Please wait a minute...
Acta Psychologica Sinica    2018, Vol. 50 Issue (7) : 703-714     DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.00703
Reports of Empirical Studies |
Landmark attraction effect and landmark repulsion effect on representational momentum in airplane movement scene
Bihua YAN(),Xiaomin LIU,Haozhe LIU
School of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi′an 710062, China
Download: PDF(568 KB)   HTML Review File (1 KB) 
Export: BibTeX | EndNote | Reference Manager | ProCite | RefWorks     Supporting Info
Guide   
Abstract  

The term representational momentum has been used to describe a forward displacement of the final position of a moving target and also as an explanatory mechanism for that forward displacement. Landmark attraction effect refers to the remembered orientation of a target shifted towards the orientation of a surrounding context object. Previous studies on moving targets and landmarks have been limited to the effects of physical properties, such as the size of the stimulus, or the speed of movement, on the displacement of another object. The present study focused on the effects of the situational significance, the relative relationship between the moving target and the relevant landmark, the movement directions of the target and the presentation time of the landmarks on the judgment of the moving target position.

The inducing movement paradigm was adopted to investigate the representational momentum effect in the flight scene, in which the airplane target and the landmarks were designed as simple line-drawing images. The stationary airstrip and the mountain were classified as safe landmarks and dangerous landmarks, respectively, forming a causal relationship with the aircraft target, which together constituted a specific aircraft movement scene. Four experiments were designed to compare the effects of different landmarks. Experiments 1a and 1b focused on the relationship between the aircraft and the safe landmark or dangerous landmark and the movement direction of the aircraft on the representational momentum, while the landmark remained visible in the induction duration. Experiments 2a and 2b addressed the same variable relationship as in Experiments 1a and 1b except that the landmark was shown during the retention interval.

The findings of the four experiments were as follows. First, significant forward distortions were observed under all conditions with the exception of leftward- toward the dangerous landmark motion, indicating that the representational momentum effect was universal in the flight scene. Second, forward displacement was influenced by target approaches and was larger when targets moved toward the safe landmark than when moving away from the same landmark. In addition, it was also larger when targets moved away from the dangerous landmark than when they moved toward it. The representational momentum of the approaching safe landmark was larger than that of the dangerous landmark, and the momentum of moving away from the safe landmark was smaller than that of the dangerous landmark. In this way, the safe landmark showed landmark an attraction effect, and the dangerous landmark showed a repulsion effect. Third, the direction of movement of the aircraft had no significant effect on the representational momentum, and the representational momentum increased when the safe landmark or the dangerous landmark was shown during the retention interval, demonstrating the effect of the scenario on the directional effect and attention effect of the momentum.

In conclusion, the results of this study suggested that the landmark effect of the representational momentum was influenced by the relevant features of the landmark, which was measured by the causality and the situational significance between the target and the landmark. The memory of the location of the moving object will shift along the direction of the object’s movement, indicating that the momentum effect is difficult to eliminate. However, when there are other related objects (landmarks) in the scene, the nature and the importance of the object has an impact on people’s judgment.

Keywords representational momentum      landmark attraction effect      landmark repulsion effect      scene      forward displacement     
ZTFLH:  B842  
Issue Date: 29 May 2018
Service
E-mail this article
E-mail Alert
RSS
Articles by authors
Bihua YAN
Xiaomin LIU
Haozhe LIU
Cite this article:   
Bihua YAN,Xiaomin LIU,Haozhe LIU. Landmark attraction effect and landmark repulsion effect on representational momentum in airplane movement scene[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(7): 703-714.
URL:  
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.00703     OR     http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/Y2018/V50/I7/703
  
  
  
  
  
  
  
  
[1] Blätter C., Ferrari V., Didierjean A., & Marmèche E . ( 2011). Representational momentum in aviation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37( 5), 1569-1577.
url: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/a0023512
[2] Blätter C., Ferrari V., Didierjean A., & Marmèche E . ( 2012). Role of expertise and action in motion extrapolation from real road scenes. Visual Cognition, 20( 8), 988-1001.
url: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/13506285.2012.716799
[3] Courtney, J. R., &Hubbard T.L, . ( 2008). Spatial memory and explicit knowledge: An effect of instruction on representational momentum. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 1778-1784.
pmid: 18609396 url: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1080/17470210802194217
[4] Didierjean A., Ferrari V. & Blättler , C. , ( 2014) . Role of knowledge in motion extrapolation: The relevance of an approach contrasting experts and novices. In B. H. Ross (Ed), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 215-235, Vol. 61). Burlington: Academic Press.
[5] Dong R. ( 2015). Facing orientation effect on representational momentum. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 47( 2), 190-202.
url: http://www.cqvip.com/QK/90117X/201502/663973258.html
[5] [ 董蕊 . ( 2015). 表征动量的朝向效应. 心理学报, 47( 2), 190-202.]
url: http://www.cqvip.com/QK/90117X/201502/663973258.html
[6] Finke, R. A., &Freyd J.J, . ( 1985). Transformations of visual memory induced by implied motions of pattern elements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 780-794.
pmid: 2932525 url: http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/xlm/11/4/780/
[7] Freyd, J.J . ( 1983). The mental representation of movement when static stimuli are viewed. Perception & Psychophysics, 33, 575-581.
pmid: 6622194 url: http://psycnet.apa.org/record/1984-05721-001
[8] Freyd,J.J., &Finke R.A, . ( 1984). Representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 126-132.
url: http://content.apa.org/journals/xlm/10/1/126
[9] Freyd, J.J., &Jones K.T, . ( 1994). Representational momentum for a spiral path. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 968-976.
pmid: 8064255 url: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/0278-7393.20.4.968
[10] Freyd, J.J., &Miller G.F, . ( 1992, November). Creature motion. Paper presented at the 33rd Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, St. Louis.
[11] Freyd J. J. & Pantzer , T. M. , ( 1995) . Static patterns moving in the mind. In S. M. Smith, T. B. Ward, & R. A. Finke (Eds.), The creative cognition approach (pp. 181-204). Cambridge, MA:MIT Press.
url: http://psycnet.apa.org/psycinfo/1995-97533-008
[12] Halpern,A.R., &Kelly M.H, . ( 1993). Memory biases in left versus right implied motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 471-484.
url: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/0278-7393.19.2.471
[13] Hayes, A.E., &Freyd J.J, . ( 2002). Representational momentum when attention is divided. Visual Cognition, 9, 8-27.
url: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/13506280143000296
[14] Hubbard,T.L . ( 1990). Cognitive representation of linear motion: Possible direction and gravity effects in judged displacement. Memory and Cognition, 18, 299-309.
pmid: 2355859 url: http://www.springerlink.com/index/10.3758/BF03213883
[15] Hubbard, T.L . ( 1993). The effect of context on visual representational momentum. Memory and Cognition, 21, 103-114.
pmid: 8433640 url: http://www.springerlink.com/index/10.3758/BF03211169
[16] Hubbard,T. L . ( 1996). Representational momentum, centripetal force, and curvilinear impetus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1049-1060.
pmid: 8708601 url: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/0278-7393.22.4.1049
[17] Hubbard, T. L . ( 2005). Representational momentum and related displacements in spatial memory: A review of the findings. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12( 5), 822-851.
pmid: 16524000 url: http://www.springerlink.com/index/10.3758/BF03196775
[18] Hubbard, T.L . ( 2008). Representational momentum contributes to motion induced mislocalization of stationary objects. Visual Cognition, 16( 1), 44-67.
url: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/13506280601155468
[19] Hubbard T. L. ( 2010). Approaches to representational momentum: Theories and models. In R. Nijhawan & B. Khurana (Eds.), Space and time in perception and action (pp.338-365). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
url: http://psycnet.apa.org/record/2010-09489-021
[20] Hubbard,T.L . ( 2013 a). Phenomenal causality Ⅰ: Varieties and variables. Axiomathes, 23( 1), 1-42.
url: http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s10516-012-9198-8
[21] Hubbard, T.L . ( 2013 b). Launching, entraining, and representational momentum: Evidence consistent with an impetus heuristic in perception of causality. Axiomathes, 23( 4), 633-643.
url: http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s10516-012-9186-z
[22] Hubbard,T.L . ( 2014). Forms of momentum across space: Representational, operational, and attentional. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 21( 6), 1371-1403.
pmid: 24917187 url: http://link.springer.com/10.3758/s13423-014-0624-3
[23] Hubbard, T.L . ( 2017). Toward a general theory of momentum- like effects. Behavioural Processes, 141, 50-56.
pmid: 28257789 url: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0376635717300827
[24] Hubbard,T.L., &Bharucha J.J, . ( 1988). Judged displacement in apparent vertical and horizontal motion. Perception and Psychophysics, 44( 3), 211-221.
url: http://www.springerlink.com/index/10.3758/BF03206290
[25] Hubbard T. L., Blessum J. A., & Ruppel S. E . ( 2001). Representational momentum and Michotte’s “Launching Effect” paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 294-301.
[26] Hubbard, T.L., &Courtney J.R, . ( 2010). Cross-modal influences on representational momentum and representational gravity. Perception, 39, 851-862.
pmid: 20698479 url: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1068/p6538
[27] Hubbard T. L., Kumar A. M., & Carp C. L . ( 2009). Effects of spatial cueing on representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 666-677.
pmid: 19379042 url: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/a0014870
[28] Hubbard, T.L., &Ruppel S.E, . ( 1999). Representational momentum and the landmark attraction effect. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53( 3), 242-256.
url: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/h0087313
[29] Hubbard, T.L., &Ruppel S.E, . ( 2000). Spatial memory averaging, the landmark attraction effect, and representational gravity. Psychological Research, 64( 1), 41-55.
pmid: 11109866 url: http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s004260000029
[30] Hubbard, T.L., &Ruppel S.E, . ( 2017). Perceived causality, force, and resistance in the absence of launching. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 24, 591-596.
pmid: 27473683 url: http://link.springer.com/10.3758/s13423-016-1121-7
[31] Johnston, H.M., &Jones M.R, . ( 2006). Higher order pattern structure influences auditory representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 2-17.
pmid: 16478322 url: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/0096-1523.32.1.2
[32] Joordens S., Spalek T. M., Razmy S., & van Duijn M . ( 2004). A clockwork orange: Compensation opposing momentum in memory for location. Memory and Cognition, 32( 1), 39-50.
pmid: 15078043 url: http://link.springer.com/10.3758/BF03195819
[33] Kerzel, D. ( 2002). Attention shifts and memory averaging. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 425-443.
pmid: 12047052 url: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1080/02724980143000424
[34] Kerzel, D. ( 2003). Attention maintains mental extrapolation of target position: Irrelevant distractors eliminate forward displacement after implied motion. Cognition, 88, 109-131.
pmid: 12711155 url: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0010027703000180
[35] Liu, H.Q . ( 2014). The influence of target motion on the momentum of representation (Unpublished doctorial dissertation). Capital Normal University.
[35] [ 刘浩强 . ( 2014). 目标运动归因对表征动量的影响 (博士学位论文). 首都师范大学.]
[36] Munger, M., &Owens T.R, . ( 2004). Representational momentum and the flash-lag effect. Visual Cognition, 11( 1), 81-103.
url: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/13506280344000257
[37] Nagai,M., &Yagi A. , ( 2001). The pointedness effect on representational momentum. Memory and Cognition, 29( 1), 91-99.
pmid: 11277468 url: http://www.springerlink.com/index/10.3758/BF03195744
[38] Rao H. Y., Han S. H., Jiang Y., Xue Y. P., Gu H., Cui Y., & Gao D. G . ( 2004). Engagement of the prefrontal cortex in representational momentum: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 23( 1), 98-103.
pmid: 15325356 url: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1053811904002952
[39] Reed,C.L., &Vinson N.G . ( 1996). Conceptual effects on representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 839-850.
pmid: 8756955 url: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/0096-1523.22.4.839
[40] Thornton,L., &Hayes A. , ( 2004). Anticipating action in complex scenes. Visual Cognition, 11, 341-370.
url: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/13506280344000374
[41] Whitney, D., &Cavanagh P. , ( 2002). Surrounding motion affects the perceived locations of moving stimuli. Visual Cognition, 9, 139-152.
url: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/13506280143000368
[42] Zhai, K. , &Zhang , Z.J . ( 2012). The effect of spatial-temporal cue on representational momentum. Journal of Psychological Science, 35( 6), 1309-1314.
url: http://cdmd.cnki.com.cn/Article/CDMD-10094-1011147674.htm
[42] [ 翟坤, 张志杰 . ( 2012). 线索呈现的时空特性对表征动量的影响. 心理科学, 35( 6), 1309-1314.]
url: http://cdmd.cnki.com.cn/Article/CDMD-10094-1011147674.htm
[43] Zhai, K. , &Zhang ,Z.J . ( 2013). The effects of cues on representational momentum. Journal of Psychological Science, 36( 1), 51-56.
[43] [ 翟坤, 张志杰 . ( 2013). 线索提示对表征动量的影响. 心理科学, 36( 1), 51-56.]
[1] BAI Lu, MAO Weibin, WANG Rui, Zhang Wenhai.  The effect of emotional scene and body expression on facial expression recognition[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(9): 1172-1183.
[2] SUN Yusheng; ZHANG Zhijun; WU Binxing. The impact of contextual expectation on rapid natural scene recognition[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(5): 577-589.
[3] DONG Rui. Facing Orientation Effect on Representational Momentum[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(2): 190-202.
[4] YANG Yaping; XU Qiang; ZHANG Lin; DENG Peizhuang; LIANG Ningjian. Scenes Differing in Spatial Frequencies Affect Facial Expression Processing: Evidence from ERP[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(12): 1433-1444.
[5] WANG Juan;ZHANG Jijia. Impact of Primed Language on Chinese-English Bilinguals’ Scene Consistency Judgment[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2014, 46(3): 331-340.
[6] ZHOU Rong-Gang,ZHANG Kan. Object Location Judgments in Spatial Memory Based on Coordinating Egocentric and Allocentric References[J]. , 2008, 40(12): 1229-1239.
[7] Zhao Mintao,Mou Weimin. ORIENTATION SPECIFICITY OF SPATIAL REPRESENTATION IN MULTI-VIEW LEARNING[J]. , 2005, 37(03): 308-313.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
Copyright © Acta Psychologica Sinica
Support by Beijing Magtech