刺激搜索提供了更完善的证据支持。

因实验1的威胁性刺激模型为动物(蛇与蜘蛛), 非威胁性刺激模型为植物(花与蘑菇), 为排除此动植物两种自变量混淆对实验结果的影响, 我们进行了实验2。实验2将刺激物模型全部更换为动物, 分别是:蛇、蜘蛛、松鼠、蝉。其余与实验1保持一致。

2.3.1 虚拟现实场景体验评定

尽管当前的Oculus DK2完全浸入式的虚拟现实呈现设备的临场感已经非常明显, 但为了准确评价所建场景是否有效提高了实验任务的生态效度, 我们对实验1的场景进行了评定。被试进行8次搜索, 每个模型搜索两次, 然后填写IGROUP问卷。该问卷包括14个问题, 每个问题都采用0~6评分, 标记从无真实感到非常真实的感觉。目前我们研究组已经对该问卷进行了中国化的修订, 相关研究报告正在投稿中。

24名被试填写的IGROUP问卷结果如表1所示。从评价的3个指标的均值上来看, 所建野外丛林场景的存在感、沉浸感与真实感都在中间值3以上。体验感的评定与国外两款虚拟游戏场景(Tomb Raider, Half Life)的评价结果相近(Schubert, Friedmann, & Regenbrecht, 2001; Felnhofer et al., 2015), 实验场景生态效度较好。

表1   丛林搜索场景与国外虚拟现实游戏场景IGROUP评定结果比较

因子丛林搜索场景Tomb RaiderHalf Life
存在感3.833.003.93
空间存在感3.833.063.99
沉浸感3.312.403.26
真实感体验3.041.922.34

新窗口打开| 下载CSV


2.3.2 目标物评定实验

采用传统的选择反应时实验范式进行刺激物的评定, 实验程序使用E-prime 2.0 (Psychology Software Tools, Sharpsburg, Pennsylvania, U.S.)软件编程实现。实验分为练习和正式实验两阶段, 正式的评定实验分为3个组块(block)。第一个组块的图片全部是肯定可以看到目标物时的图片, 每个位置的图片呈现2次, 所以第一个组块共112个试次(trial)。第二个组块的图片是基本可以看到目标物时的图片, 每个位置的图片呈现2次, 所以第二个组块共有80个试次。第三个组块的图片是隐约可以看到目标物时的图片, 每个位置的图片呈现2次, 所以第三个组块共有48个试次。总共3个组块有240个试次。实验流程是先呈现500 ms的注视点, 接着呈现图片, 被试按键反应后呈现800 ms的空屏, 实验进入到下一个试次。实验过程中, 被试双手食指分别放在“F”键和“J”键上, 实验要求如果被试看到的是蛇和蜘蛛, 按“F”键进行反应; 如果看到的是花和蘑菇或者松鼠和蝉(分别对应实验1与实验2), 按“J”键进行反应。实验流程如图5所示。

图5

图5   刺激评定实验流程图


2.3.3 物理显著性分析

采用Itti和Koch (2000)文章中的方法进行物理显著性分析。我们使用该文章中所用的MATLAB程序对材料图片进行评定处理, 得到Saliency和Winner Take All两幅图, 如图6所示。SaliencyMap是一幅清晰的二维地图, 包含对视觉环境中物体的显著性信息的编码。其具体方法是, 第一步, 将输入的图片中的每一个特征进行计算, 并转换为图标地形特征地图; 第二步, 将这些特征地图合并成为一幅地图, 这幅地图包含任意一个位置相对其他位置的显著性; 第三步, 找出显著地图中的最大值, 在指定的时间点, 最大值所在的位置就是下一个注意视觉区。Winner-take-all (WTA)是一种算法, 这种算法由Tsotsos等人(1995)提出, 用于SaliencyMap分析方法的第三步, 即找出显著地图中的最大值。得到每种条件下这两幅图后, 我们对Winner Take All图中心红框内的白色方形区域进行明度提取。我们首先使用Photoshop截取每种条件下的白色方形区域, 然后使用MATLAB程序对白色方形区域按HSV颜色分类方式提取明度值(V), 最后提取与SaliencyMap中心白色区域相等大小区域, 即为边长64个像素正方形的区域内4096个点的明度值。我们对4种条件下的明度值进行单因素方差分析。

图6

图6   物理显著性分析示意图


图7

图7   实验1刺激材料评定实验反应时结果


2.3.4 视觉搜索实验

正式的视觉搜索实验采用与Öhman和Flykt等人(2001)一样的实验范式, 但实验材料与情境均采用前述提到的三维仿真模型。实验程序在虚拟现实软件平台Virtools 5.0上运行。正式实验分为8个组块, 每个组块中目标物在14个位置出现2次共28个试次, 由于第一个试次的计时器计时可能不准确, 因此在每个组块前加了一个缓冲试次, 所以每个组块实际上有29个试次。我们对实验顺序进行了拉丁方平衡。根据Öhman和Flykt等的实验范式, 蛇和蜘蛛属于威胁性刺激, 花、蘑菇、松鼠与蝉属于非威胁性刺激。在每一个试次中, 目标物为威胁性刺激或非威胁性刺激。

实验时, 被试坐在电脑旁, 戴上头盔显示器。实验开始后, 摄像机镜头会自动地沿着预先设定好了的路径移动。实验要求被试仔细观察整个场景, 如果被试找到了所要搜索的目标, 则按游戏手柄的“LB”键, 如果一直到镜头停止移动都没有找到所要搜索的目标, 则按游戏手柄的“RB”键。按键反应后, 再按游戏手柄的“A”键即可以进入到下一个试次。在每个场景中, 整个森林的场景是不变的, 改变的只是分心物和目标物。

2.3.5 数据剔除标准测试

整个实验是在一个虚拟的三维仿真场景中进行, 目前还没有关于虚拟现实场景中反应时实验的报告, 为了能够更好地查看虚拟仿真场景中反应时测量, 我们特别增加设计了一个空场景中搜索刺激物的测试, 并以此作为后续反应时实验中反应记录的一个参考标准。并且设置这一测试也同时考虑到被试会对多个位置点的目标物进行搜索反应, 因此获得一个不同位置点简单反应时值也是一个不错的参考指标。就虚拟现实场景渲染距离来看, 本研究中的实验场景均没有出现远处物体不能渲染的问题。为此, 我们招募5名被试进行测试, 求得可以看到刺激物的最小反应时。考虑到场景开门时, 位置1~4和位置11~14可以看清楚的, 而位置5、位置6、位置7、位置15、位置16、位置17在开门后并不能真正看清楚。但是, 在虚拟现实场景漫游的动态情形下完成搜索实验, 由于有很多类似颜色的花草干扰, 被试不认真时很容易误认而做出按键反应。这正说明虚拟现实情境实验在提高生态效度的同时也增加了被试错误辨认的机率。搜索的模型为蛇、蜘蛛、花、蘑菇, 结果如表2所示。正式实验时, 当被试的反应时小于测试的反应时最小值, 则予以剔除。

表2   不同位置反应时最小值(平均数、标准误)

位置平均数(ms)标准误
位置55306.80122.41
位置65997.55138.11
位置76912.65119.33
位置154935.95109.64
位置165983.80101.94
位置176612.70141.75

新窗口打开| 下载CSV


3 结果

3.1 实验1结果

3.1.1 刺激材料评定实验

为了防止极端值以及错误反应对实验结果的影响, 剔除平均数在3个标准差之外的数据以及错误反应的数据, 共剔除极端值和错误反应数据72个, 剔除比例为3%。使用SPSS 22.0进行统计分析。

对3种不同距离的错误率分别进行单因素重复测量方差分析。肯定可以看到目标物时模型种类的主效应不显著, F(3, 27) = 1.91, p > 0.05。基本可以看到目标物时模型种类的主效应不显著, F(3, 27) = 0.61, p > 0.05。隐约可以看到目标物时模型种类的主效应不显著, F(3, 27) = 1.48, p > 0.05。

对3种不同距离的反应时进行重复测量方差分析。肯定可以看到目标物时, 模型种类的主效应不显著, F(3, 27) = 0.47, p > 0.05。位置的主效应不显著, F(13, 117) = 0.43, p > 0.05。模型种类与位置的交互作用也不显著, F(39, 351) = 0.97, p > 0.05。

基本可以看到目标物时, 模型种类的主效应不显著, F(3, 27) = 0.16, p > 0.05。位置的主效应显著, F(9, 81) = 2.02, p < 0.05, η2p= 0.18。进行事后检验发现, 对位置进行两两比较表明位置3和位置5差异显著, p < 0.05。模型种类与位置的交互作用为边缘显著, F(27, 243) = 2.45, p < 0.05, η2p = 0.21。简单效应分析表明, 位置15时, 蛇和蜘蛛差异显著, p < 0.05。位置16时, 蛇和蜘蛛差异显著, p < 0.01。其他位置时, 两两刺激之间差异均不显著。虽然位置15和16时, 蛇和蜘蛛的差异显著, 但蛇与蜘蛛都属于威胁性刺激, 而且不存在蛇中找蜘蛛和蜘蛛中找蛇的组合, 因此它们之间的知觉辨别力差异对实验结果没有影响。

隐约可以看到目标物时, 模型种类的主效应不显著, F(3, 27) = 0.88, p > 0.05。位置的主效应不显著, F(5, 45) = 1.36, p > 0.05。模型种类与位置的交互作用也不显著, F(15, 135) = 1.66, p > 0.05。

在3种不同距离时, 无论是反应时还是正确率, 模型种类的主效应均不显著, 表明4种模型在知觉辨别力上无显著差异, 排除了后面搜索过程中由于模型本身引起的变量混淆。

3.1.2 视觉搜索实验

所有被试的正确率较高。被试各个条件下错误率、反应时、反应距离如图8所示, 其中反应距离的平均正切值威胁性刺激(M = 0.61, SD = 0.08)比非威胁性刺激(M = 0.73, SD = 0.10)小, 以具体场景中的反应距离计算, 威胁性刺激(11.79米, SD = 0.77)也同样比非威胁性刺激(10.60米, SD = 0.73)为远。场景中呈现的物体大小(0.75米 × 0.75米), 因为场景较大, 所以物体适当放大了以使看起来是正常的比例。在威胁刺激条件下, 物体的平均视角为3.65°, 而非威胁性刺激条件下, 物体的平均视角为4.05°。为了防止极端值以及错误反应对实验结果的影响, 剔除平均数在3个标准差之外的数据、错误反应的数据以及位置5、位置6、位置7、位置15、位置16、位置17小于各自最小值的数据。共剔除81个数据, 剔除比例为1.8%。使用SPSS 22.0进行统计分析。

图8

图8   实验1视觉搜索实验被试错误率、反应时、反应距离(平均数±标准误)结果


我们还进一步地把反应时实验数据与前述剔除测试的反应时标准进行了对比, 结果发现, 前述各位置点上的简单反应时都落在反应时实验中的反应时数据的3个标准差之外。这说明, 虚拟仿真场景中的搜索辨别反应时的记录与传统实验是一致的。

对错误率进行配对样本t检验, 结果表明刺激类型差异不显著, t (19) = 0.25, p > 0.05。

对反应时进行2(刺激类型:威胁性刺激、非威胁性刺激)×14(位置:虚拟场景中的14个位置)重复测量方差分析。刺激类型主效应显著, F(1, 19) = 301.72, p < 0.01, η2p = 0.94。位置的主效应显著, F(13, 247) = 449.89, p < 0.01, η2p = 0.96。刺激类型和位置的交互作用显著, F(13, 247) = 2.60, p < 0.05, η2p = 0.12。简单效应分析表明, 威胁性刺激与非威胁性刺激的差异均显著(ps < 0.05), 搜索威胁性刺激显著快于非威胁性刺激。

对反应距离进行2(刺激类型:威胁性刺激、非威胁性刺激)×14(位置:虚拟场景中的14个位置)重复测量方差分析。刺激类型主效应显著, F(1, 19) = 301.72, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.94。位置的主效应显著, F(13, 247) = 202.84, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.997。刺激类型和位置的交互作用显著, F(13, 247) = 3.87, p < 0.05, η2p = 0.88。简单效应分析表明, 威胁性刺激与非威胁性刺激的差异均显著(ps < 0.05), 搜索威胁性刺激的反应距离比搜索非威胁性刺激的反应距离小。

我们对反应距离和反应时进行了皮尔逊积差相关分析, 发现负相关显著, r = -0.67, p < 0.01。相关分析表明, 反应时越长, 反应距离越短。上述结果表明, 在提高了实验生态效度的情况下, 威胁性刺激的注意偏向加工依然存在, 支持了经典的威胁性刺激优先加工假设(Cisler, Bacon, & Williams, 2009; Cisler & Koster, 2010; Yiend, 2010; 张禹, 罗禹, 赵守盈, 陈维, 李红, 2014)。如前言中所讨论到的, 近期有不少研究揭示, 动物的搜索要比植物的搜索快(Soares et al., 2009; Soares, 2012)。而Öhman和Flykt等(2001)的经典研究所使用的非威胁性刺激是植物(蘑菇与花)威胁性刺激是动物(蛇与蜘蛛)。因此在验证威胁性刺激注意偏向时有必要排除威胁性刺激与非威胁性刺激在类型属性上的差异。为此, 本研究在实验1的基础上进一步比较了蛇和蜘蛛同动物类非威胁性刺激(松鼠和蝉)的比较。

3.2 实验2结果

3.2.1 刺激材料评定实验

为了防止极端值以及错误反应对实验结果的影响, 剔除平均数在3个标准差之外的数据以及错误反应的数据。共剔除76个数据, 剔除比例为3%。使用SPSS 22.0进行统计分析。

对3种不同距离时的错误率分别进行单因素重复测量方差。肯定可以看到目标物时模型种类的主效应不显著, F(3, 27) = 1.80, p > 0.05。基本可以看到目标物时模型种类的主效应不显著, F(3, 27) = 3.72, p > 0.05。隐约可以看到目标物时模型种类的主效应不显著, F(3, 27) = 1.12, p > 0.05。

图9

图9   实验2刺激材料评定实验反应时结果


对3种不同距离的反应时进行重复测量方差分析。肯定可以看到目标物时, 模型种类的主效应不显著, F(3, 27) = 1.82, p > 0.05。位置的主效应不显著, F(13, 117) = 0.76, p > 0.05。模型种类与位置的交互作用也不显著, F(39, 351) = 1.03, p > 0.05。基本可以看到目标物时, 模型种类的主效应不显著, F(3, 27) = 0.56, p > 0.05。位置的主效应不显著, F(9, 81) = 0.64, p > 0.05。模型种类与位置的交互作用也不显著, F(39, 351) = 1.41, p > 0.05。隐约可以看到目标物时, 模型种类的主效应不显著, F(3, 27) = 0.89, p > 0.05。位置的主效应不显著, F(5, 45) = 0.97, p > 0.05。模型种类与位置的交互作用也不显著, F(15, 135) = 0.76, p > 0.05。

在3种不同距离时, 无论是反应时还是正确率, 模型种类的主效应均不显著, 表明4种模型在知觉辨别力上无显著, 排除了后面搜索过程中由于模型本身引起的变量混淆。

3.2.2 视觉搜索实验

各个条件下错误率、反应时、反应距离如图10所示, 其中反应距离的平均正切值威胁性刺激(M = 0.57, SD = 0.08)比非威胁性刺激(M = 0.71, SD = 0.10)小, 以具体反应距离计算, 威胁性刺激(12.47米, SD = 0.59)也同样比非威胁性刺激(10.69米, SD = 0.59)为远。因为场景较大, 物体的大小也同实验1有所放大(0.75米 × 0.75米)。在威胁性刺激条件下, 物体的平均视角为3.46°, 在非威胁性刺激条件下物体的平均视角为4.02°。为了防止极端值以及错误反应对实验结果的影响, 剔除平均数在3个标准差之外的数据、错误反应的数据以及位置5、位置6、位置7、位置15、位置16、位置17小于各自最小值的数据, 共剔除68个数据, 剔除比例为1.5%。使用SPSS 22.0进行统计分析。

图10

图10   实验2视觉搜索实验被试错误率、反应时、反应距离(平均数±标准误)结果


对错误率进行配对样本t检验, 结果表明刺激类型差异不显著, t (19)= -0.92, p > 0.05。

对反应时进行2(刺激类型:威胁性刺激、非威胁性刺激) × 14(位置:虚拟场景中的14个位置)重复测量方差分析。刺激类型主效应显著, F(1, 19) = 59.99, p < 0.01, η2p = 0.76。位置的主效应显著, F(13, 247) = 1462.51, p < 0.01, η2p = 1.00。刺激类型和位置的交互作用显著, F(13, 247) = 9.04, p < 0.01, η2p = 0.94。简单效应分析表明, 除位置6与位置17之外, 其余位置威胁性刺激与非威胁性刺激的差异均显著(ps < 0.05), 搜索威胁性刺激显著快于搜索非威胁性刺激。位置6与位置17差异不显著的原因是:在摄像机的行驶过程中, 被试最先会看到前6个位置, 行驶一段距离后, 会看到中间4个位置, 最后看到位于最后方的4个位置, 这三部分每部分内部出现的时间差异较小, 因此会有差异不显著的可能性。

对反应距离进行2(刺激类型:威胁性刺激、非威胁性刺激) × 14(位置:虚拟场景中的14个位置)重复测量方差分析。刺激类型主效应显著, F(1, 19) = 59.99, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.76。位置的主效应显著, F(13, 247) = 1462.51, p < 0.001, η2p = 1.00。刺激类型和位置的交互作用显著, F(13, 247) = 9.04, p < 0.01, η2p = 0.94。简单效应分析表明, 除位置6与位置17差异不显著之外, 其余位置威胁性刺激与非威胁性刺激的差异均显著(ps < 0.05), 搜索威胁性刺激的反应距离比搜索非威胁性刺激的反应距离小。

对反应距离和反应时进行了皮尔逊积差相关分析, 负相关边缘显著, r = -0.33, p = 0.08。同实验1的结果, 反应时越长, 反应距离越短。上述结果表明, 在把威胁性刺激替换成同样的动物后, 蛇与蜘蛛的搜索反应仍然显著高于非威胁刺激, 说明威胁性刺激的注意偏向加工现象是独立于刺激类型和动植物属性影响的。考虑到模型显著性的可能影响, 我们采用前人的方法对模型在搜索中的显著性进行了分析(Itti & Koch, 2000)。根据上述搜索实验的结果判定, 实际搜索到的目标物均在“基本可见”条件下发生, 因此我们对此条件下的模型在场景中的截图在MATLAB中进行了显著性分析, 结果如图11所示。从显著性分析结果图上可以看出, 无论是注意点还是显著性分布图均较一致, 未见明显差异。为了更好地比较搜索不同目标物在显著性上的差异, 我们对显著性结果做了进一步的量化分析。选取显著性分析明度分布图的中心区域(64像素×64像素), 计算各像素点的明度值(V), 然后对不同目标物的明度值进行单因素方差分析, 结果表明, 实验1中明度主效应不显著, F(3, 16380) = 0.48, p > 0.05。实验2中的明度主效应不显著, F(3, 16380) = 1.51, p > 0.05, 各目标物的平均明度值见表3。这些结果表明, 在实际搜索场景中, 各目标物本身的显著性彼此之间并无差异。

表3   图片物理显著性的明度值平均数(括号内为标准误)

实验蜘蛛蘑菇松鼠
实验10.605 (0.002)0.606 (0.002)0.605 (0.002)0.602 (0.002)
实验20.605 (0.002)0.606 (0.002)0.611 (0.002)0.605 (0.002)

新窗口打开| 下载CSV


图11

图11   实验中所用模型在搜索局部场景中的显著性

上:实验1模型显著性, 下:实验2模型显著性)


4 讨论

本研究的目的是要在提高实验生态效度的前提下, 进一步验证威胁性刺激注意偏向加工现象, 从而检验威胁性刺激注意加工偏向现象的进化心理学解释的可靠性。研究采用完全浸入式的虚拟仿真技术, 首次抽象模拟了人类祖先生存的野外丛林环境, 为视觉搜索威胁性刺激提供了必要的野外丛林环境与接近刺激时的光流动态信息, 最终实现了提高传统行为实验生态效度的目的。研究中收集的环境体验数据表明, 实验中建构的仿真环境能够给人带来较好的真实体验, 能够保证实验生态效度提高的有效性。实验1与实验2的结果表明, 在提高了生态效度的情况下, 人们在视觉搜索威胁性刺激时确实要比搜索非威胁性刺激快, 即使把非威胁性刺激由蘑菇和花替换成松鼠和蝉后, 被试仍然是搜索蛇与蜘蛛要比搜索松鼠和蝉快, 验证了威胁性刺激的注意加工偏向现象, 夯实了进化心理学解释的实验基础。除此之外, 本研究还首次发现了注意偏向加工在空间距离尺度上的存在证据, 即在视觉搜索中, 人对威胁性刺激搜索反应的距离显著远于对非威胁性刺激搜索反应的距离。反应距离的数据与反应时的数据负相关显著, 两个指标可互相补充, 趋势一致。无论是反应时数据还是反应距离数据都稳定一致地支持威胁性刺激的注意偏向加工效应, 支持进化心理学的解释。

实验中的反应距离信息有重要的验证性价值。传统的二维图片呈现实验只是从反应时指标上揭示这种威胁性刺激的优先反应, 尚不能从空间距离上模拟动态的搜索过程, 以更好的生态效度来呈现这一结果。本研究的空间距离信息首次弥补了这方面的信息。与传统二维图片实验结果一样, 威胁性刺激优先加工与已有认知神经科学的研究预期相一致。Vuilleumier等(2003)的研究揭示人对威胁性刺激的加工更多地是存在于视觉信息加工的早期阶段, 这一阶段的加工是一种自动化的加工, 主要依赖于低频轮廓信息。本研究通过反应距离的计算证实, 威胁性刺激(3.5º视角处)相比于非威胁性刺激(4.0º视角)在更远的距离上就被检测确认。当然因为是动态的搜索, 被试在看到搜索物到按下键还有一段时间, 这个时间会导致实际记录的视角变得更大。从刺激信息上看, 距离越远越只能依赖于低频轮廓信息进行加工。Vuilleumier等(2003)的研究是通过人为地操纵刺激物的空间频率信息来验证威胁性刺激的早期自动加工的, 而本研究则是通过自然的仿真情境观察直接得到这一结果。虽然我们从反应时数据上完全可以推测出反应距离的结果, 但是这种推测仍然是建立在可以测量搜索过程中的空间距离这一条件之下的, 这与传统实验中固定刺激物与被试眼睛之间的距离有着很大的不同, 这种不同正是本研究提高实验生态效度的结果。因此, 反应距离信息在视觉搜索实验中可以作为反应时数据之外的一个重要补偿性指标。同时, 它具有进化心理学的重要意义。反应距离数据提示我们, 搜索威胁性刺激更可能是一种低频自动化加工。从进化论的角度来看, 一种心理行为如果是适应环境的结果, 那么它更可能是情绪性的反应, 是快速的自动化反应(Tooby & Cosmides, 1990)。反应距离数据恰好告诉我们, 威胁性刺激的搜索反应是快速的自动化反应, 符合适应进化的特征。

实验中的情境信息很重要。在已有研究中, 有少数研究并没有重复出威胁性刺激导致注意偏向的结果, 比如快乐面孔的搜索要比愤怒面孔快。Juth, Lundqvist, Karlsson和Öhman (2005)采用了视觉搜索的范式让被试在中性面孔中寻找快乐、愤怒和恐惧目标面孔。结果表明, 探测快乐面孔要显著地快于探测愤怒和恐惧面孔。Savage, Lipp, Craig, Becker和Horstmann (2013)使用来自Ekman和Friesen面孔图片库的面孔图片作为实验材料, 结果也表明, 在中性面孔中搜索快乐面孔要快于搜索愤怒面孔。现在看来, 这些相反的结果很可能是由实验的生态效度不够导致的。就一般的社会刺激情境而言, 威胁性刺激并不一定会启动人类的生存适应器, 而如果提供了一些必要的情境信息这些结果就可能反转。那么什么样的信息才是关键的情境信息?本研究是通过概念释义的方式来确定的, 即根据Öhman (2009)的人类祖先的野外生存适应假说, 我们将这种必要的情境具体化为野外丛林, 草中的蛇与蜘蛛, 那么对于威胁性的面孔来说, 可能就要用更为社会化的情境来解释。

如何理解对威胁性刺激的注意偏向加工的认知机制还需要进一步的研究证据的支持。注意偏向是优先捕获, 还是特异性特征的加工, 目前尚无法确认。知觉过程包含两种加工:自上而下的加工和自下而上的加工。自上而下的加工是由有关知觉对象的一般知识开始的加工, 而自下而上的加工是由外部刺激开始的加工。Lindsay和Norman (1977)将自上而下的加工称为概念驱动加工, 而将自下而上的加工称为数据驱动加工。在此基础上, Torralba, Oliva, Castelhano和Henderson (2006)提出背景引导模型, 认为观察者根据从场景中获得的信息, 结合个体的知识经验, 从而注视场景中最可能存在目标物的位置。同时, 该模型认为对目标物的搜索依赖于自上而下的知识经验, 并结合自下而上的场景信息。Theeuwes (2010)认为人类对威胁自己生存的信息始终保持着注意, 这可以看作始终存在的自上而下的“预设”任务目标。从本研究的结果来看, 对威胁性刺激的早期预警反应可能是开始于某种特异性特征的驱动, 继而启动自上而下加工的确认所导致的。

本研究使用了虚拟现实技术, 大大提高了实验结果的生态效度, 但也存在着一些问题。首先, 虚拟现实环境中的远景渲染与实际环境中不太一样, 考虑到虚拟现实场景一般都比较大, 相应的硬件性能还不够强, 一般对远处场景及物件的渲染都有一个限定距离, 这可能会给实验带来一些影响。好在已有研究表明, 威胁性刺激的情绪反应主要是在意识层面(Peira et al., 2012)。本研究为了避免这个问题的影响, 专门进行了最小反应时的测定, 并据此剔除那些低于这一反应时数值的反应。其次, 虚拟仿真场景目前受限于硬件性能, 场景的逼真度还不够, 尤其是头盔的刷新率及分辨率与真实场景比较还有很大的距离, 这可能也会影响被试的浸入感。本研究为了弱化这一影响, 对场景中的外围模型进行了面数优化, 保留了搜索范围内的物件模型的高精度, 并且在性能较高的工作站上运行实验程序, 以求实验中的最佳体验。头盔问题是目前虚拟现实实验技术中的瓶颈, 在短时间内还难以解决。最后, 虚拟现实技术在提高实验的生态效度的同时也为实验的精确控制提出了挑战。自然场景条件下我们的搜索是完全不受限制的, 但是在虚拟现实技术中, 这种限制总是无法避免, 比如头盔的视野范围, 触压觉、动觉等感官信息的提供, 仿真模型彼此之间在大小与形状上的差异导致遮挡或物件的知觉水平可能有差异等等。尽管如此, 虚拟现实在改善心理学已有实验的生态效度上确实是前所未有的, 在心理学领域的未来应用前景值得期待。目前, 虚拟现实技术在心理学上的应用还比较少, 就我们所做的数据库检索发现, 目前, 虚拟现实技术在心理学上的应用还比较少, 有文献支持临床干预的有效性(Urech, Krieger, Chesham, Mast, & Berger, 2015), 但在基础心理研究上除了传统的空间认知研究外几乎没有多少应用, 关于情绪刺激对注意偏向影响的研究根本没有。本研究不仅为已有研究提供了有力的支持, 还为新技术、新方法应用于心理学研究提供了范例。在未来的研究中, 我们期待将虚拟现实技术特有的沉浸性、互动性和构想性特点与心理学实验设计紧密结合, 进一步提高心理学实验的生态效度。

5 结论

本研究的结论是, 在自然场景的虚拟仿真场景中, 人们搜索威胁性刺激要快于非威胁性刺激。生态效度与理论假说相结合为注意偏向加工说提供了进一步的事实依据, 同时本研究也是运用虚拟现实技术提高实验生态效度的一次尝试。

致谢:

李然、姚维佳对本文进行了认真的校对, 在此表示感谢。

参考文献

Buss, D. M . ( 1999).

Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind. Boston: Allyn and

Bacon.

[本文引用: 1]

Cisler J. M., Bacon A. K., & Williams N. L . ( 2009).

Phenomenological characteristics of attentional biases towards threat: A critical review

Cognitive Therapy & Research, 33(2), 221-234.

DOI:10.1007/s10608-007-9161-y      URL     PMID:2901130      [本文引用: 1]

Although research has consistently revealed the presence of a general attentional bias towards threat, empirical and theoretical ambiguity exists in determining whether attentional biases are comprised of facilitated attention to threat, difficulty in disengagement from threat, or both, as well as whether attentional biases reflect automatic or strategic processes. This paper reviews empirical investigations across four common assessment tasks: the Stroop (masked and unmasked), dot probe, visual search, and the Posner tasks. Although the review finds inconsistencies both within and between assessment tasks, the evidence suggests that attentional biases towards threat are comprised of each of the phenomenological characteristics addressed in this paper. Contemporary theoretical models of attentional biases in anxiety are summarized and critically reviewed in light of the current evidence. Suggestions for future research are addressed, including a need to investigate the psychometric properties of the assessment tasks, to utilize consistent theoretically driven operationalizations of attentional biases, and to provide a temporal description of the characteristics of attentional biases towards threat.

Cisler J. M., & Koster E. H. W . ( 2010).

Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative review

Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 203-216.

DOI:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.003      URL     PMID:2814889      [本文引用: 1]

A wealth of research demonstrates attentional biases toward threat in the anxiety disorders. Several models have been advanced to explain these biases in anxiety, yet the mechanisms comprising and mediating the biases remain unclear. In the present article, we review evidence regarding the mechanisms of attentional biases through careful examination of the components of attentional bias, the mechanisms underlying these components, and the stage of information processing during which the biases occur. Facilitated attention, difficulty in disengagement, and attentional avoidance comprise the components of attentional bias. A threat detection mechanism likely underlies facilitated attention, a process that may be neurally centered around the amygdala. Attentional control ability likely underlies difficulty in disengagement, emotion regulation goals likely underlie attentional avoidance, and both of these processes may be neurally centered around prefrontal cortex functioning. The threat detection mechanism may be a mostly automatic process, attentional avoidance may be a mostly strategic process, and difficulty in disengagement may be a mixture of automatic and strategic processing. Recommendations for future research are discussed.

Dudeney J., Sharpe L., & Hunt C . ( 2015).

Attentional bias towards threatening stimuli in children with anxiety: A meta-analysis

Clinical Psychology Review, 40, 66-75.

DOI:10.1016/j.cpr.2015.05.007      URL     PMID:26071667      [本文引用: 1]

Although it is well known that anxious adults show selective attention to threatening stimuli, research investigating attentional bias in children with anxiety has produced mixed results. The purpose of this paper is to provide a comprehensive analysis of studies investigating attentional bias in children with anxiety. Using a systematic search for articles which included both children with anxiety and reported data suitable for a meta-analysis, 38 articles were identified involving 4221 subjects (anxiety n02=022222). We used a random effects meta-analysis with standardized mean difference as our primary outcome to estimate between- and within-group effects of attentional bias towards threat-related information in children with anxiety. Overall, children with anxiety showed a significantly greater bias to threat-related stimuli, compared to controls ( d 02=020.21). Children with anxiety also showed a significant bias to threat-related stimuli, over neutral stimuli ( d 02=020.54), which was greater than the bias shown by control children ( d 02=020.15). Specific variables in attentional bias were also explored, with varying results. The review concluded that anxious children do show a similar bias towards threatening stimuli as has been documented in adults, albeit to a lesser degree and this bias is moderated by age, such that the difference between anxious and control children increases with age. Given the small number of studies in some areas, future research is needed to understand the precise conditions under which anxious children exhibit selective attentional biases to threat-related stimuli.

Felnhofer A., Kothgassner O. D., Schmidt M., Heinzle A. K., Beutl L., Hlavacs H., & Kryspin-Exner I . ( 2015).

Is virtual reality emotionally arousing? Investigating five emotion inducing virtual park scenarios. International Journal of

Human-Computer Studies, 82, 48-56.

DOI:10.1016/j.ijhcs.2015.05.004      URL     [本文引用: 2]

61Four virtual park scenarios elicited the intended emotional states.61Presence levels were the same across different virtual environments.61Skin conductance level seems to be a poor indicator of presence.

Gibson, J. J . ( 1979).

The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston, MA:

Houghton Mifflin.

DOI:10.1080/00071773.1982.11007570      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Reviews the non-fiction book 'The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception,' by James J. Gibson.

Hansen C. H., & Hansen R. D . ( 1988).

Finding the face in the crowd: An anger superiority effect

Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 54(6), 917-924.

DOI:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.917      URL     PMID:3397866      [本文引用: 1]

Facial gestures have been given an increasingly critical role in models of emotion. The biological significance of interindividual transmission of emotional signals is a pivotal assumption for placing the face in a central position in these models. This assumption invited a logical corollary, examined in this article: Face-processing should be highly efficient. Three experiments documented an asymmetry in the processing of emotionally discrepant faces embedded in crowds. The results suggested that threatening faces pop out of crowds, perhaps as a result of a preattentive, parallel search for signals of direct threat.

Itti L., & Koch C . ( 2000).

A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention

Vision Research, 40(10-12), 1489-1506.

DOI:10.1002/jid.1310      URL     PMID:1078865422      [本文引用: 2]

Most models of visual search, whether involving overt eye movements or covert shifts of attention, are based on the concept of a saliency map, that is, an explicit two-dimensional map that encodes the saliency or conspicuity of objects in the visual environment. Competition among neurons in this map gives rise to a single winning location that corresponds to the next attended target. Inhibiting this location automatically allows the system to attend to the next most salient location. We describe a detailed computer implementation of such a scheme, focusing on the problem of combining information across modalities, here orientation, intensity and color information, in a purely stimulus-driven manner. The model is applied to common psychophysical stimuli as well as to a very demanding visual search task. Its successful performance is used to address the extent to which the primate visual system carries out visual search via one or more such saliency maps and how this can be tested.

Janczyk M., Augst S., & Kunde W . ( 2014).

The locus of the emotional Stroop effect: A study with the PRP paradigm

Acta Psychologica, 151, 8-15.

DOI:10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.05.011      URL     PMID:24904999      [本文引用: 1]

Stimuli that are clearly positive or negative (hence valence-laden stimuli) have the potential to interrupt unrelated task processing. A typical example is the emotional Stroop effect (ESE) in which responding to a certain task feature (e.g., color) is delayed by the presentation of task-irrelevant valent stimuli (e.g., negative pictures) compared to valence-neutral stimuli. Here we scrutinize which processes are slowed down by irrelevant but valent stimulation. In Experiment 1, participants performed in a Psychological Refractory Period (PRP) experiment with tone discrimination as Task 1 and color discrimination as Task 2. Importantly, colors in Task 2 were accompanied by valent or neutral pictures. Valent pictures delayed responding in Task 2 (thus an ESE) and this delay was additive to the time interval between tasks. In Experiment 2, task order was reversed and the ESE in Task 1 fully propagated to the Task 2 tone discrimination. These results imply that irrelevant valence-laden stimulation delays capacity-limited processes, and we suggest that this is a late perceptual process acting on stimulus categorization.

Juth P., Lundqvist D., Karlsson A., & Öhman A . ( 2005).

Looking for foes and friends: Perceptual and emotional factors when finding a face in the crowd

Emotion, 5(4), 379-395.

DOI:10.1037/1528-3542.5.4.379      URL     PMID:16366743      [本文引用: 1]

In a face-in-the-crowd setting, the authors examined visual search for photographically reproduced happy, angry, and fearful target faces among neutral distractor faces in 3 separate experiments. Contrary to the hypothesis, happy targets were consistently detected more quickly and accurately than angry and fearful targets, as were directed compared with averted targets. There was no consistent effect of social anxiety. A facial emotion recognition experiment suggested that the happy search advantage could be due to the ease of processing happy faces. In the final experiment with perceptually controlled schematic faces, the authors reported more effective detection of angry than happy faces. This angry advantage was most obvious for highly socially anxious individuals when their social fear was experimentally enhanced.

Ledoux, J. E . ( 2000).

Emotion circuits in the brain

Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 155-184.

[本文引用: 1]

Lee D. N., & Kalmus H . ( 1980).

The optic flow field: The foundation of vision [and discussion]

Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 290(1038), 169-179.

[本文引用: 1]

Lindsay P. H. , & Norman, D. A.( 1977) . Human information processing: An introduction to psychology New York: Academic Press An introduction to psychology. New York: Academic Press.

[本文引用: 1]

LoBue, V. ( 2010).

And along came a spider: An attentional bias for the detection of spiders in young children and adults

Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107(1), 59-66.

DOI:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.04.005      URL     PMID:20529694      [本文引用: 1]

Spiders are among the most common targets of fears and phobias in the world. In visual search tasks, adults detect their presence more rapidly than other kinds of stimuli. Reported here is an investigation of whether young children share this attentional bias for the detection of spiders. In a series of experiments, preschoolers and adults were asked to find the single spider picture among an array of eight mushrooms or cockroaches or the reverse. Both children and adults detected the presence of spiders more rapidly than both categories of distracter stimuli. Furthermore, there was no difference between the detection of two neutral stimuli (cockroaches vs. mushrooms). These results provide the first evidence of enhanced visual detection of spiders in young children.

LoBue V., & Rakison D. H . ( 2013).

What we fear most: A developmental advantage for threat-relevant stimuli

Developmental Review, 33(4), 285-303.

DOI:10.1016/j.dr.2013.07.005      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Fear is one of our most basic emotions. It is an important social signal and alerts us to when a situation is safe or risky. Interestingly, not all fears are created equal: Several researchers have proposed that humans develop specific fears, such as fear of threatening stimuli, more readily than others. Here we discuss three major theories of fear acquisition, and consider the possibility that some fears are privileged in learning. Second, we review a growing literature that suggests that humans have perceptual biases that quickly draw attention to threatening stimuli in the environment. In particular, we highlight recent developmental work that shows that even infants and young children respond rapidly to the presence of threat well before they acquire any threat-relevant fears. Finally, we argue that such biases may play a causal role in privileging fear learning for certain threats, and we suggest directions for future work that can clarify whether early biases in perception indeed facilitate the development of our most common fears.

Matsumoto, E. ( 2010).

Bias in attending to emotional facial expressions: Anxiety and visual search efficiency

Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(3), 414-424.

DOI:10.1002/acp.1686      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Abstract There has been much controversy around the relationship between anxiety and attentional processing of threat-related information. The purpose of this study was to examine how threatening facial expressions affect attentional processing, according to the level of trait anxiety. Through visual search tasks, two different components of attentional bias to threat were investigated: engagement and disengagement of attention from an angry face. Two main results were found. First, reaction times (RTs) were slower in detecting the absence of a discrepant face in the all angry-display conditions rather than other expression conditions; however, there was no difference between anxiety groups. Second, the difference in search efficiency for the angry versus happy target was significant within the high-anxiety group but not within the low-anxiety group. The results suggest that the detection process for angry faces is more efficient for highly anxious people. On the other hand, the time to disengage attention from angry faces was not associated with anxiety level. Copyright 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Mogg K., & Bradley B. P . ( 2006).

Time course of attentional bias for fear-relevant pictures in spider-fearful individuals

Behaviour Research & Therapy, 44(9), 1241-1250.

DOI:10.1016/j.brat.2006.05.003      URL     PMID:16870133      [本文引用: 1]

The time course of attentional biases for spider stimuli was assessed in two groups of individuals with high or low levels of spider fear. Pairs of photographs of spiders and cats were presented in a visual probe task with three exposure durations: 200, 500 and 2000 ms. Results indicated greater attentional bias for spider stimuli in high fear, than in low fear, individuals in the 200 ms condition. The attentional bias in the high fear group significantly reduced as stimulus exposure duration increased, with no significant biases found in the longer exposure conditions. Results support the view that high fear is associated with an enhanced initial attentional bias for fear-relevant stimuli, but that this attentional bias is not maintained over time.

Mogg K., Bradley B. P., & Hallowell N . ( 1994).

Attentional bias to threat: Roles of trait anxiety, stressful events, and awareness

The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 47(4), 841-864.

DOI:10.1080/14640749408401099      URL     PMID:7809399      [本文引用: 1]

Abstract Attentional biases for threat stimuli were assessed in high and low trait anxious subjects (n = 66) using a probe detection task. To examine the effects of trait anxiety and situational stressors, each subject was tested three times: Under no stress, laboratory-induced stress, and examination-induced stress. To evaluate the role of awareness, half the word stimuli were presented very briefly (14 msec) and masked, and the other half were presented for 500 msec without a mask. Results showed that high trait anxious subjects under exam stress showed an attentional bias towards unmasked threat stimuli compared with low trait subjects. This effect was not found under lab-induced stress, suggesting that the attentional bias for unmasked threat in high trait subjects may be a function of a prolonged stressor, rather than a transient increase in state anxiety. The results from the masked exposure condition were not predicted; high trait anxious subjects shifted attention towards the spatial location of threat words despite lack of awareness of their lexical content, but this bias was only apparent in the no-stress condition. The results are discussed in relation to recent cognitive theories of anxiety.

Öhman, A. ( 2009).

Of snakes and faces: An evolutionary perspective on the psychology of fear. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 50(6), 543-552.Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(3), 466-478.

[本文引用: 8]

Öhman A., Lundqvist D., & Esteves F . ( 2001).

The face in the crowd revisited: A threat advantage with schematic stimuli

Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 80(3), 381-396.

[本文引用: 3]

Öhman A., & Mineka S . ( 2001).

Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning

Psychological Review, 108, 483-522.

Öhman A., Soares S. C., Juth P., Lindström B., & Esteves F . ( 2012).

Evolutionary derived modulations of attention to two common fear stimuli: Serpents and hostile humans

Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24(1), 17-32.

DOI:10.1080/20445911.2011.629603      URL     [本文引用: 1]

In this paper we present an evolutionary analysis of attention to stimuli that are threatening from an evolutionary perspective, such as angry faces and snakes. We review data showing that angry, photographically depicted angry faces are more rapidly detected than happy faces in a visual search setting provided that they are male and that distractors are redundant in the sense that they are drawn from a small set of faces. Following Isbell's (2009) novel Snake Detection Theory, we predicted that snakes, as the prototypical predators, should be more rapidly detected than spiders, given that spiders have provided less of a predatory threat for primates. We review a series of experiments from our laboratory showing that snakes indeed are more rapidly detected than spiders provided that the target stimuli are presented in a demanding visual context, such as many distractor stimuli, or in peripheral vision. Furthermore, they are more distracting than spiders on the performance of a primary attention task. Because snakes were not affected by perceptual load, whereas spiders followed the usual rule of better detection with low perceptual load, we concluded that attending to snakes might constitute an evolutionary adaptation.

Peira N., Golkar A., Öhman A., Anders S., & Wiens S . ( 2012).

Emotional responses in spider fear are closely related to picture awareness

Cognition and Emotion, 26(2), 252-260.

DOI:10.1080/02699931.2011.579087      URL     PMID:21973031      [本文引用: 2]

Theories of emotion propose that responses to emotional pictures can occur independently of whether or not people are aware of the picture content. Because evidence from dissociation paradigms is inconclusive, we manipulated picture awareness gradually and studied whether emotional responses varied with degree of awareness. Spider fearful and non-fearful participants viewed pictures of spiders and flowers at four levels of backward masking while electrodermal activity and heart rate were measured continuously. Recognition ratings confirmed that participants picture awareness decreased with masking. Critically, effects of spider fear on emotion ratings and heart rate also decreased with masking. These findings suggest that effects of spider fear on emotion ratings and heart rate are closely related to picture awareness.

Peng D. L. ( 2012). General psychology (4th ed.) . Beijing, China: Beijing Normal University Press.

[本文引用: 1]

[ 彭聃龄 . ( 2012). 普通心理学 (第4版). 北京: 北京师范大学出版社.]

[本文引用: 1]

Savage R. A., Lipp O. V., Craig B. M., Becker S. I., & Horstmann G . ( 2013).

In search of the emotional face: Anger versus happiness superiority in visual search

Emotion, 13(4), 758-768.

DOI:10.1037/a0031970      URL     PMID:23527503      [本文引用: 1]

Abstract Previous research has provided inconsistent results regarding visual search for emotional faces, yielding evidence for either anger superiority (i.e., more efficient search for angry faces) or happiness superiority effects (i.e., more efficient search for happy faces), suggesting that these results do not reflect on emotional expression, but on emotion (un-)related low-level perceptual features. The present study investigated possible factors mediating anger/happiness superiority effects; specifically search strategy (fixed vs. variable target search; Experiment 1), stimulus choice (Nimstim database vs. Ekman & Friesen database; Experiments 1 and 2), and emotional intensity (Experiment 3 and 3a). Angry faces were found faster than happy faces regardless of search strategy using faces from the Nimstim database (Experiment 1). By contrast, a happiness superiority effect was evident in Experiment 2 when using faces from the Ekman and Friesen database. Experiment 3 employed angry, happy, and exuberant expressions (Nimstim database) and yielded anger and happiness superiority effects, respectively, highlighting the importance of the choice of stimulus materials. Ratings of the stimulus materials collected in Experiment 3a indicate that differences in perceived emotional intensity, pleasantness, or arousal do not account for differences in search efficiency. Across three studies, the current investigation indicates that prior reports of anger or happiness superiority effects in visual search are likely to reflect on low-level visual features associated with the stimulus materials used, rather than on emotion. PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2013 APA, all rights reserved.

Schubert T., Friedmann F., & Regenbrecht H . ( 2001).

The experience of presence: Factor analytic insights

Presence, 10(3), 266-281.

DOI:10.1162/105474601300343603      URL     [本文引用: 2]

= 296) assessing self-reports of presence and immersion experiences. Additionally, judgments of “realness” were observed as a third presence component. A second-order factor analysis showed a distinction between presence, immersion, and interaction factors. Building on these results, a thirteen-item presence scale consisting of three independent components was developed and verified using confirmatory factor analyses across the two studies.

Soares, S. C . ( 2012).

The lurking snake in the grass: Interference of snake stimuli in visually taxing conditions

Evolutionary Psychology, 10(2), 187-197.

DOI:10.1177/147470491201000202      URL     PMID:22947633      [本文引用: 2]

ABSTRACT Based on evolutionary considerations, it was hypothesized that humans have been shaped to easily spot snakes in visually cluttered scenes that might otherwise hide camouflaged snakes. This hypothesis was tested in a visual search experiment in which I assessed automatic attention capture to evolutionarily-relevant distractor stimuli (snakes), in comparison with another animal which is also feared but where this fear has a disputed evolutionary origin (spiders), and neutral stimuli (mushrooms). Sixty participants were engaged in a task that involved the detection of a target (a bird) among pictures of fruits. Unexpectedly, on some trials, a snake, a spider, or a mushroom replaced one of the fruits. The question of interest was whether the distracting stimuli slowed the reaction times for finding the target (the bird) to different degrees. Perceptual load of the task was manipulated by increments in the set size (6 or 12 items) on different trials. The findings showed that snake stimuli were processed preferentially, particularly under conditions where attentional resources were depleted, which reinforced the role of this evolutionarily-relevant stimulus in accessing the visual system (Isbell, 2009).

Soares S. C., Esteves F., Lundqvist D., & Öhman A . ( 2009).

Some animal specific fears are more specific than others: Evidence from attention and emotion measures

Behaviour Research & Therapy, 47(12), 1032-1042.

DOI:10.1016/j.brat.2009.07.022      URL     PMID:19695561      [本文引用: 3]

Using a visual search methodology we investigated the effect of feared animal stimuli on attention. Our results confirmed the important role of emotion on attention. All participants detected fear-relevant stimuli (snakes and spiders) faster than neutral (mushrooms) ones against a background of fruits. In addition, spider fearful participants were sensitized specifically to detect their feared stimulus (spiders), compared to their fear-relevant but non-feared (snakes) and neutral stimuli. However, for participants fearful of snakes there was no significant difference in detection latencies between the feared (snakes) and the fear-relevant but non-feared animal stimuli (spiders). The results from the attention task were mirrored in the emotional ratings, which showed that spider fear was highly specific, whereas snake fear was associated with a more generalized enhanced evaluation of all negative stimuli.

Soares S. C., Lindström B., Esteves F., & Öhman A . ( 2014).

The hidden snake in the grass: Superior detection of snakes in challenging attentional conditions

PLoS One, 9(12), e114724.

DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0114724      URL     PMID:4262429      [本文引用: 1]

Snakes have provided a serious threat to primates throughout evolution. Furthermore, bites by venomous snakes still cause significant morbidity and mortality in tropical regions of the world. According to the Snake Detection Theory (SDT Isbell, 2006; 2009), the vital need to detect camouflaged snakes provided strong evolutionary pressure to develop astute perceptual capacity in animals that were potential targets for snake attacks. We performed a series of behavioral tests that assessed snake detection under conditions that may have been critical for survival. We used spiders as the control stimulus because they are also a common object of phobias and rated negatively by the general population, thus commonly lumped together with snakes as "evolutionary fear-relevant". Across four experiments (N鈥=鈥205) we demonstrate an advantage in snake detection, which was particularly obvious under visual conditions known to impede detection of a wide array of common stimuli, for example brief stimulus exposures, stimuli presentation in the visual periphery, and stimuli camouflaged in a cluttered environment. Our results demonstrate a striking independence of snake detection from ecological factors that impede the detection of other stimuli, which suggests that, consistent with the SDT, they reflect a specific biological adaptation. Nonetheless, the empirical tests we report are limited to only one aspect of this rich theory, which integrates findings across a wide array of scientific disciplines.

Stormark K. M., & Hugdahl K . ( 1996).

Peripheral cuing of covert spatial attention before and after emotional conditioning of the cue

International Journal of Neuroscience, 86(3-4), 225-240.

DOI:10.3109/00207459608986713      URL     PMID:8884393      [本文引用: 2]

Spatial cuing of attentional shifts were investigated before and after the visual cue had acquired emotional significance through a classical conditioning procedure. The study consisted of three phases; an attention preconditioning phase, the conditioning phase and an attention postconditioning (extinction) phase. In the attention phases, subjects participated in a trial-by-trial cuing task, in which the location of the target was validly or invalidly cued by either a frame-lit or a completely lit rectangle. During conditioning, half the subjects (Conditioning group) had a 90 dB white noise unconditioned stimulus (UCS) presented together with one of the two attentional cues. This cue was, thus, turned into a conditioned stimulus (CS+), while the other cue became a CS-. The Control group received the noise uncontingent upon presentations of these stimuli. The Conditioning group showed greater skin conductance responses (SCRs) to the CS+ compared to the CS-, reflecting that a conditioned response was established. When the CS+ served as attentional cue, there was no difference in RTs between validly and invalidly cued targets, while responses to invalidly cued targets were delayed on all other trials. This suggests that the CS+ reduced the cognitive cost of shifting attention from the cued to the uncued location.

Stormark K. M., Nordby H., & Hugdahl K . ( 1995).

Attentional shifts to emotionally charged cues: Behavioural and ERP data

Cognition & Emotion, 9(5), 507-523.

DOI:10.1080/02699939508408978      URL     [本文引用: 1]

When information activated in memory involves emotional associations, the ability to shift attention away from an emotional cue is impaired compared to an emotionally neutral cue. The purpose of the present study was to investigate how emotional stimuli modulate attentional processes, and how this is reflected in localised brain electrical activity. Eight emotion and eight neutral words served as cues in a covert attention spatial orienting task. The cues were either valid or invalid indicators of which hemifield the target would be presented to. In the remaining trials, no cue was presented prior to the target. Twenty subjects were instructed to manually respond to the target as fast as possible. Event-related potentials (ERPs) showed an enhanced P3 component to the emotion words. The ERPs to the target showed enhanced P1 and P3 components on invalid trials, with emotional cues. There were faster reaction times (RTs) to validly cued targets, but only when the emotion words served as cues. The results demonstrated that the emotional cues elicited sustained focused attention, facilitating an engage mechanism of spatial orienting.

Sun H. J., Carey D. P., & Goodale M. A . ( 1992).

A mammalian model of optic-flow utilization in the control of locomotion

Experimental Brain Research, 91(1), 171-175.

DOI:10.1007/BF00230026      URL     PMID:1301371      [本文引用: 1]

Abstract Gibson (1966, 1979) and Lee (1976) have described the potential usefulness of optic-flow information for the control of locomotion. One variable that might be particularly important for an animal approaching a target is time-to-collision, which Lee argues is most efficiently specified by the tau margin (the inverse of the relative rate of expansion of the target image on the retina). In humans, most empirical studies of optic flow have required perceptual judgements or have examined catching/intercepting behaviours. In animals, most studies have been strictly observational. This is particularly true for mammals, where there has been no experimental work of any kind looking at the control of locomotion. The present experiment demonstrates that the Mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus) uses time-to-collision information to control deceleration as it runs towards a target. The development of this animal model will aid investigation of the neural circuitry underlying optic flow utilization in motor control.

Theeuwes, J. ( 2010).

Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection

Acta Psychologica, 135(2), 77-99.

DOI:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.02.006      URL     PMID:20507828      [本文引用: 1]

Abstract The present paper argues for the notion that when attention is spread across the visual field in the first sweep of information through the brain visual selection is completely stimulus-driven. Only later in time, through recurrent feedback processing, volitional control based on expectancy and goal set will bias visual selection in a top-down manner. Here we review behavioral evidence as well as evidence from ERP, fMRI, TMS and single cell recording consistent with stimulus-driven selection. Alternative viewpoints that assume a large role for top-down processing are discussed. It is argued that in most cases evidence supporting top-down control on visual selection in fact demonstrates top-down control on processes occurring later in time, following initial selection. We conclude that top-down knowledge regarding non-spatial features of the objects cannot alter the initial selection priority. Only by adjusting the size of the attentional window, the initial sweep of information through the brain may be altered in a top-down way. Copyright 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Thrasher C., & LoBue V . ( 2016).

Do infants find snakes aversive? Infants' physiological responses to "fear-relevant" stimuli

Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 142, 382-389.

DOI:10.1016/j.jecp.2015.09.013      URL     PMID:26483161      [本文引用: 1]

In the current research, we sought to measure infants' physiological responses to snakes-one of the world's most widely feared stimuli-to examine whether they find snakes aversive or merely attention grabbing. Using a similar method to DeLoache and LoBue (Developmental Science, 2009, Vol. 12, pp. 201-207), 6- to 9-month-olds watched a series of multimodal (both auditory and visual) stimuli: a video of a snake (fear-relevant) or an elephant (non-fear-relevant) paired with either a fearful or happy auditory track. We measured physiological responses to the pairs of stimuli, including startle magnitude, latency to startle, and heart rate. Results suggest that snakes capture infants' attention; infants showed the fastest startle responses and lowest average heart rate to the snakes, especially when paired with a fearful voice. Unexpectedly, they also showed significantly reduced startle magnitude during this same snake video plus fearful voice combination. The results are discussed with respect to theoretical perspectives on fear acquisition.

Tooby J., & Cosmides L . ( 1990).

The past explains the present: Emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments

Ethology & Sociobiology, 11(4-5), 375-424.

DOI:10.1016/0162-3095(90)90017-Z      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Present conditions and selection pressures are irrelevant to the present design of organisms and do not explain how or why organisms behave adaptively, when they do. To whatever non-chance extent organisms are behaving adaptively, it is 1) because of the operation of underlying adaptations whose present design is the product of selection in the past, and 2) because present conditions resemble past conditions in those specific ways made developmentally and functionally important by the design of those adaptations. All adaptations evolved in response to the repeating elements of past environments, and their structure reflects in detail the recurrent structure of ancestral environments. Even planning mechanisms (such as onsciousness ), which supposedly deal with novel situations, depend on ancestrally shaped categorization processes and are therefore not free of the fast. In fact, the categorization of each new situation into evolutionarily repeating classes involves another kind of adaptation, the emotions, which match specialized modes of organismic operation to evolutionarily recurrent situations. The detailed statistical structure of these iterated systems of events is reflected in the detailed structure of the algorithms that govern emotional state. For this reason, the system of psychological adaptations that comprises each individual meets the present only as a version of the past.

Torralba A., Oliva A., Castelhano M. S., & Henderson J. M . ( 2006).

Contextual guidance of eye movements and attention in real-world scenes: The role of global features in object search

Psychological Review, 113(4), 766-786.

[本文引用: 1]

Tsotsos J. K., Culhane S. M., Kei W. W. Y., Lai Y. Z., Davis N., & Nuflo F . ( 1995).

Modeling visual attention via selective tuning

Artificial Intelligence, 78(1-2), 507-545.

DOI:10.1016/0004-3702(95)00025-9      URL     [本文引用: 1]

Abstract A model for aspects of visual attention based on the concept of selective tuning is presented. It provides for a solution to the problems of selection in an image, information routing through the visual processing hierarchy and task-specific attentional bias. The central thesis is that attention acts to optimize the search procedure inherent in a solution to vision. It does so by selectively tuning the visual processing network which is accomplished by a top-down hierarchy of winner-take-all processes embedded within the visual processing pyramid. Comparisons to other major computational models of attention and to the relevant neurobiology are included in detail throughout the paper. The model has been implemented; several examples of its performance are shown. This model is a hypothesis for primate visual attention, but it also outperforms existing computational solutions for attention in machine vision and is highly appropriate to solving the problem in a robot vision system.

Urech A., Krieger T., Chesham A., Mast F. W., & Berger T . ( 2015).

Virtual reality-based attention bias modification training for social anxiety: A feasibility and proof of concept study

Frontiers in Psychiatry, 6, 154.

DOI:10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00154      URL     PMID:4623392      [本文引用: 1]

Attention bias modification (ABM) programs have been considered as a promising new approach for the treatment of various disorders, including social anxiety disorder (SAD). However, previous studies yielded ambiguous results regarding the efficacy of ABM in SAD. The present proof-of-concept study investigates the feasibility of a newly developed virtual reality (VR)-based dot-probe training paradigm. It was designed to facilitate attentional disengagement from threatening stimuli in socially anxious individuals (N=鈥15). The following outcomes were examined: (a) self-reports of enjoyment, motivation, flow, and presence; (b) attentional bias for social stimuli; and (c) social anxiety symptoms. Results showed that ABM training is associated with high scores in enjoyment, motivation, flow, and presence. Furthermore, significant improvements in terms of attention bias and social anxiety symptoms were observed from pre- to follow-up assessment. The study suggests that VR is a feasible and presumably a promising new medium for ABM trainings. Controlled studies will need to be carried out.

van Le Q., Isbell L. A., Matsumoto J., Nguyen M., Hori E., Maior R. S., … Nishijo H . ( 2013).

Pulvinar neurons reveal neurobiological evidence of past selection for rapid detection of snakes

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(47), 19000-19005.

DOI:10.1073/pnas.1312648110      URL     PMID:24167268      [本文引用: 1]

Snakes and their relationships with humans and other primates have attracted broad attention from multiple fields of study, but not, surprisingly, from neuroscience, despite the involvement of the visual system and strong behavioral and physiological evidence that humans and other primates can detect snakes faster than innocuous objects. Here, we report the existence of neurons in the primate medial and dorsolateral pulvinar that respond selectively to visual images of snakes. Compared with three other categories of stimuli (monkey faces, monkey hands, and geometrical shapes), snakes elicited the strongest, fastest responses, and the responses were not reduced by low spatial filtering. These findings integrate neuroscience with evolutionary biology, anthropology, psychology, herpetology, and primatology by identifying a neurobiological basis for primates' heightened visual sensitivity to snakes, and adding a crucial component to the growing evolutionary perspective that snakes have long shaped our primate lineage.

Vuilleumier P., Armony J. L., Driver J., & Dolan R. J . ( 2003).

Distinct spatial frequency sensitivities for processing faces and emotional expressions

Nature Neuroscience, 6(6), 624-631.

DOI:10.1016/S0021-9924(02)00092-8      URL     PMID:12740580      [本文引用: 2]

Abstract High and low spatial frequency information in visual images is processed by distinct neural channels. Using event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in humans, we show dissociable roles of such visual channels for processing faces and emotional fearful expressions. Neural responses in fusiform cortex, and effects of repeating the same face identity upon fusiform activity, were greater with intact or high-spatial-frequency face stimuli than with low-frequency faces, regardless of emotional expression. In contrast, amygdala responses to fearful expressions were greater for intact or low-frequency faces than for high-frequency faces. An activation of pulvinar and superior colliculus by fearful expressions occurred specifically with low-frequency faces, suggesting that these subcortical pathways may provide coarse fear-related inputs to the amygdala.

Wang F. X., Li W. J., Yan Z. Q., Duan Z. H., & Li H . ( 2015).

Children's attention detection to snakes: Evidence from eye movements

Acta Psychologica Sinica, 47(6), 774-786.

[本文引用: 1]

[ 王福兴, 李文静, 颜志强, 段朝辉, 李卉 . ( 2015).

幼儿对威胁性刺激蛇的注意觉察: 来自眼动证据

心理学报, 47(6), 774-786.]

[本文引用: 1]

Yiend, J. ( 2010).

The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information

Cognition & Emotion, 24(1), 3-47.

DOI:10.1080/02699930903205698      URL     [本文引用: 1]

This review considers evidence from cognitive experimental investigations of attentional processing of emotional information. The review contrasts findings from the general population with those from populations selected for clinical disorder or vulnerability to it. Concepts critical for appreciation of this literature are presented and major cognitive theories are summarised, evaluated and compared. Empirical data are organised by type of attentional function, covering filtering (dichotic listening, emotional Stroop), search (visual search), cuing (attentional probe, spatial cuing) and multiple task (RSVP) paradigms. Conclusions are that, consistent with current models, differences in an “evaluative system” appear to lie at the heart of the phenomena reviewed and attentional biases to emotional material reflect the responsiveness of this system. If so, desensitising its over-reactivity would be the best approach to ameliorating the negative consequences of attentional biases in psychopathology. To do so requires greater understanding of how and on what basis the “evaluation” is conducted. A possible way forward is suggested.

Zhang Y., Luo Y., Zhao S. Y., Chen W., & Li H . ( 2014).

Attentional bias towards threat: Facilitated attentional orienting or impaired attentional disengagement

Advances in Psychological Science, 22(7), 1129-1138.

[本文引用: 1]

[ 张禹, 罗禹, 赵守盈, 陈维, 李红 . ( 2014).

对威胁刺激的注意偏向: 注意定向加速还是注意解除困难?

心理科学进展, 22(7), 1129-1138.]

DOI:10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.01129      URL     [本文引用: 1]

对威胁刺激的注意偏向普遍存在。注意定向加速和注意解除困难在不同阶段对注意偏向产生影响。威胁刺激属性、呈现时间以及被试特征是重要的调节因素。以杏仁核为核心的杏仁核?前扣带回网络可能是注意定向加速的神经基础,而以眶额叶为中心的前额叶皮层可能是注意解除的神经基础。未来还需要就注意定向加速与注意解除困难的关系、注意偏向的调节机制以及其神经基础做更深入的研究。

/


版权所有 © 《心理学报》编辑部
地址:北京市朝阳区林萃路16号院 
邮编:100101 
电话:010-64850861 
E-mail:xuebao@psych.ac.cn
备案编号:京ICP备10049795号-1 京公网安备110402500018号

本系统由北京玛格泰克科技发展有限公司设计开发