Please wait a minute...
Acta Psychologica Sinica    2020, Vol. 52 Issue (1) : 26-37     DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.00026
Reports of Empirical Studies |
Acute psychological stress impairs attention disengagement toward threat-related stimuli
LUO Yu1,2,NIAN Jingqing1,2,BAO Wei1,2,ZHANG Jingjing1,2,ZHAO Shouying1,PAN Yun1,XU Shuang1,2,ZHANG Yu1,2()
1 School of Psychology, Guizhou Normal University, Guiyang 550025, China
2 Stress and Attention Laboratory, School of Psychology, Guizhou Normal University, Guiyang 550025, China
Download: PDF(985 KB)   HTML Review File (1 KB) 
Export: BibTeX | EndNote | Reference Manager | ProCite | RefWorks    
Abstract  

Threat stimuli catch our attention when compared with neutral stimuli called attention bias, which includes facilitating attention engagement and difficult attention disengagement to threat. Acute stress influences our attention to threat. However, we do not know whether acute stress can enhance facilitating attention engagement or impair attention disengagement toward threat. Therefore, the present study investigated whether attention engagement to threat is enhanced or attention disengagement to threat is weakened when people are stressed.
Thirty-six healthy male adults were randomly assigned to a stress group (n = 18) and a control group (n = 18). The stress group underwent socially evaluated cold pressor test (SECPT), whereas the control group underwent a warm water control protocol. The dot-probe task was used to measure the attention bias toward threat. Event-related potentials (ERPs) were used in conjunction with reaction time measures to investigate the time course of attention to threat in the dot-probe task. The N2-posteior-contralateral (N2pc) component measured the initial shift of visual attention to the threatening stimulus, whereas the sustained posterior contralateral negativity (SPCN) component measured the maintenance of visual attention to the threatening stimulus. Reaction time, accuracy rate, and the electroencephalography data of the participants were recorded during the dot-probe task. The state anxiety questionnaire and saliva were acquired at five time points, such as 85 and 70 minutes before the SECPT, immediately before and after the dot-probe task, and 70 minutes after the SECPT.
The SECPT successfully induced stress response. Participants in the stress group showed stronger state anxiety and Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal axis response indicated by increased salivary cortisol concentration after the SECPT than the control group. In addition, no significant differences were found before the SECPT. At the behavioral level, the attention disengagement in the stress group was slower than in the control group. Regarding ERPs, we found a greater amplitude of SPCN (300~600 ms after cue) in the stress group than in the control group. However, no significant effect was found on the amplitude of N2pc between stress group and control group.
These results indicate that attention disengagement toward threat is impaired under acute stress situations.

Keywords acute stress      attention engagement      attention disengagement      N2pc      SPCN     
:  B845  
Corresponding Authors: Yu ZHANG     E-mail: yuzhang331@163.com
Issue Date: 21 November 2019
Service
E-mail this article
E-mail Alert
RSS
Articles by authors
Cite this article:   
LUO Yu,NIAN Jingqing,BAO Wei,ZHANG Jingjing,ZHAO Shouying,PAN Yun,XU Shuang,ZHANG Yu. Acute psychological stress impairs attention disengagement toward threat-related stimuli[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica,2020, 52(1): 26-37.
URL:  
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.00026     OR     http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/Y2020/V52/I1/26
[1] Armstrong T., & Olatunji B. O . ( 2012). Eye tracking of attention in the affective disorders: A meta-analytic review and synthesis. Clinical Psychology Review, 32( 8), 704-723.
[2] Arnsten A. F. T . ( 2009). Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10( 6), 410-422.
[3] Boudewyn M. A., Luck S. J., Farrens J. L., & Kappenman E. S . ( 2018). How many trials does it take to get a significant ERP effect? It depends. Psychophysiology, 55( 6), e13049.
[4] Carlson J. M., Beacher F., Reinke K. S., Habib R., Harmon-Jones E., Mujica-Parodi L. R., & Hajcak G . ( 2012). Nonconscious attention bias to threat is correlated with anterior cingulate cortex gray matter volume: A voxel- based morphometry result and replication. NeuroImage, 59( 2), 1713-1718.
[5] Carlson J. M., & Reinke K. S . ( 2008). Masked fearful faces modulate the orienting of covert spatial attention. Emotion, 8( 4), 522-529.
[6] Cisler J. M., & Koster E. H. W . ( 2010). Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30( 2), 203-216.
[7] Cooper R. M., & Langton S. R. H . ( 2006). Attentional bias to angry faces using the dot-probe task? It depends when you look for it. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44( 9), 1321-1329.
[8] Cousijn H., Rijpkema M., Qin S. Z., van Marle H. J. F., Franke B., Hermans E. J., … Fernández G . ( 2010). Acute stress modulates genotype effects on amygdala processing in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107( 21), 9867-9872.
[9] Dȩbiec J., & LeDoux J. E . ( 2006). Noradrenergic signaling in the amygdala contributes to the reconsolidation of fear memory: Treatment implications for PTSD. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1071, 521-524.
[10] de Kloet E. R., Joëls M., & Holsboer F . ( 2005). Stress and the brain: From adaptation to disease. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6( 6), 463-475.
[11] Fecteau J. H., & Munoz D. P . ( 2006). Salience, relevance, and firing: A priority map for target selection. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10( 8), 382-390.
[12] Fernández G., Weis S., Stoffel-Wagner B., Tendolkar I., Reuber M., Beyenburg S., … Elger C. E . ( 2003). Menstrual cycle-dependent neural plasticity in the adult human brain is hormone, task, and region specific. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23( 9), 3790-3795.
[13] Fox E., Russo R., Bowles R., & Dutton K . ( 2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130( 4), 681-700.
[14] Garrett N., González-Garzón A. M., Foulkes L., Levita L., & Sharot T . ( 2018). Updating beliefs under perceived threat. SSRN Electronic Journal, 38( 36), 7901-7911.
[15] Gaspelin N., & Luck S. J . ( 2018). Combined electrophysiological and behavioral evidence for the suppression of salient distractors. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 30( 9), 1265-1280.
[16] Gindt M., Nachon O., Chanquoy L., Faure S., & Garcia R . ( 2017). Attentional bias in post-traumatic stress symptoms or anxiety. European Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 1( 3), 159-164.
[17] Gottlieb J . ( 2007). From thought to action: The parietal cortex as a bridge between perception, action, and cognition. Neuron, 53( 1), 9-16.
[18] Grant D. M., Judah M. R., White E. J., & Mills A. C . ( 2015). Worry and discrimination of threat and safety cues: An event-related potential investigation. Behavior Therapy, 46( 5), 652-660.
[19] Hermans E. J., Henckens M. J. A. G., Joëls M., & Fernández G . ( 2014). Dynamic adaptation of large-scale brain networks in response to acute stressors. Trends in Neurosciences, 37( 6), 304-314.
[20] Hermans E. J., van Marle H. J. F., Ossewaarde L., Henckens M. J. A. G., Qin S. Z., van Kesteren M. T. R., … Fernández G . ( 2011). Stress-related noradrenergic activity prompts large-scale neural network reconfiguration. Science, 334( 6059), 1151-1153.
[21] Holmes A., Bradley B. P., Kragh Nielsen M., & Mogg K . ( 2009). Attentional selectivity for emotional faces: Evidence from human electrophysiology. Psychophysiology, 46( 1), 62-68.
[22] Holmes A., Mogg K., de Fockert J., Nielsen M. K., & Bradley B. P . ( 2014). Electrophysiological evidence for greater attention to threat when cognitive control resources are depleted. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 14( 2), 827-835.
[23] Jiang C. H., Buchanan T. W., Yao Z. X., Zhang K., Wu J. H., & Zhang L . ( 2017). Acute psychological stress disrupts attentional bias to threat-related stimuli. Scientific Reports, 7( 1), 14607.
[24] Kappenman E. S., Farrens J. L., Luck S. J., & Proudfit G. H . ( 2014). Behavioral and ERP measures of attentional bias to threat in the dot-probe task: Poor reliability and lack of correlation with anxiety. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1368.
[25] Kappenman E. S., MacNamara A., & Proudfit G. H . ( 2015). Electrocortical evidence for rapid allocation of attention to threat in the dot-probe task. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10( 4), 577-583.
[26] Koolhaas J. M., Bartolomucci A., Buwalda B., de Boer S. F., Flügge G., Korte S. M., … Fuchs E . ( 2011). Stress revisited: A critical evaluation of the stress concept. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35( 5), 1291-1301.
[27] Koster E. H. W., Crombez G., Verschuere B., & de Houwer J . ( 2004). Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: Differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42( 10), 1183-1192.
[28] Koster E. H. W., Crombez G., Verschuere B., van Damme S., & Wiersema J. R . ( 2006). Components of attentional bias to threat in high trait anxiety: Facilitated engagement, impaired disengagement, and attentional avoidance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44( 12), 1757-1771.
[29] LeDoux J . ( 2003). The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala. Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology, 23( 4-5), 727-738.
[30] Löw A., Weymar M., & Hamm A. O . ( 2015). When threat is near, get out of here: Dynamics of defensive behavior during freezing and active avoidance. Psychological Science, 26( 11), 1706-1716.
[31] Luo Y., Fernández G., Hermans E., Vogel S., Zhang Y., Li H., & Klumpers F . ( 2018). How acute stress may enhance subsequent memory for threat stimuli outside the focus of attention: DLPFC-amygdala decoupling. NeuroImage, 171, 311-322.
[32] Luo Y. J., Lin W. J., Wu J. H., & Qin S. Z . ( 2013). Cognitive neuroscience of stress. Progress in Physiological Sciences, 44( 5), 345-353.
[32] [ 罗跃嘉, 林婉君, 吴健辉, 秦绍正 . ( 2013). 应激的认知神经科学研究. 生理科学进展, 44( 5), 2345-2353.]
[33] Meconi F., Luria R., & Sessa P . ( 2014). Individual differences in anxiety predict neural measures of visual working memory for untrustworthy faces. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9( 12), 1872-1879.
[34] Peckham A. D., McHugh R. K., & Otto M. W . ( 2010). A meta-analysis of the magnitude of biased attention in depression. Depression and Anxiety, 27( 12), 1135-1142.
[35] Qin S. Z., Hermans E. J., van Marle H. J. F., Luo J., & Fernández G . ( 2009). Acute psychological stress reduces working memory-related activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Biological Psychiatry, 66( 1), 25-32.
[36] Rued H. A., Hilmert C. J., Strahm A. M., & Thomas L. E . ( 2019). The influence of stress on attentional bias to threat: An angry face and a noisy crowd. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 26( 3), 943-950.
[37] Salemink E., van den Hout M. A., & Kindt M . ( 2007). Selective attention and threat: Quick orienting versus slow disengagement and two versions of the dot probe task. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45( 3), 607-615.
[38] Sänger J., Bechtold L., Schoofs D., Blaszkewicz M., & Wascher E . ( 2014). The influence of acute stress on attention mechanisms and its electrophysiological correlates. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 353.
[39] Sawaki R., Geng J. J., & Luck S. J . ( 2012). A common neural mechanism for preventing and terminating the allocation of attention. Journal of Neuroscience, 32( 31), 10725-10736.
[40] Schwabe L., Haddad L., & Schachinger H . ( 2008). HPA axis activation by a socially evaluated cold-pressor test. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33( 6), 890-895.
[41] Schwabe L., Höffken O., Tegenthoff M., & Wolf O. T . ( 2013). Stress-induced enhancement of response inhibition depends on mineralocorticoid receptor activation. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38( 10), 2319-2326.
[42] Schwabe L., Joëls M., Roozendaal B., Wolf O. T., & Oitzl M. S . ( 2012). Stress effects on memory: An update and integration. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36( 7), 1740-1749.
[43] Schwabe L., & Schächinger H . ( 2018). Ten years of research with the Socially Evaluated Cold Pressor Test: Data from the past and guidelines for the future. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 92, 155-161.
[44] Sessa P., Luria R., Gotler A., Jolicœur P., & Dell'acqua R . ( 2011). Interhemispheric ERP asymmetries over inferior parietal cortex reveal differential visual working memory maintenance for fearful versus neutral facial identities. Psychophysiology, 48( 2), 187-197.
[45] Shackman A. J., Maxwell J. S., McMenamin B. W., Greischar L. L., & Davidson R. J . ( 2011). Stress potentiates early and attenuates late stages of visual processing. Journal of Neuroscience, 31( 3), 1156-1161.
[46] Spielberger C. D . ( 1970). STAI manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Self-Evaluation Questionnaire, 1-24.
[47] Sun M., Wang E., Huang J., Zhao C., Guo J., Li D., .. Song Y . ( 2018). Attentional selection and suppression in children and adults. Developmental Science, 21( 6), e12684.
[48] Taylor C. T., Cross K., & Amir N . ( 2016). Attentional control moderates the relationship between social anxiety symptoms and attentional disengagement from threatening information. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 50, 68-76.
[49] Ulrich-Lai Y. M., & Herman J. P . ( 2009). Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress responses. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10( 6), 397-409.
[50] van Marle H. J. F., Hermans E. J., Qin S. Z., & Fernández G . ( 2009). From specificity to sensitivity: How acute stress affects amygdala processing of biologically salient stimuli. Biological Psychiatry, 66( 7), 649-655.
[51] Wang E., Sun L., Sun M., Huang J., Tao Y., Zhao X. X., ... Song Y . ( 2016). Attentional selection and suppression in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 1( 4), 372-380.
[52] Wang Y., & Luo Y. J . ( 2005). Standardization and assessment of college students' facial expression of emotion. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 13( 4), 396-398.
[52] [ 王妍, 罗跃嘉 . ( 2005). 大学生面孔表情材料的标准化及其评定. 中国临床心理学杂志, 13 ( 4), 396-398.]
[53] Weerda R., Muehlhan M., Wolf O. T., & Thiel C. M . ( 2010). Effects of acute psychosocial stress on working memory related brain activity in men. Human Brain Mapping, 31( 9), 1418-1429.
[54] Weymar M., Schwabe L., Löw A., & Hamm A. O . ( 2012). Stress sensitizes the brain: Increased processing of unpleasant pictures after exposure to acute stress. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24( 7), 1511-1518.
[55] Wu J. H., Yuan Y. R., Duan H. X., Qin S. Z., Buchanan T. W., Zhang K., & Zhang L . ( 2014). Long-term academic stress increases the late component of error processing: An ERP study. Biological Psychology, 99, 77-82.
[56] Zhang Y., Luo Y., Zhao S. Y., Chen W., & Li H . ( 2014). Attentional bias towards threat: Facilitated attentional orienting or impaired attentional disengagement? Advances in Psychological Science, 22( 7), 1129-1138.
[56] [ 张禹, 罗禹, 赵守盈, 陈维, 李红 . ( 2014). 对威胁刺激的注意偏向: 注意定向加速还是注意解除困难?. 心理科学进展, 22( 7), 1129-1138.]
[1] HU Na,CHEN Antao,WANG Yanqing,LI Qing,XU Zhenzhen,LONG Quanshan. Acute stress impairs error monitoring and post-error adjustment[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2020, 52(2): 162-172.
[2] Sisi WANG,Yixuan KU. The causal role of right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in visual working memory[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(7): 727-738.
[3] HU Yanmei; ZHANG Ming. Electrophysiological evidence for memory-based attentional capture and memory-based attentional rejection effects[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2016, 48(1): 12-21.
[4] Li Huanhuan,Lin-Wenjuan,Li Junfa. The Effects of Acute Emotional Stressor on Behavior and Phosphorylation Levels of Neurogranin in Rats[J]. , 2006, 38(04): 576-582.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
Copyright © Acta Psychologica Sinica
Support by Beijing Magtech