Please wait a minute...
Acta Psychologica Sinica
|
Chinese character practice: Comparison between children using handwriting and Pinyin keyboarding
CHEN Jingjun1; XU Lei2,3; CHENG Xiaorong2; LIU Huashan2
(1 Department of Psychology, Hunan University of Science and Technology, Xiangtan 411201, China) (2 School of Psychology, Central China Normal University, Wuhan 430079, China) (3 School of Education, Jianghan University, Wuhan 430056, China)
Download: PDF(512 KB)   Review File (1 KB) 
Export: BibTeX | EndNote | Reference Manager | ProCite | RefWorks    
Abstract  

A long-standing point is that the operational motion of handwriting has a special role in word encoding and retrieving, indicating that word representation may consist of a handwriting movement component. If so, highly frequent use of keyboarding may change the mental representation of words and further affect word learning. Thus, it is highly possible that performance in learning new Chinese characters with Pinyin keyboarding (which is widely used among Chinese teenagers) is worse than with handwriting. Further, the movement memory codes of learned characters may be weakened because of long-time Pinyin keyboarding and lack of handwriting track feedback. In addition, the glyphs of Chinese characters will be more likely to be forgotten when reviewed with Pinyin input than with handwriting. To test the above propositions, we compared Chinese character learning and retention between two groups of 6 graders, with one group using paper-pen writing and the other using Pinyin keyboarding. First, we used the traditional method to teach 2 classes of students 30 low-frequency characters in classrooms and participants were labeled as “mastered” or “un-mastered” according to their dictation performances. Then, these 2 classes were assigned randomly to practice with handwriting or Pinyin input. Thereafter, participants were instructed to practice with handwriting or Pinyin input once a week for six weeks and then tested in recognition and writing twice, one week and three months after the practice. We found that, in the first test, for the un-mastered group, the positive effects from the two practice methods were similar in recognition, but the writing performance after the handwriting practice was significantly better than that after the Pinyin keyboarding practice. For the mastered group, performances in recognition and writing after the two kinds of practices were similar and dictation and writing performances before and after the practice were also similar. In the second test, children’s performances were similar after handwriting and Pinyin keyboarding practices in both recognition and writing. The results suggested that Chinese characters learned with Pinyin keyboarding practice were not more likely to be forgotten than characters learned with handwriting practice. In conclusion, although handwriting and Pinyin keyboarding practices have different positive effects on learning Chinese characters, i.e., similar positive effects in recognition but a greater effect for handwriting than Pinyin keyboarding in writing, they have the same positive effects on reviewing characters. This suggests that compared with English words, Chinese characters learning is more dependent on the feedback of handwriting movement, which may be related to spacial features of Chinese characters. These results also suggest that we may use Pinyin input to teach children to read Chinese characters and use traditional handwriting to teach them to write. In addition, memory retention of the mastered characters after the two practices are similar after an interval without practice, suggesting that character amnesia may not be caused by the Pinyin keyboarding specifically, but by a weakened orthographic code due to lack of practice of handwriting or Pinyin input on a long-term basis.

Keywords handwriting      Pinyin keyboarding      Chinese characters      recognition      writing      character amnesia     
Corresponding Authors: LIU Huashan, E-mail: hsliupsycho@263.net    
Issue Date: 25 October 2016
Service
E-mail this article
E-mail Alert
RSS
Articles by authors
CHEN Jingjun
XU Lei
CHENG Xiaorong
LIU Huashan
Cite this article:   
CHEN Jingjun,XU Lei,CHENG Xiaorong, et al. Chinese character practice: Comparison between children using handwriting and Pinyin keyboarding[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2016.01258
URL:  
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2016.01258     OR     http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/Y2016/V48/I10/1258
[1] SUN Yusheng; ZHANG Zhijun; WU Binxing. The impact of contextual expectation on rapid natural scene recognition[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(5): 577-589.
[2] SUN Juncai; SHI Rong. Attentional bias to crying facial expressions: Evidence from eye movements[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(2): 155-163.
[3] LI Guangzheng; WANG Lijuan. The automatic pop-out mechanism of subject performed tasks effect: Evidence from output monitoring[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2016, 48(3): 238-247.
[4] WANG Xieshun; WU Yan; ZHAO Simin; NI Chao; ZHANG Ming. The effects of semantic radicals and phonetic radicals in Chinese phonogram recognition[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2016, 48(2): 130-140.
[5] MAO Xinrui; XU Huifang; GUO Chunyan. Emotional Memory Enhancement Effect in Dual-processing Recognition Retrieval[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(9): 1111-1123.
[6] NIE Aiqing; JIANG Jingguo; FU Qiao; ZHANG Ruiqing. The Conjunction Effect and Feature Effect in Faces Are Modulated by Task Type[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(5): 570-583.
[7] WU Yan; WANG Xieshun; CHEN Hsuan-chih. ERP effects of Radical Combinability in Chinese Character Recognition[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(2): 157-166.
[8] XIAO Hongrui; HUANG Yifan2; GONG Xianmin; WANG Dahua. Age Alters the Effects of Emotional Valence on False Memory: Using the Simplified Conjoint Recognition Paradigm[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(1): 19-28.
[9] HU Xueping;SUN Jimin;CAO Rui;YAO Wenqing;WANG Meizhu. The Impact of the Perceptual Similarity of Concrete Objects’ Shape in Visual Metaphor Processing[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2014, 46(5): 607-620.
[10] MENG Yingfang. Neural Correlates at Encoding of Covert Face Recognition[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2013, 45(9): 935-943.
[11] SUN Tianyi;XU yuanli;GUO Chunyan. The Brain Potential Features of Human Face Recognition during Working Memory[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2013, 45(10): 1072-1084.
[12] CHEN Sui-Qing,ZHANG Ji-Jia,WU Xue-Yun,GAO Ke-Juan. The Effect of Semantic Classifier on Lexical Recognition and Semantic Extraction of Chinese Sign Language[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2012, 44(8): 1004-1014.
[13] LI Ya-Dan,MA Wen-Juan,LUO Jun-Long,ZHANG Qing-Lin. Competition and Emotion Impact on Effect of Prototype Elicitation during Insight Problem Solving[J]. , 2012, 44(1): 1-13.
[14] NIE Ai-Qing,GUO Chun-Yan,SHEN Mo-Wei. The Influence of the Testing Paradigm on Location Source Retrieval: An Event-Related Potentials Study[J]. , 2011, 43(05): 473-482.
[15] WANG Ya-Min,WANG Zhi-Xian,HUANG Ya-Mei,JIANG Jing,DING Jin-Hong. Effects of Spatial Frequencies on Recognition of Facial Identity and Facial Expression[J]. , 2011, 43(04): 373-383.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
Copyright © Acta Psychologica Sinica
Support by Beijing Magtech