Please wait a minute...
Acta Psychologica Sinica    2018, Vol. 50 Issue (7) : 693-702     DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.00693
Reports of Empirical Studies |
Influence of aesthetics on unconscious processing of western paintings
Junchen SHANG1,Zhihui LIU1,Wenfeng CHEN2,3(),Xiaolan FU3,4
1 School of Psychology, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian 116029, China
2 Department of Psychology, Renmin University of China, Beijing 100872, China
3 State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China
4 Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049, China
Download: PDF(4280 KB)   HTML Review File (1 KB) 
Export: BibTeX | EndNote | Reference Manager | ProCite | RefWorks     Supporting Info
Guide   
Abstract  

People prefer beautiful visual artworks. Aesthetic experiences to beautiful and ugly images are different. Studies on neuroaesthetics showed different neural responses to beautiful stimuli compared to ugly stimuli. The first stage of aesthetic experience of a visual artwork is visual perception of the stimulus. Most research about aesthetic processing focused on stimuli presented consciously. Little is known about whether aesthetic processing can occur unconsciously. Previous research suggested that both beautiful paintings and attractive faces can elicit activation of the reward circuitry in our brain. Attractive faces break through continuous flash suppression more quickly than unattractive faces. Thus, it is possible that unconscious processing of beautiful paintings is different from that of less beautiful paintings. In two experiments, the present study adopted continuous flash suppression paradigm to investigate whether aesthetic ratings of western paintings influenced the time for stimuli to break suppression. We also compared the suppression effect of achromatic (Experiment 1) and chromatic (Experiment 2) noise pictures.

In Experiment 1, 20 participants (8 females, 12 males) took part in the experiment. The independent variable was aesthetic rating of western paintings (high, average, and low). The achromatic suppression noises were presented to the participants’ dominant eye and continued to flash at 10 Hz. A western painting was presented to the nondominant eye, at either above or below the central cross, with contrast increasing from 0 to 100% within 1s and remaining constant until response. Participants were instructed to respond as accurately and quickly as possible when any part of the painting was detected, and report whether the target was presented above or below the cross. 18 participants (9 females, 9 males) took part in Experiment 2. Experiment 2 was identical with Experiment 1 except that chromatic suppression noises were presented to the dominant eye.

The results of Experiment 1 showed that paintings with low aesthetic ratings took significantly longer time to break into awareness, comparing to paintings with average and high aesthetic ratings. The reaction times were not significantly different between paintings with average and high aesthetic ratings. In Experiment 2, however, the times for breaking suppression were not significantly different among the three categories of paintings. Moreover, the effect of suppression was different between different noises, such that reaction times were longer under suppression by chromatic noises (Experiment 2) than achromatic noises (Experiment 1).

These results suggested different unconscious processing of western paintings with different aesthetic ratings. Similar to attractive faces, paintings with high and average aesthetic ratings were easier to be detected under suppression. Our findings provided evidence that aesthetic processing can occur unconsciously under suppression by achromatic noises. The present study also suggested that the suppression effect of achromatic noises is different from that of chromatic noises. Chromatic noises may interfere with the color information of paintings and disrupt the aesthetic perception of paintings.

Keywords aesthetic judgments      binocular rivalry      continuous flash suppression      western paintings      unconscious processing     
ZTFLH:  B842  
Issue Date: 29 May 2018
Service
E-mail this article
E-mail Alert
RSS
Articles by authors
Junchen SHANG
Zhihui LIU
Wenfeng CHEN
Xiaolan FU
Cite this article:   
Junchen SHANG,Zhihui LIU,Wenfeng CHEN, et al. Influence of aesthetics on unconscious processing of western paintings[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(7): 693-702.
URL:  
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.00693     OR     http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/Y2018/V50/I7/693
  
  
美感等级 唤醒度 愉悦度 美感
M SD M SD M SD
美感低 5.00 0.31 4.96 0.14 4.00 0.47
美感中等 4.77 0.25 4.96 0.13 4.79 0.06
美感高 5.03 0.26 5.07 0.11 5.46 0.44
  
  
  
  
  
  
[1] Aharon I., Etcoff N., Ariely D., Chabris C. F., O'Connor E., & Breiter H. C . ( 2001). Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron, 32( 3), 537-551.
url: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0896627301004913
[2] Anderson E., Siegel E. H., Bliss-Moreau E., & Barrett L. F . ( 2011). The visual impact of gossip. Science, 332, 1446-1448.
pmid: 3141574 url: http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/doi/10.1126/science.1201574
[3] Anderson E., Siegel E., White D., & Barrett L. F . ( 2012). Out of sight but not out of mind: Unseen affective faces influence evaluations and social impressions. Emotion, 12( 6), 1210-1221.
pmid: 4957816 url: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/a0027514
[4] Bradley , M.M., & Lang, P. J . ( 1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry, 25( 1), 49-59.
[5] Bray , S., &O'Doherty J. , ( 2007). Neural coding of reward- prediction error signals during classical conditioning with attractive faces. Journal of Neurophysiology, 97( 4), 3036-3045.
pmid: 17303809 url: http://www.physiology.org/doi/10.1152/jn.01211.2006
[6] Chai F. Y., Yu F., & Peng K. P . ( 2016). Insights into aesthetic pleasure and processing fluency. Psychological Exploration, 36( 2), 101-106.
[6] [ 柴方圆, 喻丰, 彭凯平 . ( 2016). 审美愉悦与加工流畅性. 心理学探新, 36( 2), 101-106.]
[7] Chatterjee, A., &Vartanian O. , ( 2016). Neuroscience of aesthetics. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1369, 172-194.
url: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/nyas.13035
[8] Cela-Conde C. J., Marty G., Maestú F., Ortiz T., Munar E., Fernández A., … Quesney F . ( 2004). Activation of the prefrontal cortex in the human visual aesthetic perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101( 16), 6321-6325.
url: http://www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.0401427101
[9] Costello P., Jiang Y., Baartman B., McGlennen K., & He S . ( 2009). Semantic and subword priming during binocular suppression. Consciousness and Cognition, 18( 2), 375-382.
pmid: 4521603 url: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1053810009000087
[10] Cupchik G. C., Vartanian O., Crawley A., & Mikulis D. J . ( 2009). Viewing artworks: Contributions of cognitive control and perceptual facilitation to aesthetic experience. Brain & Cognition, 70( 1), 84-91.
pmid: 19223099 url: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19223099
[11] Ding, X.J., & Zhou, C. L . ( 2006). A neurological study on aesthetic process and its significance to aesthetics. Psychological Science, 29( 5), 1247-1249.
url: http://d.wanfangdata.com.cn/Periodical/xlkx200605056
[11] [ 丁晓君, 周昌乐 . ( 2006). 审美的神经机制研究及其美学意义. 心理科学, 29( 5), 1247-1249.]
url: http://d.wanfangdata.com.cn/Periodical/xlkx200605056
[12] Fang ,F., &He S. , ( 2005). Cortical responses to invisible objects in the human dorsal and ventral pathways. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1380-1385.
pmid: 16136038 url: http://www.nature.com/articles/nn1537
[13] Feng , C.Z., & Jia ,F.Q . ( 2008). Review on binocular rivalry. Advances in Psychological Science, 16( 2), 213-221.
url: http://www.cqvip.com/Main/Detail.aspx?id=26612373
[13] [ 冯成志, 贾凤芹 . ( 2008). 双眼竞争研究现状与展望. 心理科学进展, 16( 2), 213-221.]
url: http://www.cqvip.com/Main/Detail.aspx?id=26612373
[14] Hong , S.W., &Blake R. , ( 2009). Interocular suppression differentially affects achromatic and chromatic mechanisms. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71( 2), 403-411.
pmid: 2734383 url: http://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/APP.71.2.403
[15] Huang,Z. L., &Zhang W.D, . ( 2012). Neuroaesthetics: Exploring aesthetics and the brain. Advances in Psychological Science, 20( 5), 672-681.
url: http://d.wanfangdata.com.cn/Periodical/xlxdt201205005
[15] [ 黄子岚, 张卫东 . ( 2012). 神经美学: 探索审美与大脑的关系. 心理科学进展, 20( 5), 672-681.]
url: http://d.wanfangdata.com.cn/Periodical/xlxdt201205005
[16] Hahn, A. C., &Perrett D.I, . ( 2014). Neural and behavioral responses to attractiveness in adult and infant faces. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 46, 591-603.
pmid: 25199981 url: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0149763414002139
[17] Hung S. M., Nieh C. H., & Hsieh P. J . ( 2016). Unconscious processing of facial attractiveness: Invisible attractive faces orient visual attention. Scientific Reports, 6, 37117.
pmid: 27848992 url: http://www.nature.com/articles/srep37117
[18] Jiang Y., Costello P., & He S . ( 2007). Processing of invisible stimuli: Advantage of upright faces and recognizable words in overcoming interocular suppression. Psychological Science, 18( 4), 349-355.
pmid: 17470261 url: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01902.x
[19] Jiang,Y., &He S. , ( 2006). Cortical responses to invisible faces: Dissociating subsystems for facial-information processing. Current Biology, 16( 20), 2023-2029.
pmid: 17055981 url: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S096098220602152X
[20] Kawabata, H., &Zeki S. , ( 2004). Neural correlates of beauty. Journal of Neurophysiology, 91( 4), 1699-1705.
url: http://www.physiology.org/doi/10.1152/jn.00696.2003
[21] Kovács I., Papathomas T. V., Yang M., & Fehér A . ( 1996). When the brain changes its mind: Interocular grouping during binocular rivalry. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 93( 26), 15508-15511.
pmid: 8986842 url: http://www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.93.26.15508
[22] Li M. L., Chen J., & Wu Y . ( 2015). The research of aesthetic cognitive difference in Chinese and Western paintings between art and ordinary undergraduates. Journal of Psychological Science, 38( 2), 366-372.
url: http://www.cqvip.com/QK/95682A/201502/664049106.html
[22] [ 李苗利, 陈晶, 吴杨 . ( 2015). 美术专业与普通专业学生对中西方绘画作品的审美认知差异. 心理科学, 38( 2), 366-372.]
url: http://www.cqvip.com/QK/95682A/201502/664049106.html
[23] Morris,J. D . ( 1995). Observations: SAM: The self-assessment manikin. An efficient cross-cultural measurement of emotional response. Journal of Advertising Research, 35, 63-68.
[24] Massar , K., &Buunk, A.P . ( 2010). Judging a book by its cover: Jealousy after subliminal priming with attractive and unattractive faces. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 634-638.
url: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0191886910002849
[25] O'Doherty J., Winston J., Critchley H., Perrett D., Burt D. M., & Dolan R. J . ( 2003). Beauty in a smile: The role of medial orbitofrontal cortex in facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia, 41( 2), 147-155.
pmid: 12459213 url: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0028393202001458
[26] Olson, I. R., &Marshuetz C. , ( 2005). Facial attractiveness is appraised in a glance. Emotion, 5( 4), 498-502.
pmid: 16366753 url: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/1528-3542.5.4.498
[27] Pearce M. T., Zaidel D. W., Vartanian O., Skov M., Leder H., Chatterjee A., & Nadal M . ( 2016). Neuroaesthetics: The cognitive neuroscience of aesthetic experience. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11( 2), 265-279.
url: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/1745691615621274
[28] Somoon, K., & Moorapun, C. , ( 2016). The roles of aesthetic and cultural perception affected by window display of Thai crafts products to increase purchasing intention. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 234, 55-63.
url: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1877042816314720
[29] Stein T., Hebart M. N., & Sterzer P . ( 2011). Breaking continuous flash suppression: A new measure of unconscious processing during interocular suppression? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 167.
pmid: 3243089 url: http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC3243089/
[30] Tan, J.S ., &Yeh S.L, . ( 2015). Audiovisual integration facilitates unconscious visual scene processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41( 5), 1325-1335.
pmid: 26076179 url: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/xhp0000074
[31] Tsuchiya, N., &Koch C ., ( 2005). Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1096-1101.
url: http://www.nature.com/articles/nn1500
[32] Tsukiura, T., &Cabeza R ., ( 2011). Remembering beauty: Roles of orbitofrontal and hippocampal regions in successful memory encoding of attractive faces. NeuroImage, 54( 1), 653-660.
pmid: 20659568 url: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1053811910010190
[33] Vartanian, O., &Goel V , . ( 2004). Emotion pathways in the brain mediate aesthetic preference. Bulletin of Psychology and the Arts, 5( 1), 37-42.
[34] Wang N. Y., Luo Y. J., & Dong Q . ( 2010). Neural correlates of aesthetics. Advances in Psychological Science, 18( 1), 19-27.
[34] [ 王乃弋, 罗跃嘉, 董奇 . ( 2010). 审美的神经机制. 心理科学进展, 18( 1), 19-27.]
[35] Winston J. S., O’Doherty J., Kilner J. M., Perrett D. I., & Dolan R. J . ( 2007). Brain systems for assessing facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia, 45, 195-206.
url: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0028393206001758
[36] Xu, Q., &Jiang Y ., ( 2012). Unconscious processing of emotional faces and its probable neural Mechanism. Science Bulletin, 57( 35), 3358-3366.
[36] [ 徐茜, 蒋毅 . ( 2012). 无意识的情绪面孔加工及其潜在神经机制. 科学通报, 57( 35), 3358-3366.]
[37] Yang, E., & BlakeR ., ( 2012). Deconstructing continuous flash suppression . Journal of Vision, 12( 3), 8.
[38] Yang E., Brascamp J., Kang M. S., & Blake R . ( 2014). On the use of continuous flash suppression for the study of visual processing outside of awareness. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 724.
pmid: 4093749 url: http://pubmedcentralcanada.ca/pmcc/articles/PMC4093749/
[39] Yang ,Y.H., &Yeh S.L, . ( 2011). Accessing the meaning of invisible words. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 223-233.
pmid: 20709576 url: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1053810010001479
[40] Yang E., Zald D. H., & Blake R . ( 2007). Fearful expressions gain preferential access to awareness during continuous flash suppression. Emotion, 7( 4), 882-886.
pmid: 4038625 url: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.882
[41] Yue X., Vessel E. A., & Biederman I . ( 2007). The neural basis of scene preferences. NeuroReport, 18 6), 525-529.
url: https://insights.ovid.com/crossref?an=00001756-200704160-00001
[42] Zhang X. J., Liu Y. J., & Liu C . ( 2015). The neural substrate underlying the aesthetic processing of facial beauty and individual differences. Journal of Psychological Science, 38( 3), 574-579.
[42] [ 张小将, 刘迎杰, 刘昌 . ( 2015). 面孔审美加工的神经机制及个体差异. 心理科学, 38( 3), 574-579.]
[43] Zhang X. L., Dong B., Jiang Y. P., & Zhang M . ( 2012). The gestalt in unconscious processing: Evidence for the unconscious binding hypothesis. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 44( 12), 1563-1570.
[43] [ 张秀玲, 董波, 姜云鹏, 张明 . ( 2012). 无意识信息加工中的“完型”——无意识捆绑假说的新证据. 心理学报, 44( 12), 1563-1570.]
[44] Zhu B. X. ( 2001). The world dictionary of classical art appreciation. Beijing , China:China Youth Press.
[44] [ 朱伯雄 . ( 2001). 世界经典美术鉴赏辞典. 北京: 中国青年出版社.]
[1] LUO Ting, QIU Ruyi, CHEN Bin, FU Shimin.  The stimulus representation of unconscious information and its temporal characteristics[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(5): 473-482.
[2] ZHANG Xiuling, PANG Zhaoyang, JIANG Yunpeng, ZHANG Ming, JIANG Yi.  Access to awareness is improved by affective learning[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(3): 253-259.
[3] CHEN Airui; DONG Bo; FANG Ying; YU Changyu; ZHANG Ming. The Role of Cue Type in the Subliminal Gaze-cueing Effect[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2014, 46(9): 1281-1288.
[4] ZHANG Xiu-Ling;DONG Bo;JIANG Yun-Peng;ZHANG Ming. The Gestalt in Unconscious Processing: Evidence for the Unconscious Binding Hypothesis[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2012, 44(12): 1563-1570.
[5] WANG Rui-Ming,WU Di,ZOU Yan-Rong,ZHOU Ai-Si,ZENG Xiang-Yan. The Influence of Coherence on the Consciousness of Text Representation[J]. , 2011, 43(10): 1114-1123.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
Copyright © Acta Psychologica Sinica
Support by Beijing Magtech