Please wait a minute...
   2012, Vol. 44 Issue (5) : 634-646     DOI:
|
Six-year-old Children’s Ability on Category Learning: Category Representation, Attention and Learning Strategy
LIU Zhi-Ya;SONG Xiao-Hong;Carol A. SEGER
(1 Center for Studies of Psychological Application, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China)
(2 Colorado State University, Fort Collins, USA, CO 80523)
Download: PDF(371 KB)  
Export: BibTeX | EndNote | Reference Manager | ProCite | RefWorks    
Abstract  This paper explores 6-year-old children’s category representation and learning strategies. Category learning is a fundamental ability through which human beings acquire and organize new knowledge about the world (Ashby, 2005), and is critical for normal cognitive development.
There are three major theories or models of how categories are represented: Rule-based, Prototype-based, and Exemplar-based models. Rule-based models assume that category learning is a process of discovering an explicit rule to maximize accuracy (Ashby, 2005; Seger, 2006). Prototype-based models assume that stimuli are categorized on the basis of their similarity to category prototypes stored in memory (Rosch & Mervis, 1975; Smith, Chapman, & Redford, 2010; Coutinho, Redford, & Smith, 2010). A category prototype is generally defined as the average, or most typical, member of a category. Exemplar-based models assume that the categorization of a new exemplar is based on the similarity of the new exemplar to the representations of all previously encountered exemplars stored in memory (Medin & Schaffer, 1978; Kruschke, 1992; Nosofsky, 1992).
Previous studies suggest that 6-year-old children have developed some ability to use category knowledge to solve problems (Wilburn & Feeney, 2008; Sloutsky & Lo, 1999; Sloutsky & Fisher, 2001). Furthermore, several critical aspects of category learning are acquired at this age. Fang, Fang, & Xi (1991) pointed out that 6-year-old is a critical period for children to learn to understand the relation between the whole and the part of a subject. Yin (1996) further suggested that 6 years is an important age to learn superordinate categories (for example, the category “furniture”).
Two category structures were used in this study. Experiment 1 used the “5/4 category structure” from Medin and Schaffer (1978) and experiment 2 used the “3/3 category structure” from Yamauchi, Love, & Markman (2002). The category structures were adapted in order to be able to identify which kinds of representation the children were forming: rule, exemplar or prototype. 62 6-year-old children took part in the experiments. During each trial, an individual exemplar was presented, the participant was asked to infer and indicate which category (A or B) the exemplar belonged to, and feedback as to whether the subject was right or wrong was provided. After a number of such trials of inference and feedback, participants reached the learning criterion and were considered to have formed new category knowledge. A mathematical technique of “Model Fitting” was introduced to analyze the data from two experiments. Different models were used to examine whether childrens’ responses were best fit by exemplar or prototype models, to identify which features the children paid attention to, and to identify which classification strategy children used.
Experiment 1 showed that 6-year-old children were able to learn the 5/4 categorization task and reach criterion. Model fitting analyses of category representation found that these children tended to form exemplar representations rather than prototype representations. On measures of distribution of attention, 6-year-old children could identify and pay more attention to the more typical dimensions. Finally, when learning strategy was examined, 6-year-old children used either a single-dimension or rule-plus-exception strategy to classify the items. Experiment 2 using the 3/3 task found similar results to experiment 1, but further found that 6-year-old children could not integrate their processing of the most important distinctive dimensions across the two categories. This result was consistent with the findings of Inhelder & Piaget (1958), and Anaki & Bentin (2009) that 6-year-old children could not process information across different categories.
Keywords category learning      children      prototype      exemplar      categorization strategy     
Corresponding Authors: LIU Zhi-Ya;Carol A. SEGER   
Issue Date: 28 May 2012
Service
E-mail this article
E-mail Alert
RSS
Articles by authors
LIU Zhi-Ya
SONG Xiao-Hong
Carol A. SEGER
Cite this article:   
LIU Zhi-Ya,SONG Xiao-Hong,Carol A. SEGER. Six-year-old Children’s Ability on Category Learning: Category Representation, Attention and Learning Strategy[J]. , 2012, 44(5): 634-646.
URL:  
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/     OR     http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/Y2012/V44/I5/634
[1] Xinghua FAN, Xiaoyi FANG, Yuesheng HUANG, Fengju CHEN, Si YU. The influence mechanism of parental care on depression among left-behind rural children in China: A longitudinal study[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(9): 1029-1040.
[2] Mingliang ZHANG, Jiwei SI, Weixing YANG, Shufen XING, Hongxia LI, Jiajia ZHANG. Interaction effects between BDNF gene rs6265 polymorphism and parent-involved education on basic mathematical ability in primary school children[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(9): 1007-1017.
[3] CHENG Yahua, WANG Jian, WU Xinchun.  The role of morphological awareness in Chinese children’s reading comprehension: The mediating effect of word reading fluency[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(4): 413-425.
[4] YANG Wenjing, JIN Yule, QIU Jiang, ZHANG Qinglin.  The effect of prototype difficulty and semantic similarity on the prototype activation[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(3): 260-269.
[5] PENG Jian, WANG Zhen.  Being a prototypic follower: Burdening or enabling? The paradoxical effect of followership prototype-trait match[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(2): 216-225.
[6] WANG Yan, QIAN Xiaoyun, TIAN Qian, GAO Jun, LI Xiaoxu.  Parent-child relationship and the comparison between parents and their children on their children’s mate preference[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(1): 91-100.
[7] LIANG Feifei; WANG Yongsheng; YANG Wen; BAI Xuejun. The modulation of reading level on the development of children’s eye movement characteristics: Evidence from 9- to 11-year-old children[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(4): 450-459.
[8] GUO Haiying; CHEN Lihua; YE Zhi; PAN Jin; LIN Danhua. Characteristics of peer victimization and the bidirectional relationship between peer victimization and internalizing problems among rural-to-urban migrant children in China: A longitudinal study[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(3): 336-348.
[9] LIU Wen, ZHANG Xue, ZHANG Yu, YU Ruiwei.  Fairness cognition-behavior gap in 4~8 year-old children: The role of social comparison[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(12): 1504-1512.
[10] HU Qingfen; LU Jing. The combination of self-position and self-orientation in children’s map task[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2016, 48(9): 1143-1150.
[11] PENG Jian; WANG Xiao. I will perform effectively if you are with me: Leader-follower congruence in followership prototype, job engagement and job performance[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2016, 48(9): 1151-1162.
[12] ZHANG Lijin; BI Yuan; LIANG Yi; LIU Minhong. Early screening and dynamic intervention of children with insufficient number sense[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2016, 48(7): 804-817.
[13] ZHAO Ying; CHENG Yahua; WU Xinchun; NGUYEN Thi Phuong. The reciprocal relationship between morphological awareness and vocabulary knowledge among Chinese children: A longitudinal study[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2016, 48(11): 1434-1444.
[14] LI Liping; WU Xinchun; ZHOU Ningning; CHENG Yahua; NGUYEN Thi Phuong. The cognitive characteristics and influential factors of Chinese word callers in primary school[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2016, 48(10): 1270-1281.
[15] LI Yanfang; LIU Lijun; LV Ying; LUO Fang; WANG Yun. The Influence of Parent-child Relationship and Teacher-child Relational #br# Climate on Rural Migrant Children’s Early Behavior Problems[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(7): 914-927.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
Copyright © Acta Psychologica Sinica
Support by Beijing Magtech