Please wait a minute...
Acta Psychologica Sinica    2013, Vol. 45 Issue (3) : 345-352     DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2013.00345
|
Introducing Neutrals: A Reinterpretation of the Endowment Effect
CHEN Qunlin;YUAN Xiaolin;JIA Lei;XIAO Shaobei;ZHANG Qinlin
(1 Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Ministry of Education, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China) (2 School of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China) (3 College of Educational Science, Hainan Normal University, Haikou 571158, China)
Download: PDF(296 KB)  
Export: BibTeX | EndNote | Reference Manager | ProCite | RefWorks    
Abstract  In behavioral economics, the endowment effect refers to the phenomenon that people usually demand higher prices to relinquish an object they own than they would be willing to pay to acquire the object they did not own (Kahneman, Knetsch, & Thaler, 1990). For this phenomenon, dozens of studies have found the significant disparity between willingness to pay (WTA) and willingness to accept compensation (WTA), and this disparity is believed as an important reason to account for this effect. However, the WTA-WTP disparity cannot adequately interpret the endowment effect because sellers or buyers may be irrational traders. Therefore it is necessary to compare the endowment effect with the rational behavior. For this reason, in the present study we introduced the neutrals whose evaluation was regarded as reference price for reinterpreting endowment effect. In addition, one notable weakness of the previous researches in this domain is that the time period of the ownership is ignored. Although endowment effect can come from an instant valuation, the ownership in real life is dynamic and it may change with the time. Thereby, it is necessary to weigh the endowment effect on a dynamic time of change. Considering that, we inspected the changes of the WTA-WTP disparity in two continuous time periods including “past-present” and “present-future”. In this study, we posited that valuation of sellers or buyers was decreased both in the “past-present” and “present-future” time periods, whereas WTA-WTP disparity was unchanged. When the valuation of neutrals as reference price (market price, MP) was introduced, WTA-WTP disparity would be divided into two parts: WTA-MP and MP-WTP. We inferred that MP would decline into a range between WTA and WTP, and the variation tendency of WTA-MP would be different from MP-WTP in a given time period. A total of 674 undergraduates and 35 computer sellers participated in three experiments. In Experiment 1, the participants were randomly assigned to each of 12 scenarios: 3 roles (seller, buyer, and neutral) × 4 time periods from past to present (3 months, 6 months, 12 months, and 24 months). Participants were asked to fill in expected price for a bike after reading a description including its cost price and usage time. In Experiment 2, participants were randomly assigned to each of 8 scenarios: 2 roles (seller and buyer) × 4 time periods from present to future (3 months, 6 months, 12 months, and 24 months). Besides that, 35 computer sellers were specifically assigned to all of 4 time scenarios as the neutrals. They were asked to fill in expected price for a computer after reading a description including its cost price, usage time and configuration. The results revealed that: (1) in all three experiments, selling price was always higher than the buying price, and this gap (WTA-WTP) was even unchanged both in the “past-present” and “present-future” time periods. (2) When valuation of neutrals was introduced as reference price, selling price was closed to reference price in three months, and their gap (WTA-MP) gradually increased following the time prolonging of the ownership. Whereas buying price was closed to reference price in a long time period of two years, and their gap (MP-WTP) gradually decreased following the time prolonging of the ownership. To sum up, the present study showed that endowment effect in trade stably existed even in a long time period. Whereas the introduction of the neutrals, which take the endowment effect into a dynamic time course, could provide a new perspective for the contestable previous viewpoints.
Keywords endowment effect      neutral      seller      buyer      time     
Corresponding Authors: ZHANG Qinlin   
Issue Date: 20 March 2013
Service
E-mail this article
E-mail Alert
RSS
Articles by authors
Cite this article:   
. Introducing Neutrals: A Reinterpretation of the Endowment Effect[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica,2013, 45(3): 345-352.
URL:  
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2013.00345     OR     http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/Y2013/V45/I3/345
[1] HOU Lulu, CHEN Lirong, ZHOU Renlai. Altered reward processing in women with premenstrual syndrome: Evidence from ERPs and time-frequency analysis[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2020, 52(6): 742-757.
[2] WANG Panpan,HE Jiamei. Effects of episodic foresight on intertemporal decision-making[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2020, 52(1): 38-54.
[3] LIU Jingyuan,LI Hong. How state anxiety influences time perception: Moderated mediating effect of cognitive appraisal and attentional bias[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(7): 747-758.
[4] WANG Peng,WANG Xiaotian,GAO Juan,LI Xialan,XU Jing. Adaptive time management: The effects of death awareness on time perception and intertemporal choice[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(12): 1341-1350.
[5] XU Lan,CHEN Quan,CUI Nan,LU Kaili. Enjoy the present or wait for the future? Effects of individuals’ view of time on intertemporal choice[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(1): 96-105.
[6] Yiping ZHONG, Wenjie ZHANG, Yalei LI, Wei FAN. Influence of time stress on mood-congruent false memories[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(9): 929-939.
[7] Mingliang ZHANG, Jiwei SI, Weixing YANG, Shufen XING, Hongxia LI, Jiajia ZHANG. Interaction effects between BDNF gene rs6265 polymorphism and parent-involved education on basic mathematical ability in primary school children[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(9): 1007-1017.
[8] LI Aimei, WANG Haixia, SUN Hailong, XIONG Guanxing, YANG Shaoli . The nudge effect of “foresight for the future of our children”: Pregnancy and environmental intertemporal choice[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(8): 858-867.
[9] Erjia XU, Xue SUI. Effects of predictability on the time course of identity information and location information in Chinese word recognition[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(6): 606-621.
[10] FU Chao, ZHANG Zhen, HE Jinzhou, HUANG Silin, QIU Jianyin, WANG Yiwen.  Brain dynamics of decision-making in the generalized trust game: Evidence from ERPs and EEG time-frequency analysis[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(3): 317-326.
[11] SHAO Jianping, HAN Xue, LIU Wumei. The influence and mechanism of external environment resource scarcity on employees remuneration preference[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(12): 1428-1437.
[12] Xuejun BAI,Haijuan YAO. Differences in cognitive inhibition between persons with high and low creativity: Evidences from behavioral and physiological studies[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(11): 1197-1211.
[13] Heng LI,Yu CAO. The influence of temporal focus on implicit space-time mappings on the front-back axis: Evidence from Han and Qiang Chinese[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(10): 1083-1093.
[14] GONG Shaoying, SHANGGUAN Chenyu, ZHAI Kuihu, GUO Yawei.  The effects of emotional design on multimedia learning[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(6): 771-782.
[15] PAN Yingqiu.  Long-term effects of choice of major, social support, learning engagement on college students’ interest in their major[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(12): 1513-1523.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
Copyright © Acta Psychologica Sinica
Support by Beijing Magtech