Please wait a minute...
Acta Psychologica Sinica    2018, Vol. 50 Issue (12) : 1390-1399     DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.01390
Reports of Empirical Studies |
Preschoolers' selective trust and belief revision in conflicting situation
LI Tingyu1,2,LIU Li1,2,LI Yilin1,2,ZHU Liqi1()
1 CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Beijing 100101, China
2 University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China
Download: PDF(1230 KB)   HTML Review File (1 KB) 
Export: BibTeX | EndNote | Reference Manager | ProCite | RefWorks     Supporting Info
Guide   
Abstract  

One of the most imperative issues in developmental research on social cognition is whether young children selectively trust informants' testimonies and revise their beliefs based on those testimonies. Previous research has shown that both the nature of the information and the traits of informants affect young children's selective trust and belief revision. However, the role that contradictory information may play in young children's selective trust and belief revision has yet to be examined. The present study examined Chinese preschoolers' selective trust and belief revision in situations in which their beliefs were contradicted by information provided by a familiar informant (the mother).

The present study adopted the conflicting sources paradigm. Testimonies about hybrid pictures with different perceptual cues (the 50%-50% hybrids task vs the 75%-25% hybrids task) were presented to 74 4- to 6-year-old preschoolers, to investigate young children's selective trust and belief revision. Participants heard two informants (the mother and the stranger) name the hybrid differently. In all tasks, the stranger’s naming was more consistent with the perceptual cues. Children were asked about their own naming (What animal is this?), whom they would like to ask (mother or stranger?), whom to believe and the endorsement question (What animal do you think this is?).

The results showed that children's selective trust and belief revision are influenced by the degree of conflict between mother's testimonies and perceptual evidence. In the low conflicting situation (50%-50% hybrids task), children are more inclined to ask for and explicitly trust the mother's testimony than in the high conflicting situation (75%-25% hybrids task). Furthermore, in the low conflicting situation, older children are more likely to revise their beliefs than younger children; whereas in the high conflicting situation, older children are more hesitant to revise their beliefs than younger children. The findings indicate that older children are more flexible in selective trust and belief revision. They are better able to factor the available perceptual cues into their consideration of the adult's testimony.

In summary, 4- to 6-year-old preschoolers' selective trust and belief revision are influenced by the degree of conflict between others' testimonies and perceptual evidence. In high conflicting situations, they typically refuse to ask for and endorse the testimonies provided by the familiar informant, or to revise their beliefs. The results of the present study demonstrate that young children are able to distinguish between different degrees of conflict. When the mother's testimony conflicted dramatically with the young children's existing beliefs, older Chinese preschoolers in our study tended to distrust the mother and refused to revise their beliefs.

Keywords 4- to 6-year-old preschoolers      selective trust      belief revision      informant      familiarity     
ZTFLH:  B844  
Issue Date: 30 October 2018
Service
E-mail this article
E-mail Alert
RSS
Articles by authors
LI Tingyu
LIU Li
LI Yilin
ZHU Liqi
Cite this article:   
LI Tingyu,LIU Li,LI Yilin, et al. Preschoolers' selective trust and belief revision in conflicting situation[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(12): 1390-1399.
URL:  
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.01390     OR     http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/Y2018/V50/I12/1390
  
  
任务 实验材料 母亲 陌生人
50%-50%合成动物 牛-马
熊-猪
25%-75%合成动物 鱼-鸟
松鼠-兔子 松鼠 兔子
  
组别 50%-50%合成动物 25%-75%合成动物
4岁组 65.10 69.85
5岁组 63.95 63.90
6岁组 88.90** 88.10**
  
组别 50%-50%合成动物 25%-75%合成动物
4岁组 56.35 14.30**
5岁组 53.30 8.20**
6岁组 67.45 9.55**
  
组别 50%-50%合成动物 25%-75%合成动物
初始 最终 初始 最终
4岁组 12.70** 32.55 1.60** 13.50**
5岁组 21.45* 34.45 1.65** 9.05**
6岁组 12.70** 32.55 0.00** 9.55**
  
1 Akhtar N., &Jaswal , V. K . ( 2013). Deficit or difference? Interpreting diverse developmental paths: An introduction to the special section. Developmental Psychology, 49( 1), 1-3.
pmid: 23316772 url: http://med.wanfangdata.com.cn/Paper/Detail/PeriodicalPaper_PM23316772
2 Alchourrón C. E., G?rdenfors P., & Makinson D. C . ( 2014). On the logic of theory change: Partial meet contraction and revision functions. Journal of Symbolic Logic, 50( 2), 510-530.
3 Amsterlaw J., &Wellman , H. M . ( 2006). Theories of mind in transition: A microgenetic study of the development of false belief understanding. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7( 2), 139-172.
url: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1207/s15327647jcd0702_1
4 Bascandziev I., &Harris P.L, . ( 2011). Gravity is not the only ruler for falling events: Young children stop making the gravity error after receiving additional perceptual information about the tubes mechanism. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109( 4), 468-477.
pmid: 21497828 url: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022096511000919
5 Bascandziev I., &Harris P.L, . ( 2014). In beauty we trust: children prefer information from more attractive informants. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 32( 1), 94-99.
pmid: 24164592 url: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/bjdp.12022/full
6 Birch S. A. J., Akmal N., & Frampton K. L . ( 2010). Two-year-olds are vigilant of others’ non-verbal cues to credibility. Developmental Science, 13( 2), 363-369.
pmid: 20136933 url: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00906.x/full
7 Boseovski J.J., &Thurman S.L, . ( 2014). Evaluating and approaching a strange animal: Children's trust in informant testimony. Child Development, 85( 2), 824-834.
pmid: 24032359 url: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/cdev.12156/full
8 Carey S.., ( 1991). Knowledge acquisition: enrichment or conceptual change? In S. Carey & R. Gelman (Eds.), The epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and cognition (pp. 257-291). Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
url: http://psycnet.apa.org/record/1991-97841-009
9 Chan, C. C. Y., &Tardif , T. ( 2013). Knowing better: The role of prior knowledge and culture in trust in testimony. Developmental Psychology, 49( 3), 591-601.
pmid: 23294151 url: http://med.wanfangdata.com.cn/Paper/Detail/PeriodicalPaper_PM23294151
10 Chen X. Y., Rubin K. H., Liu M. W., Chen H. C., Wang L., Li D., .. Li B. S . ( 2003). Compliance in Chinese and Canadian toddlers: A cross-cultural study. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27( 5), 428-436.
url: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/01650250344000046
11 Chouinard M. M., Harris P. L., & Maratsos M. P . ( 2007). Children's questions: A mechanism for cognitive development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 72( 1), Serial No. 286, i-129.
pmid: 17394580 url: http://www.jstor.org/stable/30163594
12 Clément F., Koenig M., & Harris P . ( 2004). The ontogenesis of trust. Mind & Language, 19 (4), 360-379.
13 Coady C. AJ.. ( 1992) . Testimony: A philosophical study Oxford: Clarendon Press A philosophical study. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
14 Corriveau K., &Harris P.L, . ( 2009). Choosing your informant: Weighing familiarity and recent accuracy. Developmental Science, 12( 3), 426-437.
pmid: 19371367 url: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00792.x/full
15 , Corriveau K.H., &Harris P.L, . ( 2010). Preschoolers (sometimes) defer to the majority in making simple perceptual judgments. Developmental Psychology, 46( 2), 437-445.
pmid: 20210502 url: http://med.wanfangdata.com.cn/Paper/Detail/PeriodicalPaper_PM20210502
16 Corriveau K. H., Harris P. L., Meins E., Fernyhough C., Arnott B., Elliott L., .. de Rosnay M . ( 2009). Young children’s trust in their mother’s claims: Longitudinal links with attachment security in infancy. Child Development, 80( 3), 750-761.
pmid: 1948990119489901 url: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01295.x/full
17 de Leeuw N. , ( 1993, June). Students’ beliefs about the circulatory system: are misconceptions universal? In Proceedings of the Fifteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 389-393), Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
18 Doan T., Denison S., Lucas C., & Gopnik A . ( 2015). Learning to reason about desires: An infant training study. In D. C. Noelle et al. (Eds.), Proceedings for the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 578-583). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
19 Einav S., &Robinson , E. J . ( 2010). Children's sensitivity to error magnitude when evaluating informants. Cognitive Development, 25( 3), 218-232.
url: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201410000328
20 Ganea P. A., Shutts K., Spelke E. S., & DeLoache J. S . ( 2007). Thinking of things unseen: Infants' use of language to update mental representations. Psychological Science, 18( 8), 734-739.
pmid: 17680946 url: http://www.jstor.org/stable/40064767
21 Ho D. Y. F.( 1986) . Chinese patterns of socialization: a critical review In M H Bond (Ed), The psychology of the Chinese people (pp 1-37) New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press a critical review. In M. H. Bond (Ed.), The psychology of the Chinese people. (pp. 1-37). New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press.
22 Jaswal V.K . ( 2004). Don't believe everything you hear: Preschoolers' sensitivity to speaker intent in category induction. Child Development, 75( 6), 1871-1885.
pmid: 15566385 url: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00822.x/full
23 Jaswal V.K . ( 2006). Preschoolers favor the creator's label when reasoning about an artifact's function. Cognition, 99( 3), B83-B92.
pmid: 16185679 url: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027705001423
24 Jaswal V.K., &Malone , L. S . ( 2007). Turning believers into skeptics: 3-year-olds' sensitivity to cues to speaker credibility. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8( 3), 263-283.
url: http://psycnet.apa.org/record/2007-13536-001
25 Jaswal V.K., &Markman , E. M . ( 2007). Looks aren't everything: 24-month-olds' willingness to accept unexpected labels. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8( 1), 93-111.
26 Jaswal V. K., Pérez-Edgar K., Kondrad RL., Palmquist C. M., Cole C. A., & Cole C. E . ( 2014). Can't stop believing: Inhibitory control and resistance to misleading testimony. Developmental Science, 17( 6), 965-976.
pmid: 24806881 url: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/desc.12187/full
27 Kinzler K. D., Corriveau K. H., & Harris P. L . ( 2011). Children's selective trust in native-accented speakers. Developmental Science, 14( 1), 106-111.
28 Koenig M.A., &Echols , C. H . ( 2003). Infants' understanding of false labeling events: The referential roles of words and the speakers who use them. Cognition, 87( 3), 179-208.
pmid: 12684199 url: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027703000027
29 Kushnir T., &Gopnik A. , ( 2007). Conditional probability versus spatial contiguity in causal learning: Preschoolers use new contingency evidence to overcome prior spatial assumptions. Developmental Psychology, 43( 1), 186-196.
pmid: 17201518 url: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/resolve/reference/PMED?id=17201518
30 Lane J. D., Harris P. L., Gelman S. A., & Wellman H. M . ( 2014). More than meets the eye: Young children’s trust in claims that defy their perceptions. Developmental Psychology, 50( 3), 865-871.
31 Lucas A. J., Lewis C., Pala F. C., Wong K., & Berridge D . ( 2013). Social-cognitive processes in preschoolers' selective trust: Three cultures compared. Developmental Psychology, 49( 3), 579-590.
pmid: 22946437 url: http://med.wanfangdata.com.cn/Paper/Detail/PeriodicalPaper_PM22946437
32 Lucas C. G., Bridgers S., Griffiths T. L., & Gopnik A . ( 2014). When children are better (or at least more open-minded) learners than adults: Developmental differences in learning the forms of causal relationships. Cognition, 131( 2), 284-299.
pmid: 24566007 url: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027713002540
33 Luo G.., (1996). Chinese traditional social and moral ideas and rules. Beijing, China: The University of Chinese People Press.
34 Melnyk L., Crossman A. M., & Scullin M. H . ( 2007). The suggestibility of children's memory. In M. P. Toglia, J. D. Read, D. F. Ross, & R. C. L. Lindsay (Eds.), The handbook of eyewitness psychology, Vol I: Memory for events. (pp. 401-427). Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
35 Robinson E. J., Mitchell P., & Nye R . ( 1995). Young children's treating of utterances as unreliable sources of knowledge. Journal of Child Language, 22( 3), 663-685.
pmid: 8789518 url: http://europepmc.org/abstract/med/8789518
36 Schauble L ( 1990). Belief revision in children: The role of prior knowledge and strategies for generating evidence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49( 1), 31-57.
pmid: 2303776 url: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/002209659090048D
37 Slaughter V., &Gopnik A. , ( 1996). Conceptual coherence in the child's theory of mind: Training children to understand belief. Child Development, 67( 6), 2967-2988.
pmid: 9071768 url: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01898.x/full
38 Vosniadou S. &Verschaffel L. , ( 2004). Extending the conceptual change approach to mathematics learning and teaching. Learning and Instruction, 14( 5), 445-451.
url: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0959475204000428
39 Young A. G., Alibali M. W., & Kalish C. W . ( 2012). Disagreement and causal learning: Others' hypotheses affect children's evaluations of evidence. Developmental Psychology, 48(5), 1242-1253.
pmid: 22390663 url: http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/dev/48/5/1242/
40 , , Zarbatany L. &Lamb M.E, . ( 1985). Social referencing as a function of information source: Mothers versus strangers. Infant Behavior and Development, 8(1), 25-33.
41 , Zhang Y.H., &Zhu L.Q, . ( 2014). Epistemic trust: How preschoolers selectively learn from others. Advances in Psychological Science,22( 1), 86-96.
url: http://en.cnki.com.cn/Article_en/CJFDTOTAL-XLXD201401010.htm
42 [ 张耀华, 朱莉琪 . ( 2014). 认识性信任: 学龄前儿童的选择性学习. 心理科学进展, 22( 1), 86-96.]
url: http://d.wanfangdata.com.cn/Periodical/xlxdt201401010
[1] LIU Guixiong, JIA Yongping, WANG Yujuan, MAIHEFULAITI·Kanji, GUO Chunyan. The bilingual L2 advantage in associative recognition[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(1): 14-23.
[2] TANG Weihai, ZHONG Rubo, XU Xiaoxu, LIU Xiping. Effects of facial attractiveness and information accuracy on preschoolers’ selective trust[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(1): 71-84.
[3] JIA Yongping; ZHOU Chu; LI Lin; GUO Xiuyan. Recognition without cued recall (RWCR) phenomenon in Chinese characters: Effects of restudying and testing[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2016, 48(2): 111-120.
[4] MAO Xinrui; XU Huifang; GUO Chunyan. Emotional Memory Enhancement Effect in Dual-processing Recognition Retrieval[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(9): 1111-1123.
[5] YE Xiaohong; CHEN Youzhen; MENG Yingfang. Neural Processing of Recollection, Familiarity and Priming at Encoding[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(9): 1101-1110.
[6] WANG Pei;CHEN Li; XIE Yiwen; ZHANG Qin. The Effect of Familiarity and Compatibility on Mental Representation of the Stereotype in Compound Social Categories[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(3): 375-388.
[7] WU Binxing; ZHANG Zhijun; SUN Yusheng. Facial Familiarity Modulates the Interaction between Facial Gender and Emotional Expression[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(10): 1201-1212.
[8] CHEN Yongxiang; ZHU Liqi. Predictors of Action Picture Naming in Mandarin Chinese[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(1): 11-18.
[9] Zhang-Jijia,Kong Changfeng. Effects of Categorical Variables on Fale Recognition[J]. , 2006, 38(03): 324-332.
[10] Li Shouxin,Ding Zhaoye,Zhang Lizeng. THE EFFECT OF COGNITIVE STYLE AND CUE CHARACTERISTICS ON PROSPECTIVE MEMORY[J]. , 2005, 37(03): 320-327.
[11] Guan Yijie,Fang Fuxi (Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101). THE EFFECT OF WORD LEARNING AGE IN PRIMARY SCHOOL CHILDREN'S VISUAL CHINESE CHARACTER RECOGNITION(II)[J]. , 2002, 34(01): 24-29.
[12] Guan Yijie 1,2 Fang Fuxi 2(1Institute of Mental Health and Education,Beijing Normal University,Beijing 100875)(2Institute of Psychology,Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101). THE EFFECT OF WORD LEARNING AGE IN PRIMARY SCHOOL CHILDREN'SVISUAL CHINESE CHARACTER RECOGNITION (I)[J]. , 2001, 33(05): 42-47.
[13] Han Kai , Shen Dawei, Li Bo (Department of Psychology, Peking University, Beijing 100871). RE-VERIFICATION OF THE MECHANISM OF FEELING-OF-KNOWING[J]. , 2001, 33(01): 13-16.
[14] Gao Xiangping (Educational Science Institution, Shanghai Teachers' University, Shanghai 200234). RECOGNIZING-TYPE EXPERIMENTAL SCHEDULE OF PDP AND DISCRIMINABILITY OF STIMULUS RESOURCE[J]. , 2000, 32(04): 381-386.
[15] Yang Zhiliang(Psychology Departent, East China Normal University, Shanghai 200062)Du Jianzheng(Institate of Prychology, Chinese Academp of Sciences, Beijing 10010l). FOK: CUE-FAMILIARITY, OR TARGET-RETRIEVABILITY?[J]. , 2000, 32(03): 241-246.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
Copyright © Acta Psychologica Sinica
Support by Beijing Magtech