Please wait a minute...
Acta Psychologica Sinica    2018, Vol. 50 Issue (4) : 363-376     DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.00363
|
 Beauty is in the eye of the beholder: The halo effect and generalization effect in the facial attractiveness evaluation
 HAN Shangfeng1; LI Yue1,2; LIU Shen1,3; XU Qiang1; TAN Qun1; ZHANG Lin1
 (1 Department and Institute of Psychology, Ningbo University, Ningbo 315211, China) (2 Yunnan Business Vocational College, Kunming 650000, China) (3 School of Humanities and Social Science, University of Science and Technology of China, Hefei 230022, China)
Download: PDF(719 KB)   Review File (1 KB) 
Export: BibTeX | EndNote | Reference Manager | ProCite | RefWorks     Supporting Info
Guide   
Abstract   Even though people usually agreed that “a book should not be judged by its cover”, researches had repeatedly demonstrated that individuals spontaneously and very swiftly formed impression on others based merely on the appearance of their faces. Facial attractiveness is an important content in the first perception. Which had been linked to outcomes as diverse as mate choice, job hunting, and cooperation. Given these real world consequences of the first impressions, it is important to understand how these impressions are formed. Some studies found that facial physical characteristics, such as symmetry, averageness and sexual dimorphism, had a great impact on facial attractiveness. While different individuals have different experience, when faced with the same face in the same context, different individuals have different evaluations on facial attractiveness. Some researchers put forward a new theory, namely, the observer hypothesis, which demonstrated that beauty is in the eye of the beholder. However, the processing of unfamiliar facial attractiveness remained unclear. The goal of the current study was to explore how we processed the impression of unfamiliar facial attractiveness. 19 males and 27 females took part in the experiment one and 16 males and 22 females participated in the experiment two, each experiment contained two phases that were learning tasks and evaluation tasks. In the learning phase, participants firstly learned to associate faces with negative, neutral, or positive trait words or imaged the behavior of the individuals to form different impression, which was contribute to the same valence between the neutral face and trait words or sentence. When participants could evaluate the valence of the face correctly, they could proceed to the next phase. In the experiment one, 13 males and 25 females had passed learning phase and evaluated the original faces and the unfamiliar faces. In the experiment two, 12 males and 17 females had done the learning task and evaluation task successfully. And in the evaluation phase, extend of warmth, competence and facial attractiveness of the unfamiliar faces, which had 50% similarity with the learned faces, were evaluated. Both of the two experiments had the same results, which showed that there are two ways to form unfamiliar facial attractiveness: (1) the first one is that generalization effect occurred after halo effect, compared with negative familiar faces, positive familiar faces were evaluated more attractive, so did the unfamiliar faces that were familiar with positive familiar faces; (2) the second one is that halo effect occurred after generalization effect, unfamiliar faces which were similar with positive familiar faces were not only evaluated more positive but also more attractive. The results suggested that generalization effect occurred after halo effect and halo effect occurred after generalization effect were the two ways to form unfamiliar facial attractiveness. In conclusion, halo effect and generalization effect play an important role in the processing of unfamiliar facial attractiveness.
Keywords similarity      halo effect      generalization effect      unfamiliar facial attractiveness      impression formation     
ZTFLH:     
  B849:C91  
Fund: 
Corresponding Authors: ZHANG Lin, E-mail: zhanglin1@nbu.edu.cn; 刘燊, E-mail: liushenpsy@ustc.edu.cn     E-mail: E-mail: zhanglin1@nbu.edu.cn; E-mail: liushenpsy@ustc.edu.cn
Issue Date: 28 February 2018
Service
E-mail this article
E-mail Alert
RSS
Articles by authors
HAN Shangfeng
LI Yue
LIU Shen
XU Qiang
TAN Qun
ZHANG Lin
Cite this article:   
HAN Shangfeng,LI Yue,LIU Shen, et al.  Beauty is in the eye of the beholder: The halo effect and generalization effect in the facial attractiveness evaluation[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(4): 363-376.
URL:  
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.00363     OR     http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/Y2018/V50/I4/363
[1] JIN Xiangdong, ZHANG Guangling, CAO Jing, GU Chuanhua, WEI Hua, XIE Zhipeng, DUAN Zhaohui.  Spillover effects of strong brands competition[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(6): 678-692.
[2] YANG Wenjing, JIN Yule, QIU Jiang, ZHANG Qinglin.  The effect of prototype difficulty and semantic similarity on the prototype activation[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(3): 260-269.
[3] GONG Xiushuang, JIANG Jing.  The effect of incidental similarity (“dress same”) on consumers’ product disposition intentions and its underlying mechanism[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(3): 337-348.
[4] ZHANG Jianxin; TANG Dan; LI Yingli; LIU Dianzhi. Anti-opposition logic paradigm: An improvement on the opposition logic in implicit artificial grammar learning[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2016, 48(9): 1130-1142.
[5] HU Xueping;SUN Jimin;CAO Rui;YAO Wenqing;WANG Meizhu. The Impact of the Perceptual Similarity of Concrete Objects’ Shape in Visual Metaphor Processing[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2014, 46(5): 607-620.
[6] CHEN Shujuan; WANG Pei; LIANG Yajun. Perceiving Groups and Persons: Inverse Base-Rate Effect[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2014, 46(11): 1772-1781.
[7] LIN Xiaomin; BAI Xinwen; LIN Lin. Effects of Similarity and Accuracy Indices of Shared Mental Models on Team Creativity[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2014, 46(11): 1734-1747.
[8] FANG Yanhong;ZHANG Jijia. Roles of Semantic Similarity and Category Size on Semantic Effect in Picture-Word Interference Paradigm[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2013, 45(5): 523-537.
[9] CHI Liping. Trust: Parents’ Similarity and Parent-Child Transmissibility[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2013, 45(3): 336-344.
[10] LI Xuan;LIU Si-Yun. The Effects of Phonological Similarity and Visual Similarity in Immediate Serial Recall of Chinese Characters[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2012, 44(12): 1571-1582.
[11] WANG Juan,SHEN Shu-Hua,ZHANG Ji-Jia. Undergraduates’ Classification of Odor Terms: A Study Based on Semantic and Imagined Similarity[J]. , 2011, 43(10): 1124-1137.
[12] WU Lei,MO Lei. Re-examining the Classifying Advantage and Basic Level Effect[J]. , 2011, 43(02): 143-151.
[13] XU Jing-Zhe,XIE Xiao-Fei. Self-Other Decision Making Difference: A Construal Level Perspective[J]. , 2011, 43(01): 11-20.
[14] MA Xiao-Qing,FENG Ting-Yong,LI Hong,LONG Chang-Quan. The Roles of Thematic Relations in 4-5 Years Children’s Inductive Reasoning of Different Properties[J]. , 2009, 41(03): 249-258.
[15] Wang Moyun. The Interaction Effect of Connection Strength and Relevance Similarity in Feature Induction[J]. , 2008, 40(03): 291-300.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
Copyright © Acta Psychologica Sinica
Support by Beijing Magtech